Perception of the horizontal and vertical in tangible displays: minimal gender differences

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Perception of the horizontal and vertical in tangible displays: minimal gender differences"

Transcription

1 Perception, 1999, volume 28, pages 387 ^ 394 DOI: /p2655 Perception of the horizontal and vertical in tangible displays: minimal gender differences Morton A Hellerô Winston-Salem State University, Winston-Salem, NC 27110, USA Jeffrey A Calcaterra Wright State University, Dayton, OH 45435, USA Shavonda L Green# Winston-Salem State University, Winston-Salem, NC 27110, USA Stevette L Barnette Radford University, Radford, VA 24142, USA Received 25 June 1997, in revised form 29 June 1998 Abstract. A study is reported of gender differences in a haptic version of three Piagetian tests that assess understanding of Euclidian space. A raised-line drawing kit was used both for subject responses and for production of stimuli. To test understanding of the horizontal, subjects felt pictures of a jar at four tilts and were asked to draw the water line. Two methods were used to examine understanding of the vertical. First, subjects drew a hanging electrical cord and light bulb, attached to the ceiling of a bus, parked on hills of four different angles. Subsequently, subjects drew telephone poles (represented by a single line) on hills of four different angles. In the jar task, males and females showed comparable performance, both groups showing large errors. Judgments of the vertical were very similar for males and females in the bus task, but errors diminished considerably for both genders when subjects drew telephone poles on hills. It is suggested that better judgments of the vertical in the pole task probably derive from the use of body-centered spatial reference information. 1 Introduction The water-level and plumb-line tasks were proposed by Piaget and Inhelder (1956) to demonstrate the development of spatial reasoning about the horizontal and vertical in children. In these tasks, subjects draw a line to indicate the water level in a tilted jar or draw a hanging plumb line. The original version of the task required children to anticipate the water level on being given a tilted jar. Subsequently, Piaget asked children to draw the water level while looking at a real tilted jar with water in it. Piaget was interested in whether children would discover the principle that the water stays horizontal during an experimental session. In order to complete these tasks correctly, subjects must understand that the direction of the water level and the direction of the hanging plumb line do not vary as their containers are tilted. An understanding of the invariance of the horizontal and vertical is important for locating objects in Euclidian space, and processing pattern information. Later studies have found that a surprisingly large proportion of college students fail at the tasks and, further, that males often show higher performance than females (Harris et al 1977; Robert and Harel 1996; Robert and Ohlmann 1994; Voyer et al 1995). Robert and her colleagues (eg Berthiaume et al 1993; Robert et al 1994) have claimed that the gender difference in the water-level task disappeared when subjects were given a haptic version of the task. In this task, subjects moved a magnetic bar to show the water level in a tangible jar, represented by a rectangular hole in a metal plate. Robert et al attributed the comparable performance of males and females to the lack of salience of ô Current address to which correspondence and requests for reprints should be sent: Department of Psychology, Eastern Illinois University, 119 Physical Sciences Building, 600 Lincoln Avenue, Charleston, IL , USA; cfmah1@ciu.edu.

2 388 M A Heller, J A Calcaterra, S L Green, S L Barnette irrelevant cues (ie the sides of the jar) when the task was done haptically. According to them, when the task is done visually, the oblique lines are more difficult to ignore. Two methodological factors in these experiments served to make the reduced haptic salience argument less convincing. First, visually, subjects use a pencil to add a line to a drawing, using a familiar form of depiction. Haptically, subjects were required to perform a very different tasköto imagine that the rectangular hole represented a jar, the solid plate represented empty space, and a magnetic bar represented the water line. Note that task requirements, involving manipulating the bar, would tend to draw subjects' attention away from the shape of the jar. Under these circumstances, subjects could not readily feel the hole representing the jar while manipulating the bar with both hands, and this may have helped them ignore the distracting framework representing the jar. Second, subjects were tested in room lighting, where touch has been shown to be less accurate for some spatial tasks involving imagery (Heller 1993). Conceivably, access to visual information about the vertical and horizontal in the room could have aided subjects and improved performance. In the present study, subjects completed a haptic drawing version of the water-level task. Subsequently, subjects completed a haptic version of the plumb-line task. Finally, subjects completed an additional task for assessing the verticalöthe telephone-pole task in which they were asked to make raised-line drawings of telephone poles on hills varying in angle. Males were expected to show superior performance in each of the tasks, if gender differences previously found in vision generalize to touch. 2 General method 2.1 Subjects Subjects were twenty-four (twelve male, twelve female) undergraduate volunteers recruited on the Winston-Salem State University campus. Subjects were blindfolded throughout the experiments, and participated in each of the drawing tasks reported here. 2.2 Stimuli and apparatus Durable raised-line drawings were produced with a Swedish raised-line drawing kit. The Swedish drawing kit produces a tangible and visible line when a ball-point pen is drawn over the surface (see Heller et al 1996a, 1996b; Heller and Schiff 1991) Jar task. The jar was depicted as a rectangle, 3.5 cm wide by 7 cm high (the height included a 0.5 cm cap). The standard jar was drawn in its normal upright position, at 08 (see figure 1). Jars were drawn at four different tilts: 608, 308, 308, and 608. Jars at negative angles leaned to the left of the vertical; ones at positive angles leaned to the right. The upright standard, at 08, showed the jar half-full of water (with a horizontal line connecting the midpoints of the vertical segments). A 9-cm-long horizontal line was drawn at the bottom of each jar to show the horizontal table surface. (a) (b) Figure 1. (a) Drawing of the jar used for the water-level problem. (b) An illustration of a test drawing of a tilted jar.

3 Haptic space Bus task. The standard bus was drawn parked on a 15.5-cm-long flat surface (see figure 2). The bus was a rectangle, 8 cm by 4.5 cm, with the top two corners beveled (see Robert and Harel 1996). Two circles, 2 cm in diameter, represented the wheels. A rectangle, 8 mm by 4 mm, was drawn on the inside roof of the bus to show the `junction box' where the electrical cord was attached to the roof of the bus. A 7.5-cmlong horizontal line segment was used to represent the horizontal at the bottom of the hill. The standard depicted the bus at 08, and an electrical cord, 2.4 cm in length, with a circle, 0.6 cm in diameter, drawn at the end represented the light bulb. The electrical cord was connected to the center of the junction box. The 10-cm-long hills were at four different angles: 158, 308, 458, and 608, with 08 representing the horizontal. (a) (b) Figure 2. (a) An illustration of the standard bus. (b) An example of a test stimulus. Drawings of the hills used in the telephone-pole task were like the hills used in the bus task, but without the depiction of the bus Telephone-pole task. A 6-cm-long horizontal line segment was connected to the hill, depicted by a 10-cm-long line. The line representing the hill was drawn at one of four angles (158, 308, 458, and 608; with08representing the horizontal). 2.3 Design and procedure The data on the horizontal were treated separately from the judgments of the vertical. The data for judgments of the horizontal were treated as a 2 (gender)64 (angle of tilt) design, with repeated measures on angle of tilt. Judgments of the vertical were analyzed as a between ^ within ANOVA, with the independent groups factors being task (bus vs hills), gender, and repeated measures on angle of tilt. In all conditions, subjects were asked to make a raised-line drawing to indicate their answers. They were not given feedback, and no time constraints were imposed. All subjects completed the three tasks in the same order, with the jar task first, followed by the bus task, and finally the telephone-pole task. Task order was not varied, since it was thought that this would be too disorienting for the subjects. The drawing task was novel and unfamiliar, and pilot data indicated rather large error scores. Subjects were allowed to redo their drawings until satisfied, but were not allowed to alter previous drawings Jar task. Subjects felt the standard drawing and were instructed to guess at the identity of the tangible picture. All drawings were presented flat on the horizontal table surface, with the horizontal lines parallel to the front edge of the table. Many subjects indicated they thought the standard was a picture of furniture, such as a table, or clothing, such as a hat. They were then told the drawing was that of a jar with water in it, sitting on the surface of a table. The blindfolded subjects felt the standard drawing and were instructed to find the parts of the picture representing the cap, the table surface, and the water level, and identify them. This procedure was adopted to be sure that all subjects were able to correctly interpret the drawings during the testing phase of the experiments. Subjects were then told to note that the jar was half-full of water. Next, subjects felt the drawings of the tilted jar, and were asked to identify the cap and the table surface. Before drawing each picture, subjects were reminded that the jar was

4 390 M A Heller, J A Calcaterra, S L Green, S L Barnette ``super-glued in place and not moving''. Subjects then drew a tangible line representing the water level in each of the drawings. They were told to draw the water level as it would be in the real world, assuming that the jar was fixed in place for some time. A ruler was visible to subjects before they were blindfolded, and was available for subjects' use if they wished Bus task. Subjects first felt the standard drawing, which showed the bus at 08. An electrical cord, 2.4 cm in length, with a circle, 0.6 cm in diameter, drawn at the end represented the light bulb. Subjects were told that the bus was parked and not moving, and had been ``sitting there for a few minutes''. Subjects were told to find the wheels, the light bulb, the electrical cord, and the junction box. Feedback was given during this familiarization phase, but not during testing. Next, they felt pictures of the bus on the hill, and they were then asked to draw the cord and the light bulb. Subjects were asked to connect the cord to the rectangular junction box hanging from the ceiling of the bus Telephone-pole task. Subjects were told to draw a single line to represent a telephone pole on the hill at four different angles. They were told to try to draw the pole as it would be in the real world, in the middle of the hill portion of each drawing. The center of each line representing the hill was at the person's midline, flat on the table surface in front of the subjects. 3 Results The response measure was the absolute angular error. The horizontal task was analyzed separately as a 2 (gender)64 (angle) ANOVA. Judgments of the vertical were analyzed separately as a 2 (task)62 (gender)64 (angle) ANOVA. 3.1 Jar task Subjects generally showed large error scores and were systematically influenced by the angle of the jar (see table 1). A correct response is a line drawn parallel to the table surface. Thus, an error score was calculated by measuring the angle of the subject's line and noting the deviation from the horizontal. A line was drawn to connect the points where the subject's line intersected each side of the jar. The response measure was the absolute value of the angular difference between this line and the horizontal. Table 1. Haptic jar task and assessment of horizontality: absolute error scores (M ), percent correct (P ), and mean number correct (m), with standard deviations (SD) for males and females. Angle of tilt m P Males M % SD Females M % SD A between ^ within ANOVA showed that the main effect of gender failed to reach significance (F 122, ˆ 2:5, p ˆ 0:13).The interaction of gender and angle was also nonsignificant (F 5 1). Subject responses, however, varied significantly with angle (F 366, ˆ 11:6, p 5 0:0001). Because the correct answer at each tilt entails an error score of zero, an effect of angle indicates that subjects were influenced systematically by the tilt of the jar. Errors given 608 and 308 were generally negative degrees deviated from true. A Newman ^ Keuls a posteriori test showed that jars that were tilted equally (ie 608 and 608) gave the same pattern of results (mean absolute error for 608 was 38.68,

5 Haptic space 391 and for 608, 37.98; for 308 it was 24.18, and for 308 it was 24.18), regardless of the direction of tilt ( p 4 0:05). All other means were significantly different ( p 5 0:01). In earlier studies (Rebelsky 1964) the data were analyzed in terms of the number of correct answers (with absolute errors of 58 or less). The data were reanalyzed with the use of that scoring method. A one-tailed t-test (predicting a better score for males) showed a nonsignificant difference between the groups (t 22 ˆ 1:22, p 4 0:20). Many subjects were unable to accurately represent the water surface. Subjects giving incorrect answers generally made one of two types of drawings: lines were either parallel to the top of the jar; or sloping upward, tending toward approaching a line parallel to the side of the jar. Absolute error scores increased as the overall tilt of the jar increased. It should be noted, however, that subjects showed a great deal of variability. On average, males did slightly better than females, but, given the variability of performance, the difference was small and statistically nonsignificant. 3.2 Bus task The response measure was the absolute value of the angular difference between the response and the vertical. In all cases, the correct response was a line perpendicular to the horizontal. Table 2. Haptic bus task: absolute error scores (M), percent correct (P), mean number correct (m), and standard deviations (SD) in drawings of the vertical by males and females. Angle of hill m P Males M % SD Females M % SD Table 3. Haptic hill/pole task: absolute error scores (M), percent correct (P), mean number correct (m), and standard deviations (SD) in production of the vertical by males and females. Angle of hill m P Males M % SD Females M % SD A between ^ within ANOVA revealed highly significant effects of task (F 144, ˆ 11:34, p 5 0:01), and angle of tilt (F 3132, ˆ 12:78, p 5 0:001), but a nonsignificant effect of gender (F 5 1). The interaction between task and gender was nonsignificant (F 5 1), as was the interaction between task and angle (F 3, 132 ˆ 2:0, p 4 0:10). The interaction between gender and angle was marginal (F 3, 132 ˆ 2:55, p ˆ 0:058), as was the interaction between task, gender, and angle (F 3, 132 ˆ 2:15, p ˆ 0:10). Males and females showed very similar performance on judgments of the vertical (mean error for males, M ˆ 20:68; for females, M ˆ 19:28). Furthermore, errors were much lower for drawing poles on hills (M ˆ 13:28) than in the bus task (M ˆ 26:58).

6 392 M A Heller, J A Calcaterra, S L Green, S L Barnette These data suggest that subjects may use a body-centered frame of reference which helps them draw the vertical in the hill task (see Millar 1991, 1994). There are two important differences between the two verticality tasks that could explain the difference in performance levels. First, the bus task provides an oblique framework that subjects must ignore in order to perform the task correctly. In the telephone-pole task, the only distracting oblique line is the actual hill, and subjects can simply use their body midline as a frame of reference to determine the vertical (see Millar 1991, 1994). Subjects are then able to draw a line receding from the body midline. In the bus task, subjects must draw the cord and light bulb inside the boundaries provided by the bus. Subject responses may vary with field dependence or the relative salience of cues, when using haptics. Second, although both tasks involve an understanding of the vertical, several subjects were distracted by the possibility of mobility of the bus. Despite being told that the bus had been ``parked on a hill'' and that it had been ``sitting there for a few minutes'', several subjects still asked about the direction in which the bus was moving before it stopped. It is possible, of course, that practice effects would have led to improved performance on the hill task. However, subjects were not given feedback, and so this explanation of the data seems unlikely, especially given the large improvement from the bus to the hill task. 4 General discussion Gender differences were not found for the haptic drawing version of the water-level task. This result is consistent with the findings of Robert and her colleagues (Berthiaume et al 1993; Robert et al 1994). It should be noted that both males and females had relatively high error scores when judging the vertical with the bus task, but both groups improved considerably when asked to draw a telephone pole. The results suggest that possible gender differences are probably linked to the response measure. Furthermore, it is likely that improved performance with the pole task reflects the greater ease with which subjects could use body-centered spatial reference frames when drawing the vertical (Millar 1991, 1994). As inclines become sharper, it is more difficult to ignore the reference frame provided by a surround. However, this is not the case for drawings of the vertical pole on a hill, and subjects do much better on this task. Part of the difficulty that some subjects have with the water-level problem may involve difficulty with producing raised-line drawings while blindfolded. Sighted subjects are unfamiliar with the production of raised-line drawings, and may be disoriented initially when blindfolded (Heller 1993). In the rod-and-frame task, Bitterman and Worchel (1958) reported that sighted persons were more disoriented by body tilt than blind subjects when making judgments of the vertical. They concluded that sighted subjects are susceptible to disorientation when denied visual spatial information, reflecting visual dominance in orientation of normal individuals. Also, blindfolded sighted subjects did not do as well as blind subjects when interpreting tangible pictures of a board at a slant, but only for raised-line drawings of the vertical panel (Heller et al 1996b). This argument is also consistent with gender differences that have been obtained in the rod-and-frame task. The behavior and comments of one female subject in the present report would be consistent with this interpretation of the data. She stated that she found the use of blindfolds disorienting. This subject made an error with the first set of choice pictures. On the second set of choice pictures she suddenly recalled the physical principle (horizontality) that the water level is always horizontal, and was correct on all subsequent trials. Performance on these tasks is dependent upon many factors, not only one's knowledge of physics (Robert and Harel 1996). Thus, an individual may understand a spatial concept, yet not show perfect performance. Consequently, an additional experiment was conducted, with the use of a multiplechoice variant of the water-level problem. Blindfolded subjects (twenty-eight new and

7 Haptic space 393 naive subjects: fourteen male and fourteen female) were initially exposed to the standard drawing, and then to sets of multiple-choice pictures. Each of the four angles ( 608, 308, 308, and608) was presented twice, in a random arrangement. For each angle, four raised-line picture choices were generated, including a correct choice and three additional picture foils. One incorrect choice included a drawing with the water level parallel to the base of the jar. In each of the two other picture choices, the water level was 308 or 308 from the line drawn parallel to the base. Thus, each subject was exposed to eight trials. As in prior research, the subjects were blindfolded throughout, and feedback was not given. Subjects were told that they would feel a set of pictures showing water in a jar, and should pick out the picture that showed ``... how the water would be in the real world''. As in experiment 1 reported here, subjects were told to imagine the jar was stationary, and that they should think of how the water would eventually wind up in the jar. Table 4 summarizes the results of the multiple-choice version of the haptic Piagetian water-level problem, and shows little difference in the performance of males (mean number correct M ˆ 4:93 out of 8 total choices) and females (M ˆ 4:5). An ANOVA on mean number correct indicated a nonsignificant effect of gender (F 5 1). The effect of tilt of the jar was highly significant (F 378, ˆ 5:84, p 5 0:01), but the interaction between gender and tilt was nonsignificant (F 378, ˆ 1:48, p 4 0:20). The multiple-choice version of the water-level problem was replicated with drawings of flasks (with round bottoms) to determine if a reduction in the salience of the raised lines would alter the results. The results for an additional twenty-eight new subjects (fourteen males and fourteen females) were similar to those for the multiple-choice task reported above, since the effect of gender on number correct was nonsignificant (F 126, ˆ 2:5, p ˆ 0:127). However, all of the other effects were nonsignificant (all Fs 5 1). An ANOVA was performed which compared the data for the two multiple-choice water-level problems (total N ˆ 56). The results were similar to those for each analysis considered separately. The effect of bottle shape (jar versus flask) was nonsignificant (F 5 1), and the effect of gender failed to reach significance (F 152, ˆ 2:57, p 4 0:10). However, the effect of angle was highly significant (F 3, 156 ˆ 4:41, p 5 0:01), reflecting lower performance for greater inclines. The interaction between shape and angle was also significant (F 3, 156 ˆ 2:79, p 5 0:05), with tests of simple effects showing significantly lower performance for the sharper inclines and the jar (F 3, 156 ˆ 6:57, p 5 0:001), but not for the flask (F 5 1). The shape by gender interaction was nonsignificant (F 5 1), as was the interaction between gender and angle (F 3, 156 ˆ 1:91, p ˆ 0:13). The interaction between gender, shape, and angle failed to reach significance (F 5 1). The results of the multiple-choice tasks are consistent with the idea that gender differences in spatial tasks, like those devised by Piaget, are inconsistent and influenced Table 4. Piagetian multiple-choice task: mean number correct (M ), standard deviation (SD), and percent correct (P) (N ˆ 28; 14 males, 14 females). Angle of water line in the jar Males M SD P 79% 40% 50% 79% Females M SD P 54% 47% 54% 72% Note: Total mean number correct possible M ˆ 8.

8 394 M A Heller, J A Calcaterra, S L Green, S L Barnette by a number of variables. Any obtained gender differences were small in magnitude, and could be affected by the response measure that was used. The results from the multiplechoice task were consistent with a lack of gender differences that were obtained in a drawing task, and the negative report by Robert and her colleagues. The only gender differences that appeared in the present study involved a marginally significant interaction between gender and angle in judgments of the vertical. Perhaps some females are slightly more subject to being thrown off by context effects. One reviewer suggested that one should exercise considerable caution when trying to interpret studies of gender, because gender cannot be manipulated experimentally. It is an individual difference variable, which might require relatively large samples from representative populations. The present data are certainly consistent with this analysis, especially given the largely nonsignificant effects of gender over different tasks. It should be noted that the present results are not consistent with the idea that information from tangible raised-line drawings is not perceptually salient for touch. This argument could not explain the large differences that were found between judging the vertical in the bus task and for the telephone pole on a hill. Thus, subjects are influenced by the spatial reference information provided by the drawings. Subjects are sometimes better able to ignore distracting cues in haptic conditions, as when drawing telephone poles on a hill. Acknowledgements. Preparation of this report and some of the research were supported by NIH Grant 2 SO6 RR and NIH RO1 EY We are grateful to Cindy Flynt for assistance with data collection and analyses. References Berthiaume F, Robert M, St-Onge R, Pelletier J, 1993 `Àbsence of a gender difference in a haptic version of the water-level task'' Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society ^ 60 Bitterman M E, Worchel P, 1958 ``The phenomenal vertical and horizontal in blind and sighted subjects'' American Journal of Psychology ^ 602 Harris L J, Hanley C, Best C T, 1977 ``Conservation of horizontality: Sex differences in sixth graders and college students'', in Readings in Child Development and Relationships Eds R C Smart, M S Smart (London: Macmillan) pp 375 ^ 387 Heller M A, 1993 ``Influence of visual guidance on braille recognition: Low lighting also helps touch'' Perception & Psychophysics ^ 681 Heller M A, Calcaterra J A, Burson L L, Tyler L A, 1996a ``Tactual picture identification by blind and sighted people: Effects of providing categorical information'' Perception & Psychophysics ^ 323 Heller M A, Calcaterra J A, Tyler L A, Burson L L, 1996b ``Production and interpretation of perspective drawings by blind and sighted people'' Perception ^334 Heller M A, Schiff W (Eds), 1991 The Psychology of Touch (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates) Millar S, 1991 ``A reverse lag in the recognition and production of tactual drawings: Theoretical implications forhaptic coding'', inthe Psychology of Touch Eds M A Heller,W Schiff (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates) pp 301 ^ 325 Millar S, 1994 Understanding and Representing Space: Theory and Evidence from Studies with Blind and Sighted Children (Oxford: Oxford University Press) Piaget J, Inhelder B, 1956 The Child's Conception of Space (New York: W W Norton) Rebelsky F, 1964 `Àdults perception of the horizontal'' Perceptual and Motor Skills ^ 374 Robert M, Harel F, 1996 ``The gender difference in orienting liquid surfaces and plumb-lines: Its robustness, its correlates, and the associated knowledge of simple physics'' Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology ^314 Robert M, Ohlman T, 1994 ``Water-level representation by men and women as a function of rod-andframe proficiency and visual and postural information'' Perception ^1333 Robert M, Pelletier J, St-Onge R, Berthiaume F, 1994 ``Women's deficiency in water-level representation: Present in visual conditions yet absent in haptic contexts'' Acta Psychologica ^ 32 Voyer D, Voyer S, Bryden M P, 1995 ``Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A metaanalysis and consideration of critical variables'' Psychological Bulletin ^ 270 ß 1999 a Pion publication

The influence of exploration mode, orientation, and configuration on the haptic Mu«ller-Lyer illusion

The influence of exploration mode, orientation, and configuration on the haptic Mu«ller-Lyer illusion Perception, 2005, volume 34, pages 1475 ^ 1500 DOI:10.1068/p5269 The influence of exploration mode, orientation, and configuration on the haptic Mu«ller-Lyer illusion Morton A Heller, Melissa McCarthy,

More information

Tangible pictures: Viewpoint effects and linear perspective in visually impaired people

Tangible pictures: Viewpoint effects and linear perspective in visually impaired people Perception, 2002, volume 31, pages 747 ^ 769 DOI:10.1068/p3253 Tangible pictures: Viewpoint effects and linear perspective in visually impaired people Morton A Heller, Deneen D Brackett, Eric Scroggs,

More information

Spatial Judgments from Different Vantage Points: A Different Perspective

Spatial Judgments from Different Vantage Points: A Different Perspective Spatial Judgments from Different Vantage Points: A Different Perspective Erik Prytz, Mark Scerbo and Kennedy Rebecca The self-archived postprint version of this journal article is available at Linköping

More information

The Haptic Perception of Spatial Orientations studied with an Haptic Display

The Haptic Perception of Spatial Orientations studied with an Haptic Display The Haptic Perception of Spatial Orientations studied with an Haptic Display Gabriel Baud-Bovy 1 and Edouard Gentaz 2 1 Faculty of Psychology, UHSR University, Milan, Italy gabriel@shaker.med.umn.edu 2

More information

Superior haptic perceptual selectivity in late-blind and very-low-vision subjects

Superior haptic perceptual selectivity in late-blind and very-low-vision subjects Perception, 2003, volume 32, pages 499 ^ 511 DOI:10.1068/p3423 Superior haptic perceptual selectivity in late-blind and very-low-vision subjects Morton A Heller, Kathy Wilson, Heather Steffen, Keiko Yoneyama

More information

Egocentric reference frame bias in the palmar haptic perception of surface orientation. Allison Coleman and Frank H. Durgin. Swarthmore College

Egocentric reference frame bias in the palmar haptic perception of surface orientation. Allison Coleman and Frank H. Durgin. Swarthmore College Running head: HAPTIC EGOCENTRIC BIAS Egocentric reference frame bias in the palmar haptic perception of surface orientation Allison Coleman and Frank H. Durgin Swarthmore College Reference: Coleman, A.,

More information

Haptic perception of spatial relations

Haptic perception of spatial relations Perception, 1999, volume 28, pages 781 ^ 795 DOI:1.168/p293 Haptic perception of spatial relations Astrid M L Kappers, Jan J Koenderink HelmholtzInstituut,Princetonplein5,3584CCUtrecht,TheNetherlands;e-mail:a.m.l.kappers@phys.uu.nl

More information

Attenuating the haptic horizontal vertical curvature illusion

Attenuating the haptic horizontal vertical curvature illusion Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2010, 72 (6), 1626-1641 doi:10.3758/app.72.6.1626 Attenuating the haptic horizontal vertical curvature illusion MORTON A. HELLER, ANNE D. MCCLURE WALK, RITA SCHNA

More information

Tactile picture convergence 1. Blind man draws using diminution in three dimensions. John M. Kennedy and Igor Juricevic. University of Toronto

Tactile picture convergence 1. Blind man draws using diminution in three dimensions. John M. Kennedy and Igor Juricevic. University of Toronto Tactile picture convergence 1 Blind man draws using diminution in three dimensions John M. Kennedy and Igor Juricevic University of Toronto Running head: Tactile picture convergence Word count 3260 Address

More information

The Shape-Weight Illusion

The Shape-Weight Illusion The Shape-Weight Illusion Mirela Kahrimanovic, Wouter M. Bergmann Tiest, and Astrid M.L. Kappers Universiteit Utrecht, Helmholtz Institute Padualaan 8, 3584 CH Utrecht, The Netherlands {m.kahrimanovic,w.m.bergmanntiest,a.m.l.kappers}@uu.nl

More information

Salient features make a search easy

Salient features make a search easy Chapter General discussion This thesis examined various aspects of haptic search. It consisted of three parts. In the first part, the saliency of movability and compliance were investigated. In the second

More information

Discriminating direction of motion trajectories from angular speed and background information

Discriminating direction of motion trajectories from angular speed and background information Atten Percept Psychophys (2013) 75:1570 1582 DOI 10.3758/s13414-013-0488-z Discriminating direction of motion trajectories from angular speed and background information Zheng Bian & Myron L. Braunstein

More information

Role of gravitational cues in the haptic perception of orientation

Role of gravitational cues in the haptic perception of orientation Perception & Psychophysics 1996, 58 (8), 1278 1292 Role of gravitational cues in the haptic perception of orientation EDOUARD GENTAZ and YVETTE HATWELL Université Pierre Mendès France, Grenoble, France

More information

Enclosure size and the use of local and global geometric cues for reorientation

Enclosure size and the use of local and global geometric cues for reorientation Psychon Bull Rev (2012) 19:270 276 DOI 10.3758/s13423-011-0195-5 BRIEF REPORT Enclosure size and the use of local and global geometric cues for reorientation Bradley R. Sturz & Martha R. Forloines & Kent

More information

Module 2. Lecture-1. Understanding basic principles of perception including depth and its representation.

Module 2. Lecture-1. Understanding basic principles of perception including depth and its representation. Module 2 Lecture-1 Understanding basic principles of perception including depth and its representation. Initially let us take the reference of Gestalt law in order to have an understanding of the basic

More information

The eyes have it: Naïve beliefs about reflections. Luke A. Jones*, Marco Bertamini* and Alice Spooner L. *University of Liverpool

The eyes have it: Naïve beliefs about reflections. Luke A. Jones*, Marco Bertamini* and Alice Spooner L. *University of Liverpool * Manuscript The eyes have it 1 Running head: REFLECTIONS IN MIRRORS The eyes have it: Naïve beliefs about reflections Luke A. Jones*, Marco Bertamini* and Alice Spooner L *University of Liverpool L University

More information

NAVIGATIONAL CONTROL EFFECT ON REPRESENTING VIRTUAL ENVIRONMENTS

NAVIGATIONAL CONTROL EFFECT ON REPRESENTING VIRTUAL ENVIRONMENTS NAVIGATIONAL CONTROL EFFECT ON REPRESENTING VIRTUAL ENVIRONMENTS Xianjun Sam Zheng, George W. McConkie, and Benjamin Schaeffer Beckman Institute, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign This present

More information

Size Illusion on an Asymmetrically Divided Circle

Size Illusion on an Asymmetrically Divided Circle Size Illusion on an Asymmetrically Divided Circle W.A. Kreiner Faculty of Natural Sciences University of Ulm 2 1. Introduction In the Poggendorff (18) illusion a line, inclined by about 45 0 to the horizontal,

More information

Can a haptic force feedback display provide visually impaired people with useful information about texture roughness and 3D form of virtual objects?

Can a haptic force feedback display provide visually impaired people with useful information about texture roughness and 3D form of virtual objects? Can a haptic force feedback display provide visually impaired people with useful information about texture roughness and 3D form of virtual objects? Gunnar Jansson Department of Psychology, Uppsala University

More information

Perceived Image Quality and Acceptability of Photographic Prints Originating from Different Resolution Digital Capture Devices

Perceived Image Quality and Acceptability of Photographic Prints Originating from Different Resolution Digital Capture Devices Perceived Image Quality and Acceptability of Photographic Prints Originating from Different Resolution Digital Capture Devices Michael E. Miller and Rise Segur Eastman Kodak Company Rochester, New York

More information

Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion

Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion Vision Research 42 (2002) 2731 2735 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion Erich W. Graf *, Wendy J. Adams, Martin Lages Department

More information

Comparison between audio and tactile systems for delivering simple navigational information to visually impaired pedestrians

Comparison between audio and tactile systems for delivering simple navigational information to visually impaired pedestrians British Journal of Visual Impairment September, 2007 Comparison between audio and tactile systems for delivering simple navigational information to visually impaired pedestrians Dr. Olinkha Gustafson-Pearce,

More information

Haptic control in a virtual environment

Haptic control in a virtual environment Haptic control in a virtual environment Gerard de Ruig (0555781) Lourens Visscher (0554498) Lydia van Well (0566644) September 10, 2010 Introduction With modern technological advancements it is entirely

More information

Comparison of Haptic and Non-Speech Audio Feedback

Comparison of Haptic and Non-Speech Audio Feedback Comparison of Haptic and Non-Speech Audio Feedback Cagatay Goncu 1 and Kim Marriott 1 Monash University, Mebourne, Australia, cagatay.goncu@monash.edu, kim.marriott@monash.edu Abstract. We report a usability

More information

MOTION PARALLAX AND ABSOLUTE DISTANCE. Steven H. Ferris NAVAL SUBMARINE MEDICAL RESEARCH LABORATORY NAVAL SUBMARINE MEDICAL CENTER REPORT NUMBER 673

MOTION PARALLAX AND ABSOLUTE DISTANCE. Steven H. Ferris NAVAL SUBMARINE MEDICAL RESEARCH LABORATORY NAVAL SUBMARINE MEDICAL CENTER REPORT NUMBER 673 MOTION PARALLAX AND ABSOLUTE DISTANCE by Steven H. Ferris NAVAL SUBMARINE MEDICAL RESEARCH LABORATORY NAVAL SUBMARINE MEDICAL CENTER REPORT NUMBER 673 Bureau of Medicine and Surgery, Navy Department Research

More information

The Representational Effect in Complex Systems: A Distributed Representation Approach

The Representational Effect in Complex Systems: A Distributed Representation Approach 1 The Representational Effect in Complex Systems: A Distributed Representation Approach Johnny Chuah (chuah.5@osu.edu) The Ohio State University 204 Lazenby Hall, 1827 Neil Avenue, Columbus, OH 43210,

More information

DECISION MAKING IN THE IOWA GAMBLING TASK. To appear in F. Columbus, (Ed.). The Psychology of Decision-Making. Gordon Fernie and Richard Tunney

DECISION MAKING IN THE IOWA GAMBLING TASK. To appear in F. Columbus, (Ed.). The Psychology of Decision-Making. Gordon Fernie and Richard Tunney DECISION MAKING IN THE IOWA GAMBLING TASK To appear in F. Columbus, (Ed.). The Psychology of Decision-Making Gordon Fernie and Richard Tunney University of Nottingham Address for correspondence: School

More information

Running an HCI Experiment in Multiple Parallel Universes

Running an HCI Experiment in Multiple Parallel Universes Author manuscript, published in "ACM CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (alt.chi) (2014)" Running an HCI Experiment in Multiple Parallel Universes Univ. Paris Sud, CNRS, Univ. Paris Sud,

More information

The horizon line, linear perspective, interposition, and background brightness as determinants of the magnitude of the pictorial moon illusion

The horizon line, linear perspective, interposition, and background brightness as determinants of the magnitude of the pictorial moon illusion Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2009, 71 (1), 131-142 doi:10.3758/app.71.1.131 The horizon line, linear perspective, interposition, and background brightness as determinants of the magnitude of

More information

The Lady's not for turning: Rotation of the Thatcher illusion

The Lady's not for turning: Rotation of the Thatcher illusion Perception, 2001, volume 30, pages 769 ^ 774 DOI:10.1068/p3174 The Lady's not for turning: Rotation of the Thatcher illusion Michael B Lewis School of Psychology, Cardiff University, PO Box 901, Cardiff

More information

TRAFFIC SIGN DETECTION AND IDENTIFICATION.

TRAFFIC SIGN DETECTION AND IDENTIFICATION. TRAFFIC SIGN DETECTION AND IDENTIFICATION Vaughan W. Inman 1 & Brian H. Philips 2 1 SAIC, McLean, Virginia, USA 2 Federal Highway Administration, McLean, Virginia, USA Email: vaughan.inman.ctr@dot.gov

More information

Experiments on the locus of induced motion

Experiments on the locus of induced motion Perception & Psychophysics 1977, Vol. 21 (2). 157 161 Experiments on the locus of induced motion JOHN N. BASSILI Scarborough College, University of Toronto, West Hill, Ontario MIC la4, Canada and JAMES

More information

Here I present more details about the methods of the experiments which are. described in the main text, and describe two additional examinations which

Here I present more details about the methods of the experiments which are. described in the main text, and describe two additional examinations which Supplementary Note Here I present more details about the methods of the experiments which are described in the main text, and describe two additional examinations which assessed DF s proprioceptive performance

More information

Consumer Behavior when Zooming and Cropping Personal Photographs and its Implications for Digital Image Resolution

Consumer Behavior when Zooming and Cropping Personal Photographs and its Implications for Digital Image Resolution Consumer Behavior when Zooming and Cropping Personal Photographs and its Implications for Digital Image Michael E. Miller and Jerry Muszak Eastman Kodak Company Rochester, New York USA Abstract This paper

More information

A Mental Cutting Test Using Drawings of Intersections

A Mental Cutting Test Using Drawings of Intersections Journal for Geometry and Graphics Volume 8 (2004), No. 1, 117 126. A Mental Cutting Test Using Drawings of Intersections Emiko Tsutsumi School of Social Information Studies, Otsuma Women s University 2-7-1,

More information

Perception of scene layout from optical contact, shadows, and motion

Perception of scene layout from optical contact, shadows, and motion Perception, 2004, volume 33, pages 1305 ^ 1318 DOI:10.1068/p5288 Perception of scene layout from optical contact, shadows, and motion Rui Ni, Myron L Braunstein Department of Cognitive Sciences, University

More information

THE POGGENDORFF ILLUSION WITH ANOMALOUS SURFACES: MANAGING PAC-MANS, PARALLELS LENGTH AND TYPE OF TRANSVERSAL.

THE POGGENDORFF ILLUSION WITH ANOMALOUS SURFACES: MANAGING PAC-MANS, PARALLELS LENGTH AND TYPE OF TRANSVERSAL. THE POGGENDORFF ILLUSION WITH ANOMALOUS SURFACES: MANAGING PAC-MANS, PARALLELS LENGTH AND TYPE OF TRANSVERSAL. Spoto, A. 1, Massidda, D. 1, Bastianelli, A. 1, Actis-Grosso, R. 2 and Vidotto, G. 1 1 Department

More information

The vertical-horizontal illusion: Assessing the contributions of anisotropy, abutting, and crossing to the misperception of simple line stimuli

The vertical-horizontal illusion: Assessing the contributions of anisotropy, abutting, and crossing to the misperception of simple line stimuli Journal of Vision (2013) 13(8):7, 1 11 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/8/7 1 The vertical-horizontal illusion: Assessing the contributions of anisotropy, abutting, and crossing to the misperception

More information

I STATISTICAL TOOLS IN SIX SIGMA DMAIC PROCESS WITH MINITAB APPLICATIONS

I STATISTICAL TOOLS IN SIX SIGMA DMAIC PROCESS WITH MINITAB APPLICATIONS Six Sigma Quality Concepts & Cases- Volume I STATISTICAL TOOLS IN SIX SIGMA DMAIC PROCESS WITH MINITAB APPLICATIONS Chapter 7 Measurement System Analysis Gage Repeatability & Reproducibility (Gage R&R)

More information

Path completion after haptic exploration without vision: Implications for haptic spatial representations

Path completion after haptic exploration without vision: Implications for haptic spatial representations Perception & Psychophysics 1999, 61 (2), 220-235 Path completion after haptic exploration without vision: Implications for haptic spatial representations ROBERTA L. KLATZKY Carnegie Mellon University,

More information

Haptic Cueing of a Visual Change-Detection Task: Implications for Multimodal Interfaces

Haptic Cueing of a Visual Change-Detection Task: Implications for Multimodal Interfaces In Usability Evaluation and Interface Design: Cognitive Engineering, Intelligent Agents and Virtual Reality (Vol. 1 of the Proceedings of the 9th International Conference on Human-Computer Interaction),

More information

Haptic pictures: Fit judgments predict identification, recognition memory, and confidence

Haptic pictures: Fit judgments predict identification, recognition memory, and confidence Perception, 2002, volume 31, pages 1013 ^ 1026 DOI:10.1068/p3259 Haptic pictures: Fit judgments predict identification, recognition memory, and confidence John M Kennedy, Juan Bai Department of Psychology,

More information

Effects of distance between objects and distance from the vertical axis on shape identity judgments

Effects of distance between objects and distance from the vertical axis on shape identity judgments Memory & Cognition 1994, 22 (5), 552-564 Effects of distance between objects and distance from the vertical axis on shape identity judgments ALINDA FRIEDMAN and DANIEL J. PILON University of Alberta, Edmonton,

More information

Haptic perception of linear extent

Haptic perception of linear extent Perception & Psychophysics 1999, 61 (6), 1211-1226 Haptic perception of linear extent LAURA ARMSTRONG and LAWRENCE E. MARKS John B. Pierce Laboratory and Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut The perception

More information

Game Mechanics Minesweeper is a game in which the player must correctly deduce the positions of

Game Mechanics Minesweeper is a game in which the player must correctly deduce the positions of Table of Contents Game Mechanics...2 Game Play...3 Game Strategy...4 Truth...4 Contrapositive... 5 Exhaustion...6 Burnout...8 Game Difficulty... 10 Experiment One... 12 Experiment Two...14 Experiment Three...16

More information

Optimizing color reproduction of natural images

Optimizing color reproduction of natural images Optimizing color reproduction of natural images S.N. Yendrikhovskij, F.J.J. Blommaert, H. de Ridder IPO, Center for Research on User-System Interaction Eindhoven, The Netherlands Abstract The paper elaborates

More information

I STATISTICAL TOOLS IN SIX SIGMA DMAIC PROCESS WITH MINITAB APPLICATIONS

I STATISTICAL TOOLS IN SIX SIGMA DMAIC PROCESS WITH MINITAB APPLICATIONS Six Sigma Quality Concepts & Cases- Volume I STATISTICAL TOOLS IN SIX SIGMA DMAIC PROCESS WITH MINITAB APPLICATIONS Chapter 7 Measurement System Analysis Gage Repeatability & Reproducibility (Gage R&R)

More information

The shape of luminance increments at the intersection alters the magnitude of the scintillating grid illusion

The shape of luminance increments at the intersection alters the magnitude of the scintillating grid illusion The shape of luminance increments at the intersection alters the magnitude of the scintillating grid illusion Kun Qian a, Yuki Yamada a, Takahiro Kawabe b, Kayo Miura b a Graduate School of Human-Environment

More information

No symmetry advantage when object matching involves accidental viewpoints

No symmetry advantage when object matching involves accidental viewpoints Psychological Research (2006) 70: 52 58 DOI 10.1007/s00426-004-0191-8 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Arno Koning Æ Rob van Lier No symmetry advantage when object matching involves accidental viewpoints Received: 11

More information

A Pilot Study: Introduction of Time-domain Segment to Intensity-based Perception Model of High-frequency Vibration

A Pilot Study: Introduction of Time-domain Segment to Intensity-based Perception Model of High-frequency Vibration A Pilot Study: Introduction of Time-domain Segment to Intensity-based Perception Model of High-frequency Vibration Nan Cao, Hikaru Nagano, Masashi Konyo, Shogo Okamoto 2 and Satoshi Tadokoro Graduate School

More information

Analyzing Situation Awareness During Wayfinding in a Driving Simulator

Analyzing Situation Awareness During Wayfinding in a Driving Simulator In D.J. Garland and M.R. Endsley (Eds.) Experimental Analysis and Measurement of Situation Awareness. Proceedings of the International Conference on Experimental Analysis and Measurement of Situation Awareness.

More information

Three stimuli for visual motion perception compared

Three stimuli for visual motion perception compared Perception & Psychophysics 1982,32 (1),1-6 Three stimuli for visual motion perception compared HANS WALLACH Swarthmore Col/ege, Swarthmore, Pennsylvania ANN O'LEARY Stanford University, Stanford, California

More information

GROUPING BASED ON PHENOMENAL PROXIMITY

GROUPING BASED ON PHENOMENAL PROXIMITY Journal of Experimental Psychology 1964, Vol. 67, No. 6, 531-538 GROUPING BASED ON PHENOMENAL PROXIMITY IRVIN ROCK AND LEONARD BROSGOLE l Yeshiva University The question was raised whether the Gestalt

More information

UNIT 5a STANDARD ORTHOGRAPHIC VIEW DRAWINGS

UNIT 5a STANDARD ORTHOGRAPHIC VIEW DRAWINGS UNIT 5a STANDARD ORTHOGRAPHIC VIEW DRAWINGS 5.1 Introduction Orthographic views are 2D images of a 3D object obtained by viewing it from different orthogonal directions. Six principal views are possible

More information

Haptic presentation of 3D objects in virtual reality for the visually disabled

Haptic presentation of 3D objects in virtual reality for the visually disabled Haptic presentation of 3D objects in virtual reality for the visually disabled M Moranski, A Materka Institute of Electronics, Technical University of Lodz, Wolczanska 211/215, Lodz, POLAND marcin.moranski@p.lodz.pl,

More information

Exploring body holistic processing investigated with composite illusion

Exploring body holistic processing investigated with composite illusion Exploring body holistic processing investigated with composite illusion Dora E. Szatmári (szatmari.dora@pte.hu) University of Pécs, Institute of Psychology Ifjúság Street 6. Pécs, 7624 Hungary Beatrix

More information

The abstraction of schematic representations from photographs of real-world scenes

The abstraction of schematic representations from photographs of real-world scenes Memory & Cognition 1980, Vol. 8 (6), 543-554 The abstraction of schematic representations from photographs of real-world scenes HOWARD S. HOCK Florida Atlantic University, Boca Raton, Florida 33431 and

More information

Synchronous Generators II EE 340

Synchronous Generators II EE 340 Synchronous Generators II EE 340 Generator P-f Curve All generators are driven by a prime mover, such as a steam, gas, water, wind turbines, diesel engines, etc. Regardless the power source, most of prime

More information

Haptic Pictures, Blindness, and Tactile Beliefs: Preliminary Analysis of a Case-Study

Haptic Pictures, Blindness, and Tactile Beliefs: Preliminary Analysis of a Case-Study Haptic Pictures, Blindness, and Tactile Beliefs: Preliminary Analysis of a Case-Study Amedeo D Angiulli (adangiulli@tru.ca), Thompson Rivers University School of Education & Department of Psychology, 103-1402

More information

Visual Processing: Implications for Helmet Mounted Displays (Reprint)

Visual Processing: Implications for Helmet Mounted Displays (Reprint) USAARL Report No. 90-11 Visual Processing: Implications for Helmet Mounted Displays (Reprint) By Jo Lynn Caldwell Rhonda L. Cornum Robert L. Stephens Biomedical Applications Division and Clarence E. Rash

More information

Verifying advantages of

Verifying advantages of hoofdstuk 4 25-08-1999 14:49 Pagina 123 Verifying advantages of Verifying Verifying advantages two-handed Verifying advantages of advantages of interaction of of two-handed two-handed interaction interaction

More information

Drawing Types & Construction Drawings

Drawing Types & Construction Drawings Drawing Types & Construction Drawings Building projects require several types of specialised drawings. This collection of drawings, known as a project set, includes: Location Plan Site Plan Floor Plan

More information

HAPTIC IDENTIFICATION OF RAISED-LINE DRAWINGS BY CHILDREN, ADOLESCENTS AND YOUNG ADULTS: AN AGE-RELATED SKILL (Short Paper)

HAPTIC IDENTIFICATION OF RAISED-LINE DRAWINGS BY CHILDREN, ADOLESCENTS AND YOUNG ADULTS: AN AGE-RELATED SKILL (Short Paper) HAPTIC IDENTIFICATION OF RAISED-LINE DRAWINGS BY CHILDREN, ADOLESCENTS AND YOUNG ADULTS: AN AGE-RELATED SKILL (Short Paper) Delphine Picard 1, Jean-Michel Albaret 2, Anaïs Mazella 1,2 1 Aix Marseille University

More information

A Three-Dimensional Evaluation of Body Representation Change of Human Upper Limb Focused on Sense of Ownership and Sense of Agency

A Three-Dimensional Evaluation of Body Representation Change of Human Upper Limb Focused on Sense of Ownership and Sense of Agency A Three-Dimensional Evaluation of Body Representation Change of Human Upper Limb Focused on Sense of Ownership and Sense of Agency Shunsuke Hamasaki, Atsushi Yamashita and Hajime Asama Department of Precision

More information

Science Binder and Science Notebook. Discussions

Science Binder and Science Notebook. Discussions Lane Tech H. Physics (Joseph/Machaj 2016-2017) A. Science Binder Science Binder and Science Notebook Name: Period: Unit 1: Scientific Methods - Reference Materials The binder is the storage device for

More information

Copyrighted Material. Copyrighted Material. Copyrighted. Copyrighted. Material

Copyrighted Material. Copyrighted Material. Copyrighted. Copyrighted. Material Engineering Graphics ORTHOGRAPHIC PROJECTION People who work with drawings develop the ability to look at lines on paper or on a computer screen and "see" the shapes of the objects the lines represent.

More information

The effect of rotation on configural encoding in a face-matching task

The effect of rotation on configural encoding in a face-matching task Perception, 2007, volume 36, pages 446 ^ 460 DOI:10.1068/p5530 The effect of rotation on configural encoding in a face-matching task Andrew J Edmondsô, Michael B Lewis School of Psychology, Cardiff University,

More information

Depth adjacency and the rod-and-frame illusion

Depth adjacency and the rod-and-frame illusion Perception & Psychophysics 1975, Vol. 18 (2),163-171 Depth adjacency and the rod-and-frame illusion WALTER C. GOGEL and ROBERT E. NEWTON University of California, Santa Barbara, California 99106 n Experiment,

More information

Chapter 6. Experiment 3. Motion sickness and vection with normal and blurred optokinetic stimuli

Chapter 6. Experiment 3. Motion sickness and vection with normal and blurred optokinetic stimuli Chapter 6. Experiment 3. Motion sickness and vection with normal and blurred optokinetic stimuli 6.1 Introduction Chapters 4 and 5 have shown that motion sickness and vection can be manipulated separately

More information

Constructing Line Graphs*

Constructing Line Graphs* Appendix B Constructing Line Graphs* Suppose we are studying some chemical reaction in which a substance, A, is being used up. We begin with a large quantity (1 mg) of A, and we measure in some way how

More information

Laboratory 1: Uncertainty Analysis

Laboratory 1: Uncertainty Analysis University of Alabama Department of Physics and Astronomy PH101 / LeClair May 26, 2014 Laboratory 1: Uncertainty Analysis Hypothesis: A statistical analysis including both mean and standard deviation can

More information

Sketch-Up Guide for Woodworkers

Sketch-Up Guide for Woodworkers W Enjoy this selection from Sketch-Up Guide for Woodworkers In just seconds, you can enjoy this ebook of Sketch-Up Guide for Woodworkers. SketchUp Guide for BUY NOW! Google See how our magazine makes you

More information

Evidence for a significant contribution of interactions between oriented line segments in the Tolansky version of the Poggendorff illusion

Evidence for a significant contribution of interactions between oriented line segments in the Tolansky version of the Poggendorff illusion Perception &: Psychophysics 1986, 39 (5), 334-338 Evidence for a significant contribution of interactions between oriented line segments in the Tolansky version of the Poggendorff illusion PETER WENDEROTH,

More information

Visual influence on haptic torque perception

Visual influence on haptic torque perception Perception, 2012, volume 41, pages 862 870 doi:10.1068/p7090 Visual influence on haptic torque perception Yangqing Xu, Shélan O Keefe, Satoru Suzuki, Steven L Franconeri Department of Psychology, Northwestern

More information

Spatial Low Pass Filters for Pin Actuated Tactile Displays

Spatial Low Pass Filters for Pin Actuated Tactile Displays Spatial Low Pass Filters for Pin Actuated Tactile Displays Jaime M. Lee Harvard University lee@fas.harvard.edu Christopher R. Wagner Harvard University cwagner@fas.harvard.edu S. J. Lederman Queen s University

More information

Orthographic Projection 1

Orthographic Projection 1 Orthographic Projection 1 What Is Orthographic Projection? Basically it is a way a representing a 3D object on a piece of paper. This means we make the object becomes 2D. The difference between Orthographic

More information

COPYRIGHTED MATERIAL. Overview

COPYRIGHTED MATERIAL. Overview In normal experience, our eyes are constantly in motion, roving over and around objects and through ever-changing environments. Through this constant scanning, we build up experience data, which is manipulated

More information

University of Tennessee at. Chattanooga

University of Tennessee at. Chattanooga University of Tennessee at Chattanooga Step Response Engineering 329 By Gold Team: Jason Price Jered Swartz Simon Ionashku 2-3- 2 INTRODUCTION: The purpose of the experiments was to investigate and understand

More information

Complete Drawing and Painting Certificate Course

Complete Drawing and Painting Certificate Course Complete Drawing and Painting Certificate Course Title: Unit Four Portraiture Foundations Medium: Drawing in graphite and charcoal Level: Beginners Week: Two Course Code: Page 1 of 15 Week Two: General

More information

COPYRIGHTED MATERIAL OVERVIEW 1

COPYRIGHTED MATERIAL OVERVIEW 1 OVERVIEW 1 In normal experience, our eyes are constantly in motion, roving over and around objects and through ever-changing environments. Through this constant scanning, we build up experiential data,

More information

Introduction to Psychology Prof. Braj Bhushan Department of Humanities and Social Sciences Indian Institute of Technology, Kanpur

Introduction to Psychology Prof. Braj Bhushan Department of Humanities and Social Sciences Indian Institute of Technology, Kanpur Introduction to Psychology Prof. Braj Bhushan Department of Humanities and Social Sciences Indian Institute of Technology, Kanpur Lecture - 10 Perception Role of Culture in Perception Till now we have

More information

Häkkinen, Jukka; Gröhn, Lauri Turning water into rock

Häkkinen, Jukka; Gröhn, Lauri Turning water into rock Powered by TCPDF (www.tcpdf.org) This is an electronic reprint of the original article. This reprint may differ from the original in pagination and typographic detail. Häkkinen, Jukka; Gröhn, Lauri Turning

More information

Distance perception from motion parallax and ground contact. Rui Ni and Myron L. Braunstein. University of California, Irvine, California

Distance perception from motion parallax and ground contact. Rui Ni and Myron L. Braunstein. University of California, Irvine, California Distance perception 1 Distance perception from motion parallax and ground contact Rui Ni and Myron L. Braunstein University of California, Irvine, California George J. Andersen University of California,

More information

Orientation-sensitivity to facial features explains the Thatcher illusion

Orientation-sensitivity to facial features explains the Thatcher illusion Journal of Vision (2014) 14(12):9, 1 10 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/14/12/9 1 Orientation-sensitivity to facial features explains the Thatcher illusion Department of Psychology and York Neuroimaging

More information

Sensory and Perception. Team 4: Amanda Tapp, Celeste Jackson, Gabe Oswalt, Galen Hendricks, Harry Polstein, Natalie Honan and Sylvie Novins-Montague

Sensory and Perception. Team 4: Amanda Tapp, Celeste Jackson, Gabe Oswalt, Galen Hendricks, Harry Polstein, Natalie Honan and Sylvie Novins-Montague Sensory and Perception Team 4: Amanda Tapp, Celeste Jackson, Gabe Oswalt, Galen Hendricks, Harry Polstein, Natalie Honan and Sylvie Novins-Montague Our Senses sensation: simple stimulation of a sense organ

More information

The Influence of Visual Illusion on Visually Perceived System and Visually Guided Action System

The Influence of Visual Illusion on Visually Perceived System and Visually Guided Action System The Influence of Visual Illusion on Visually Perceived System and Visually Guided Action System Yu-Hung CHIEN*, Chien-Hsiung CHEN** * Graduate School of Design, National Taiwan University of Science and

More information

Learning relative directions between landmarks in a desktop virtual environment

Learning relative directions between landmarks in a desktop virtual environment Spatial Cognition and Computation 1: 131 144, 1999. 2000 Kluwer Academic Publishers. Printed in the Netherlands. Learning relative directions between landmarks in a desktop virtual environment WILLIAM

More information

Lesson Sampling Distribution of Differences of Two Proportions

Lesson Sampling Distribution of Differences of Two Proportions STATWAY STUDENT HANDOUT STUDENT NAME DATE INTRODUCTION The GPS software company, TeleNav, recently commissioned a study on proportions of people who text while they drive. The study suggests that there

More information

The Perception of Optical Flow in Driving Simulators

The Perception of Optical Flow in Driving Simulators University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2009 Driving Assessment Conference Jun 23rd, 12:00 AM The Perception of Optical Flow in Driving Simulators Zhishuai Yin Northeastern

More information

This is a postprint of. The influence of material cues on early grasping force. Bergmann Tiest, W.M., Kappers, A.M.L.

This is a postprint of. The influence of material cues on early grasping force. Bergmann Tiest, W.M., Kappers, A.M.L. This is a postprint of The influence of material cues on early grasping force Bergmann Tiest, W.M., Kappers, A.M.L. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 8618, 393-399 Published version: http://dx.doi.org/1.17/978-3-662-44193-_49

More information

Pascal Contest (Grade 9)

Pascal Contest (Grade 9) Canadian Mathematics Competition An activity of The Centre for Education in Mathematics and Computing, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario Pascal Contest (Grade 9) Wednesday, February 0, 00 C.M.C.

More information

Understanding Projection Systems

Understanding Projection Systems Understanding Projection Systems A Point: A point has no dimensions, a theoretical location that has neither length, width nor height. A point shows an exact location in space. It is important to understand

More information

CB Database: A change blindness database for objects in natural indoor scenes

CB Database: A change blindness database for objects in natural indoor scenes DOI 10.3758/s13428-015-0640-x CB Database: A change blindness database for objects in natural indoor scenes Preeti Sareen 1,2 & Krista A. Ehinger 1 & Jeremy M. Wolfe 1 # Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2015

More information

Spring 2017 Math 54 Test #2 Name:

Spring 2017 Math 54 Test #2 Name: Spring 2017 Math 54 Test #2 Name: You may use a TI calculator and formula sheets from the textbook. Show your work neatly and systematically for full credit. Total points: 101 1. (6) Suppose P(E) = 0.37

More information

Eye catchers in comics: Controlling eye movements in reading pictorial and textual media.

Eye catchers in comics: Controlling eye movements in reading pictorial and textual media. Eye catchers in comics: Controlling eye movements in reading pictorial and textual media. Takahide Omori Takeharu Igaki Faculty of Literature, Keio University Taku Ishii Centre for Integrated Research

More information

Perception vs. Reality: Challenge, Control And Mystery In Video Games

Perception vs. Reality: Challenge, Control And Mystery In Video Games Perception vs. Reality: Challenge, Control And Mystery In Video Games Ali Alkhafaji Ali.A.Alkhafaji@gmail.com Brian Grey Brian.R.Grey@gmail.com Peter Hastings peterh@cdm.depaul.edu Copyright is held by

More information

C A R I B B E A N E X A M I N A T I O N S C O U N C I L REPORT ON CANDIDATES WORK IN THE SECONDARY EDUCATION CERTIFICATE EXAMINATION MAY/JUNE 2010

C A R I B B E A N E X A M I N A T I O N S C O U N C I L REPORT ON CANDIDATES WORK IN THE SECONDARY EDUCATION CERTIFICATE EXAMINATION MAY/JUNE 2010 C A R I B B E A N E X A M I N A T I O N S C O U N C I L REPORT ON CANDIDATES WORK IN THE SECONDARY EDUCATION CERTIFICATE EXAMINATION MAY/JUNE 2010 TECHNICAL DRAWING GENERAL PROFICIENCY Copyright 2010 Caribbean

More information

Reducing the motor response in haptic parallel matching eliminates the typically observed gender difference

Reducing the motor response in haptic parallel matching eliminates the typically observed gender difference Exp Brain Res (2016) 234:105 112 DOI 10.1007/s00221-015-4437-z RESEARCH ARTICLE Reducing the motor response in haptic parallel matching eliminates the typically observed gender difference Hanneke I. van

More information

Influence of stimulus symmetry on visual scanning patterns*

Influence of stimulus symmetry on visual scanning patterns* Perception & Psychophysics 973, Vol. 3, No.3, 08-2 nfluence of stimulus symmetry on visual scanning patterns* PAUL J. LOCHERt and CALVN F. NODNE Temple University, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania 922 Eye movements

More information

Perception of room size and the ability of self localization in a virtual environment. Loudspeaker experiment

Perception of room size and the ability of self localization in a virtual environment. Loudspeaker experiment Perception of room size and the ability of self localization in a virtual environment. Loudspeaker experiment Marko Horvat University of Zagreb Faculty of Electrical Engineering and Computing, Zagreb,

More information