First evaluation of a novel tactile Shear Force Display

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "First evaluation of a novel tactile Shear Force Display"

Transcription

1 First evaluation of a novel tactile Shear Force Display KNUT DREWING 1, MICHAEL FRITSCHI 2, REGINE ZOPF 1, MARC O. ERNST 1 AND MARTIN BUSS 2 1 Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics, Tübingen, Germany 2 Institute of Automatic Control Engineering, Technische Universität München, Germany Based on existing knowledge on human movement perception we constructed a prototype of a novel multi-pin Shear Force Display (SFD). Two experiments, presented in this article, focus on the question of whether the prototype SFD generates tactile stimulation that is appropriate for the sensitivity of human tactile perception. In particular, Experiment I studied human resolution for distinguishing between different directions of pin movement and Experiment II explored the perceptual integration of multi-pin movements. Both experiments demonstrated that humans can well discriminate between directions of the shear forces displayed, and also that technically realized resolution of the device exceeds the perceptual resolution (> 14 ). In Experiment II we further found evidence that the human brain does not process stimulation from the different pins of the SFD independent of another at least concerning movement direction. The acquired psychophysical knowledge based on this new technology will in return be used to improve the design of the shear force display. Categories and Subject Descriptors: H1.2 [Models and Principles]: User/Machine Systems Human Information Processing; H5.2 [Information Interfaces and Presentation]: User Interfaces - Evaluation/methodology; Haptic I/O; Theory and methods General Terms: Design, Experimentation, Human Factors Additional Key Words and Phrases: Haptic interfaces, psychophysics, tactile movement perception, shear force 1. INTRODUCTION Human tactile perception is a complex integration of various sensations evoked by forces acting on our skin. Tactile displays are the fundamental technical component of a virtual tactile environment that tries to recreate these sensations by generating forces. For a long time tactile displays have usually been constructed either as shape or as vibrotactile displays. Shape displays follow the idea to render the 3D-shape of an object to the skin. This goal in many cases is achieved with an array of small pins that are This work is part of the TOUCH-HapSys project financially supported by the 5th Framework IST Program of the European Union, action line IST , contract number IST For the content of this paper the authors are solely responsible for, it does not necessarily represent the opinion of the European Community. Authors' addresses: Knut Drewing (corresponding author) is now at the Institute of Psychology, Giessen University, Otto-Behaghel-Str. 10F, Gießen, Germany; Knut.Drewing@psychol.uni-giessen.de, Regine Zopf, Marc Ernst, Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics, Spemannstraße 38, Tübingen, Germany; s: {regine.zopf, marc.ernst}@tuebingen.mpg.de, Michael Fritschi, Martin Buss, Institute of Automatic Control Engineering (LSR) Technische Universität München, München, Germany, s: {michael.fritschi, martin.buss}@ei.tum.de. Permission to make digital/hard copy of part of this work for personal or classroom use is granted without fee provided that the copies are not made or distributed for profit or commercial advantage, the copyright notice, the title of the publication, and its date of appear, and notice is given that copying is by permission of the ACM, Inc. To copy otherwise, to republish, to post on servers, or to redistribute to lists, requires prior specific permission and/or a fee. 200X ACM /00/ $5.00

2 Evaluation of Shear Force Display 2 mutually independent and move normal to the contact surface [Lee et al. 1999; Shinohara et al. 1998; Summers et al. 2001]. Then, the pins create a 3D-pattern of indentation into the skin. Vibrotactile displays, in contrast, use a pin array to display patterns of normal forces with high frequency (about 200Hz), but very small amplitude. Vibrotactile displays create rather 2D- than 3D-patterns on the skin and have been used as aid for the blind as well as for research [e.g., Essick, 1998; Ikei 2002; Ikei et al. 1997]. Both types of displays are usually based on forces that are normal to the contact surface. However, in order to generate a realistic impression of the environment it is probably as important to provide forces lateral to the human skin, so called shear forces. Meanwhile, there are a few prototypes or experimental displays that provide shear force in differing ways to the human finger [Konyo et al. 2000, 2003; Salada et al. 2002, 2004; Hayward and Cruz- Hernandez 2000]. The display of shear force is particularly reasonable when considering movements of the skin relative to the environment, e.g. when stroking with the finger across a surface. Tactile movement perception plays an important role in haptics and haptically guided action. To give an example, imagine gripping and lifting a fragile object like an egg: grip forces should be sufficiently large to avoid slip of the object, but also sufficiently small to avoid damage to the object. Immediate tactile sensation of skin slip has been demonstrated to be crucial for a precise grip force control [Johannson and Westling, 1990]. As well, movement perception on the skin is an always present effect of every active exploration with the fingers. We are specifically interested in tactile movement perception and the development of an appropriate multi-pin Shear Force Display (SFD) for moving stimuli. Previous results demonstrate that the quality of tactile displays depends strongly on the demands of the application [Kammermeier and Schmidt 2002]. The scientific work we conducted followed this concept. Based on existing knowledge on human movement perception we constructed a prototype of a novel multi-pin SFD. However, there are still many unanswered questions related to tactile perception because the technology was not yet available. Our approach is to use the prototype SFD to obtain additional psychophysical knowledge that in turn will be used to improve the design of the display. In this article, we first present the existing psychophysical background and, then, the design of our multi-pin SFD. Two own experiments focus on the question of whether the current SFD generates tactile stimulation that is appropriate for the sensitivity of human tactile perception. In particular, Experiment I studied human sensitivity for distinguishing

3 Evaluation of Shear Force Display 3 between different directions of horizontal 2D pin movement and Experiment II explored the perceptual effect resulting from the integration of movement information from multiple pins. 2. PSYCHOPHYSICS OF TACTILE MOVEMENT PERCEPTION The perception of movement across the skin has been demonstrated to rely upon two distinguishable cues, which in most natural situations co-occur: the spatio-temporally ordered translation of a stimulation across the skin and lateral stretch of the skin [Essick 1998]. An impressive tactile movement illusion can be easily generated attaching a comb to the finger and sliding a pencil along its teeth [Hayward and Cruz-Hernandez 2000]. In this illusion probably both translation and stretch cues are effective: The teeth sequentially stimulate adjacent areas and, thereby, stretch the skin. Translation without stretch inter alia has been investigated and can be mimicked by pin displays when sequentially producing indentation at neighbouring skin areas [Gardner and Sklar 1994, 1996; Olausson 1994]. Pure stretch was investigated using a small probe glued to the skin [Gould et al. 1979] or a sliding glass plate [Srinivasan et al. 1990]. Research suggests that human movement perception is noticeably more sensitive to the stretch than to the translation component, e.g. when comparing the minimal path traversed to detect movement (e.g., 0.6 mm for stretch vs 4.4 mm for translation on the forearm [Gould et al. 1979; cf. also Essick 1998]). This holds as long as surrounding skin is not prevented from the spreading influence of stretch [Essick 1998]. Likewise, our display has been constructed to provide full stretch cues. Neurally, movement perception relates particularly to two out of four mechanoreceptor types in the skin [review in Greenspan and Bolanowski 1996]. Translation mainly corresponds to a sequential activation of adjacent fast adapting FA 1 receptors [Essick 1998], whereas stretch evokes an initial pattern of response in fast adapting FA 1 receptors and a persisting one in slow adapting SA 2 receptors [LaMotte and Srinivasan, 1991; Srinivasan et al. 1990]. A couple of characteristics of these receptor types have been reported [Johnson 2002, Mountcastle et al. 1972]: In the finger, FA1 receptors have a receptive field area of about 5 mm² and an estimated spatial resolution of about 3 mm. FA1 receptors respond very sensitively to single rapid indentations and are, for vibrations, most sensitive in the range of 40 to 60 Hz. SA2 receptors have a larger receptive field (25-60 mm²), a lower spatial resolution of 7 mm or above and respond on frequencies up to 100Hz. They are quite insensitive to skin

4 Evaluation of Shear Force Display 4 indentation [Johansson and Vallbo, 1979a], but rather sensitive to stretch [Edin 1992]. The number of both FA 1 and SA 2 units seems to increase in more distal directions of the finger, particularly at the fingernail [Johansson and Vallbo, 1976, 1979b]. In display design we took into account the above basic knowledge on human neural bandwidths. On a higher perceptual level, psychophysical studies demonstrated that tactile movement detection and discrimination between movements in opposing directions depend on various factors such as the length of the movement path [Essick and McGuire, 1986; Olausson 1994], or the innervation density of the stimulated skin area [Loomis and Collins 1978; Whitsel et al. 1979]. However, not much is known on humans resolution of movement direction, which is an important parameter for display design. To our knowledge there is only a single study on this topic. Keyson and Houtsma [1995] displayed movements on the distal phalanx of the finger via a 0.8 mm diameter Braille point that was fixed to a trackball. The trackball could be moved in any horizontal direction. A single stimulus consisted of a forward-backward movement traversing twice a straight path of 3.25 mm with a velocity of 41 mm/s. For these stimuli, Keyson and Houtsma [1995] report discrimination thresholds for movement direction of about 14 with the lowest thresholds for paths that went down to the wrist. However, for different reasons these results are not applicable to our multi-pin display. From the technical point of view it is a common problem in tactile display design to realize high pin density at concurrently large excursion and high frequency of pin movement. Our SFD was designed as a multi-pin device rather than to display movement of a length and velocity that was investigated in the study from Keyson and Houtsma [1995]. Further, in the study skin friction and, thus, skin stretch as an important cue to movement perception was reduced, but our SFD allows for full stretch. Consequently, we tested in Experiment I human directional resolution for movements provided by our SFD. The questions were whether, on one hand, the SFD is able to provide stimuli that humans can discriminate and, on the other hand, whether its directional resolution satisfies human perceptual resolution. Moreover, the SFD is a multi-pin device. Because the technology, was not yet available, so far no knowledge exists on the perceptual aspects of multiple movement signals. Experiment II, hence, explored the perceptual integration of movement information from multi-pin stimulation. 3. TECHNICAL REALIZATION OF THE SHEAR FORCE DISPLAY 3.1 Design and hardware setup

5 Evaluation of Shear Force Display 5 The fundamental concept of the display is based on a quadratically arranged 2 X 2 pin array. To exert shear force to the area of the finger, each pin is movable laterally to the skin. It is possible to move each pin independently along both axes of their twodimensional horizontal directions. A pin diameter of 1 mm, a center-to-center pin spacing of 3 mm (zero position of lateral movement) and a lateral movement of 2 mm along each axis and for each pin are realized. Fig. 1. Mechanical design of the Shear Force Display for two pins in one axis. The human finger is in contact with the pins through a gap in the base plate covering the device. The depicted optional rubber layer between the pins and the finger was not used in the present study. The mechanical design of the SFD is schematically shown in Figure 1. The four pins are in direct contact with the finger (optional filtered by an elastic rubber layer). Two rods orthogonal attached to the upper region of each pin transmit two-dimensional movement of the pins. To allow for this movement the four pin bodies are attached with universaljoint shafts that are screwed to the ground plate of the chassis. Tuning screws between the base plate and the joints enable a justification along the z-axis in a small range to vary the indentation offset normal to the finger. The rods are connected over reduction rocker arms to the servo motor levers. To reduce pin motion normal to the skin down to a tolerable value, the length of the pin raised to 69 mm. In case of a pin diameter of 1 mm a realization of a pin with these dimensions would be unusable because of bending. Furthermore, it would be difficult to attach the two steering rods providing the tangential

6 Evaluation of Shear Force Display 6 motion. To avoid this problem, the length of the pin has been reduced to 6 mm and attached to a prismatic body (Figure 2, left). The right picture of Figure 2 shows the top view of the mechanical design. The pins are fitted to the inner edge of the bodies (black points) to provide a large motion workspace for the pins. On the actuator side, we use ofthe-shelf servo motors, selected on criteria such as high performance, small deviations, and light weight. The servo motors come with a position control circuit, accessed by a pulse-width modulated signal, which contains the commanded position information. Fig. 2. Pin-body design in side view and pin arrangement in top view. The pins are attached to a prismatic body and fitted to the inner edge of the bodies. The left picture in Figure 3 shows the SFD in use with a close-up view of the pin area that is in contact with the finger through a gap in the cover plate. A more detailed view in the right picture of Figure 3 shows the mechanical details of the display like the pin bodies, the control rods, and the servo motors. Fig. 3. Hardware Setup of the Shear Force Display. The left picture depicts the SFD in use and a close-up view of the pin area; the right picture shows the mechanical details.

7 Evaluation of Shear Force Display Computer Interface and Technical Data Figure 4 shows the schematic design of the computer interface. A real-time task generates the required signals for the eight servo motors at the printer-port of the computer, whose data lines are connected to the corresponding servo motors. In the application process the position information for the servo motors are calculated and communicated to the realtime task. To provide sufficient power for the servo motors an external power supply is used. Fig. 4. Computer interface. The SFD is connected to a computer via the parallel port, and controlled via a realtime task. The SFD reaches a positioning resolution of 10 µm in a work space of 2 X 2 mm. It works with a maximum velocity of 22.8 mm/s applying maximum forces of 4.23 N per pin axis. The SFD weighs 1100 g and has a total size of 150 X 150 X 90 mm. 4. EXPERIMENT I: DISCRIMINATION OF SHEAR FORCE DIRECTION In Experiment I we studied human discrimination performance for different directions of tactile movement displayed with the SFD. To our knowledge there is just a single study on thresholds for movement direction on the finger [Keyson and Houtsma 1995]. This study, however, investigated pin movements of a length and a velocity that our multi-pin device has not been designed to display and in that study skin stretch was reduced. We measured direction thresholds for single-pin movement of the SFD while allowing for full skin stretch. We did this for eight different directions of movement (up,

8 Evaluation of Shear Force Display 8 up-right, right, down-right, down, down-left, left, up-left) using a two-interval forced choice task and the method of constant stimuli. 4.1 Method Participants. 14 right-handed participants (9 female and 5 male) took part for pay. Their age ranged from 21 to 40 years (average 26 years). None of them reported any known tactile deficit due to accident or illness concerning the left index finger. With the exception of two staff members of the laboratory, the participants were not familiar with the SFD and naïve to the purpose of the experiment Apparatus and Stimuli. In a quiet room participants sat at a table with their left elbow resting comfortably on a custom-made support. By using robust tape their left hand was attached to the SFD, so that the distal phalanx of their left index finger was reliably centered at the midpoint of the pin display. Adjustable paperboard stabilizers on the left and right side of the finger further warranted the central finger position. Adaptable plates 1 pin Fig. 5. Adaptable plates. The plates can be moved along one axis (red arrows) and allow to adjust the gap in the cover plate to the individual finger size. In the present experiments the adaptable plates were used to maximize the skin area stretched by the pin. We used the SFD with a single pin only (for this experiment the other three pins were removed) and a particular cover plate that allowed for individual adaptation of gap size along the finger and, thus, maximal spread of skin stretch (Figure 5). The rectangular gap in the cover plate was individually adjusted so that it was beneath the entire distal phalanx of the index finger. Just at the very ends the phalanx was supported by the metal plate. The SFD was placed inside a box to prevent participants from possible visual cues on pin movement, and white noise displayed via headphones masked the sounds of the

9 1 mm Evaluation of Shear Force Display 9 SFD. A custom-made program on an IBM-compatible PC controlled the stimulus presentation and collected responses, which were entered via a keyboard. Tactile stimuli were unidirectional single strokes of the single pin with a length of 1 mm and a velocity of 10 mm/s. Each stroke started at the center of the SFD. Stroke directions are defined by their circular deviation in degree from the direction center-toupper part of the phalanx (0, cf. Figure 6, left) Design and Procedure. The design comprised one within-participant variable: Stroke Direction realized by 8 standard stimuli of different directions (separated by 45, Figure 6, left). For each standard stimulus we measured the 84 %-discrimination threshold for movement direction using the method of constant stimuli in a two-interval forced choice paradigm: Each standard was paired with 19 comparison strokes, the directions of which were distributed in 10 steps around the corresponding standard within a range of +/- 90 (Figure 6, right). In each of two experimental sessions each pair of standard and comparison stroke was presented six times (order of pairs randomized). The sessions were on different days, lasted two hours each (including practice trials in the first and three breaks in each session) and included 1824 trials overall. Standard directions Comparison directions (up) 45 Standard direction (left) 90 (right) pin (down) 135 Fig. 6. The left part of the figure depicts the eight standard directions for pin movements in Experiment I (orange arrows) and their length (1mm). The right part depicts the comparison directions (gray arrows including the orange standard) in respect to the standard direction. A single trial consisted of the sequential presentation of a standard and a comparison stimulus (order balanced between repetitions). Participants self-initiated a trial via a key press, 1600 ms later the first stimulus was presented, 2800ms thereafter the second stimulus. Immediately after each stimulus a 100 ms high pitch tone signaled the participant to lift his finger until 1.1 s later another 100 ms low pitch tone signaled to

10 Evaluation of Shear Force Display 10 lower the finger back onto the device. In this time the pin was driven back to the center of the display without stimulating the finger. Starting 100 ms before each stimulus presentation and ending with the low pitch tone white noise was displayed on the earphones ms after the second stimulus participants had to decide by a key press if the direction of the second stimulus had been more in clock- or counterclock-wise direction with respect to the first stimulus. Participants were instructed to envision the different directions as being drawn on a clock. No error feedback was given Data Analysis. Using the psignifit toolbox for Matlab [Wichmann and Hill 2001a, b] we fitted individual psychometric functions (cumulative Gaussians, Maximum- Likelihood procedure) to the proportion of trials in which the comparison stroke was perceived as more in clock-wise direction than the standard stroke - against the direction of the comparison. In the fit the point of subjective equality was fixed to the direction of the standard. We estimated the 84%-discrimination threshold and a percentage of stimulus-independent errors (lapse rate; constrained to 0 to 10%), the additional estimation of which reduces biases in the threshold estimate [Wichmann and Hill 2001a]. The individual values per Stroke Direction entered further analyses. Statistics were done using non-parametric methods in order to include threshold estimates that fell outside the reliably measured range of the present task (1% estimates < 10 and 4.5% > 90, i.e., the maximal deviation of comparison stimuli from the standard). 4.2 Results and Discussion Figure 7 depicts medians and quartiles of the individual discrimination thresholds by Stroke Direction. Median thresholds ranged from 23 (for direction 0 ) to 35 (for direction 270 ). The individual thresholds per Stroke Direction entered a Friedman test. The test reached significance, χ²(7)=20.7, p <.01, indicating a perceptual anisotropy. Bonferroni-adjusted post-hoc comparisons (Wilcoxon-tests) between pairs of directions in no case reached significance. But, the descriptive data (cf. Figure 7) suggest that discrimination performance is somewhat better for upward- as compared to other directions. Between participants median thresholds (across Stroke Direction) ranged from 21 to 78. A Friedman test confirmed that these individual differences were also reliable, χ²(13)=52.4, p <.001. These differences may point to pronounced individual differences in human resolution of tactile movement direction. But they may also reflect individual

11 Evaluation of Shear Force Display 11 difficulties with the use of the display, particularly given that the thresholds of most participants (11 out of 14) fell in the almost halved range between 21 and (up) Median 50 75th percentile th percentile (right) (down) Fig. 7. Medians and quartiles of individual 84% -thresholds for direction discrimination by Stroke Direction conditions of Experiment I. At first glance, the direction of the observed perceptual anisotropy seems to be in contrast with Keyson and Houtsma s [1995] previous results who observed the lowest thresholds for movements in down-direction. However, our results match well with the increasing densities of FA 1 and SA 2 receptors towards the more distal parts of the finger [Johansson and Vallbo, 1976], because these receptors types are crucial for movement perception. And, Keyson and Houtsma [1995] argue quite similar, namely that their downward movement caused tension in the finger tip where the finger is anchored to the fingernail and where humans are particular sensitive. It may well be that by the long movement path in their study (3.25 mm in one direction) a tension component came into play, which with the present shorter path (1 mm) did not considerably contribute to movement perception. In general, we observed larger thresholds for direction discrimination than Keyson and Houtsma [1995]. But this difference was to be expected: In Keyson and Houtsma s study 70.7 % correct responses defined the discrimination threshold, whereas we used a less liberal 84 % - criterion. Moreover, in their study the movements were considerably longer and faster than in the present experiment (6.5 vs. 1 mm, and 41 vs. 10 mm/sec, respectively). Tactile movement detection is known to improve both with increasing path length [Essick and McGuire, 1986; Olausson 1994] and up to a peak with increasing

12 Evaluation of Shear Force Display 12 velocity [Whitsel et al. 1979]. Corresponding relations for discrimination performance are reasonable. Most importantly here, the magnitude of the present discrimination thresholds demonstrated that almost all participants were able to discriminate well between the movements displayed by our SFD. It is also important to note that the technical directional resolution of the SFD clearly exceeds the perceptual thresholds we observed (> 20 ). Further development of the SFD may take advantage of the obtained knowledge on the range and the optimum of human directional resolution. Of rather theoretical than technological interest are the reliable, but small differences between Stroke Directions. 5. EXPERIMENT II: PERCPTUAL INTEGRATION OF MULTI-PIN PATTERNS Experiment I demonstrated that the SFD is appropriate for the perception of different directions of movement on the human finger. In a second step, Experiment II studied the integration of perceived movement direction displayed by multiple pins. That is, we tested whether discrimination performance improves from one to two and four pins (all moving in the same direction) due to the integration of movement information across the pins. According to well-established models on human signal-integration [Ernst and Banks 2002; Ernst and Bülthoff 2004], the human brain integrates independent redundant signals from the same physical property such that the integrated signal is less noisy and thus more reliable than each individual signal. This is, metaphorically spoken, because independent errors in the signals, partially, cancel each other. More reliable signals, of course, can be better discriminated than less reliable signals. From these theories, we predict that movement direction discrimination threshold should profit from multi- as compared to single-pin presentation if the pins provide at least partially independent signals on movement direction to the brain [Oruç et al. 2003]. Movements were displayed with either one, two or four pins, in parallel. We measured discrimination thresholds for movements in up and down-direction using, again, a twointerval forced choice task and the method of constant stimuli. 5.1 Method Participants. Twelve right-handed participants (6 female, 6 male) took part for pay. Their age ranged from 19 to 40 years (average 27 years). None of them reported any known tactile deficit due to accident or illness concerning the left index finger. Eight of

13 Evaluation of Shear Force Display 13 the participants were familiar with the SFD from Experiment I. With the exception of two staff members of the laboratory, participants were naïve to the purpose of the experiment Apparatus and Stimuli. Apparatus and stimuli were similar to Experiment I. However, we provided the tactile stimuli with one, two or four pins, in parallel (other pins removed from the SFD). That is, in multi-pin conditions all pins moved simultaneously and with identical velocity and direction; the pins were separated by 3 mm center-to-center distance (Figure 8, left) Design and Procedure. The design comprised two within-participant variables: Stroke Direction realized by 2 standard stimuli (0 [up] vs. 180 [down]), and Pin Number (1, 2, vs. 4 pins). For each condition we measured the 84%-discrimination threshold for movement direction using the identical procedure as in Experiment I. Pin number and arrangement 3 mm 3 mm 3 mm one pin two pins four pins Fig. 8. The left part of the figure depicts the two standard directions for movements in Experiment II (orange arrows) and their length (1mm). The right part depicts the spatial arrangement of pins in the different Pin Number conditions (3 mm center-to-center distance). Thus, the experiment consisted of 1368 trials (=2 directions X 3 pin numbers X 19 comparisons X 12 repetitions). Trials with identical pin number were blocked in one of three 1-hour sessions (on different days within one week); otherwise trials were randomized. The six possible orders of blocks were randomly assigned to the six female as well as to the six male participants Data Analysis. Data were analyzed similarly to Experiment I. 5.2 Results and Discussion Analyzed by Stroke Direction X Pin Number conditions median thresholds (see Figure 9) in this experiment were rather uniform, ranging from 19 to 23. Threshold quartiles ranged from a maximal 75%-percentile of 33 down to a minimal 25%- percentile of 16. The individual thresholds per condition entered a Friedman test. The test clearly failed to reach significance, χ²(5)=6.7, p >.20, demonstrating that participants'

14 Evaluation of Shear Force Display 14 discrimination performance did not differ between any combinations of movement direction and pin number. This means we were not able to replicate the advantage of updirections observed in Experiment I. Further, it means that we did not find an effect of Pin Number on discrimination performance. 84% discrimination threshold (up) 180 (down) 1 pin 2 pins 4 pins Fig. 9. Medians of individual 84%-thresholds for direction discrimination by Pin Number and Stroke Direction; error bars indicate 25%- and 75%-percentiles, respectively. Estimates of individual discrimination thresholds per condition ranged from 8 to 57 in Experiment II. Individual median thresholds across conditions ranged between 14 and 34. A Friedman-test between individuals, again, reached significance, χ²(11)=38.0, p <.001. However, as compared to Experiment I individual differences were less pronounced and thresholds tended to be lower. One reason for both observations may be that most participants were already familiar with the SFD reducing individual difficulties with its use. For the lower thresholds also memory effects may have played a role: There were substantially less different Stroke Directions in the present as compared to Experiment I. However, the magnitude of discrimination thresholds in Experiment II confirmed that humans well discriminate between the directions of the movements displayed and that the perceptual resolution (> 14 ) did not exceed the technical resolution of the SFD. Importantly, we did not find an effect of Pin Number on humans discrimination performance. Well-established models on human signal-integration [Ernst and Bülthoff 2004] predict that the human brain integrates independent redundant signals on the same

15 Evaluation of Shear Force Display 15 physical property such that the integrated signal is less noisy. The lack of an integration benefit in the present study, then, indicates that the movements of the different pins of the SFD were not independently processed in the perceptual system: Tactile movement processing is believed to rely upon FA 1 receptors with a spatial resolution of about 3 mm and upon SA 2 receptors with a resolution of 7 mm or above [Essick, 1998; Johnson 2002]. Thus, the 3 mm center-to-center distance of the pins reaches the resolution of the FA 1 receptors, so that at least some additional information on movement should have been provided by the additional pins. However, it was also discussed that regarding skin stretch particularly the SA 2 receptors display directional sensitivity in their responses [Westling and Johansson 1987; Srinivasan et al. 1990]. If direction discrimination of the stimuli provided by the SFD primarily relies upon stretch and less on translation cues e.g., because of the limited length of the movement path of a single pin, then the low resolution of the SA 2 receptors can explain why additional pins did not deliver independent additional information to the brain. Future evaluation will have to explore the role of stretch and translation cues regarding the stimuli provided by the SFD. As well, it is an interesting question to what degree non-simultaneous, differential multi-pin patterns of movement are able to evoke discriminable percepts. Future development of the SFD will have to include mechanisms that allow for an increased pin distance, which enables to obtain independent signals from the different pins. 6. CONCLUSION In this article we presented the buildup and evaluation of a novel multi-pin Shear Force Display. The design profited from basic knowledge on human psychophysics. But because the technology was not yet available, there are a couple of unanswered questions on the perceptual side. In two experiments we evaluated whether the SFD produces tactile stimuli that are appropriate for human sensitivity in tactile movement perception. Both experiments demonstrated that humans can well discriminate between directions of the shear forces displayed, and also that their perceptual resolution does not exceed the already realized technical resolution of the device. In this sense, we demonstrated that the SFD is able to produce tactile movement stimuli that are appropriate for human perception, and that shear forces can be used to mediate a differentiated impression of at least one environmental aspect. Future development of the device may take advantage of the obtained magnitude for the human direction threshold, which for no individual in any

16 Evaluation of Shear Force Display 16 experiment exceeded 14. In Experiment II we further found evidence that the human brain does not process stimulation from the different pins of the SFD independent of another at least concerning movement direction. From this evidence combined with further existing psychophysical knowledge, we concluded that the pin distance of the SFD can and should be increased. An increased pin distance provides also the option to implement an increased pin excursion. Beyond evaluation, we made some observations that might be interesting from a psychophysical point of view: In Experiment I we observed a perceptual anisotropy for movement direction, namely an advantage for the perception of movement in updirections. Consistent with the explanation of an anisotropy reported by Keyson and Houtsma s [1995] this can be explained by particular high density of movement receptors in the distal parts of the finger [Johansson and Vallbo, 1976, 1979b]. Further, the lack of integration benefit for multi-pin movements observed in Experiment II may argue for a particular role of SA 2 receptors in direction perception via stretch [cf. Srinivasan et al. 1990]. The SFD will allow to follow-up these observations more thoroughly in future research. Taken together, the approach to couple display design tightly with psychophysics proved successful from a theoretical as well as a technological point of view. Psychophysics helped to design a display that mediates a differentiated impression of one environmental aspect and to provide recommendations for future development of the display. In turn, the prototype of the SFD allowed to investigate certain psychophysical questions for the first time and to increase theoretical knowledge. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We wish to thank Christoph Lange for his assistance in computer programming. REFERENCES EDIN, B.B Quantitative analysis of static strain sensitivity in human mechanoreceptors from hairy skin. Journal of Neurophysiology 67, ERNST, M. O., AND BANKS, M. S Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature 415, ERNST, M. O., AND BÜLTHOFF, H. H Merging the senses into a robust percept. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8, ESSICK, G.K Factors Affecting Direction Discrimination of Moving Tactile Stimuli. In Neural Aspects of tactile sensation, J.W. MORLEY, Ed. Elsevier, Amsterdam, ESSICK, G.K., AND MCGUIRE, M.H Role of kinetic and static cues in human subjects evaluation of direction of cutaneous stimulus motion. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts 12, 14. GARDNER, E.P., AND SKLAR, B.F Discrimination of the direction of motion on the human hand: A psychophysical study of stimulation parameters. Journal of Neurophysiology, 71,

17 Evaluation of Shear Force Display 17 GARDNER, E.P., AND SKLAR, B.F Factors influencing discrimination of direction of motion on the human hand. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts 12, 798. GOULD, W.R., VIERCK, C.J., AND LUCK, M.M Cues supporting recognition of the orientation or direction of movement of tactile stimuli. In Sensory function of the skin of humans. Proceedings of the second international Symposium on the skin senses, D.R. KENSHALO, Ed. Plenum Press: New York, GREENSPAN, J.D., AND BOLANOWSKI, S.J The psychophysics of tactile perception and its peripheral physiological basis. In Pain and Touch (2 nd ed.), L. KRUGER, Ed. Academic Press, San Diego, CA, HAYWARD, V., AND CRUZ-HERNANDEZ, J.M Tactile display device using distributed lateral skin stretch. In Proc. of the Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems Symposium, ASME IMECE2000, Orlando, Florida, November 2000, Proc. ASME Vol. DSC-69-2, IKEI, Y TextureExplorer: A tactile and force display for virtual textures. In Proceedings of the 10th International HAPTICS 02 Symposium, Orlando, Florida, March 2002, IEEE, 327. IKEI, Y., WAKAMATSU, K., AND FUKUDA, S Texture presentation by vibratory tactile display-image based presentation of a tactile texture. In Virtual Reality Annual International Symposium, Albuquerque, NM, March 1997, IEEE, JOHANSSON, R.S., AND VALLBO, A.B Skin receptors in the human hand: An inference of some population properties. In Sensory functions of the skin with special reference to man, Y. ZOTTERMAN, Ed. Pergamon Press, Oxford, UK, JOHANSSON, R.S., AND VALLBO, A.B. 1979a. Detection of tactile stimuli. Thresholds of afferent units related to psychophysical thresholds in the human hand. Journal of Physiology 297, JOHANSSON, R.S., AND VALLBO, A.B. 1979b. Tactile sensibility in the human hand: Relative and absolute densities of four types of mechanoreceptive units in glabrous skin. Journal of Physiology 286, JOHANSSON, R.S., AND WESTLING, G Tactile afferent signals in control of precision grip. In Attention and Performance XIII, M. JEANNEROD, Ed. Erlbaum, Mahwah, JOHNSON, K. O Neural basis of haptic perception. In Stevens handbook of experimental psychology (Vol. 1. Sensation and Perception), H. PASHLER AND S. YANTIS, Eds. Wiley: New York, KAMMERMEIER, P., AND SCHMIDT, G Application-Specific Evaluation of Tactile Array Displays for the Human Fingertip. In Proceedings of the 2002 IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems IROS, Lausanne, Switzerland, October 2002, KEYSON, D. V., AND HOUTSMA, A. J. M Directional sensitivity to a tactile point stimulus moving across the fingerpad. Perception and Psychophysics 57, KONYO, M., AKAZAWA, K., TADOKORO, S. AND TAKAMORI, T Tactile feel display for virtual active touch. In Proceedings of the 2003 IEEE/RSJ Intl. Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems, Vol. 3, Las Vegas, Nevada, October 2003, KONYO, M., TADOKORO, S., AND TAKAMORI, T Artificial tactile feel display using soft gel actuators. In Proceedings of the 2000 IEEE International Conference on Robotics & Automation, San Francisco, CA, April 2000, LAMOTTE, R.H., AND SRINIVASAN, M.A Surface microgeometry: Tactile perception and neural encoding. In Information Processing in the somatosensory system,o. FRANZEN AND J. WESTMAN, Eds. Macmillan Press, London, LEE, J.-H. AHN, I.-S. AND PARK, J.-O Design and implementation of tactile feedback device using electromagnetic type. In Intelligent Robots and Systems IROS 99, Vol. 3, Korea, October 1999, LOOMIS, J.M., AND COLLINS, C.C Sensitivity to shifts of a point stimulus: An instance of tactile hyperacuity. Perception and Psychophysics 24, MOUNTCASTLE, V.B., LAMOTTE, R.H., AND CARLI, G Detection thresholds for stimuli in humans and monkeys: comparison with threshold events in mechanoreceptive afferent nerve fibers innervating the monkey hand. Journal of Neurophysiology 35, OLAUSSON, H The influence of spatial summation on human tactile directional sensibility. Somatosensory and Motor Research 11, ORUÇ, I., MALONEY, T.M., AND LANDY, M.S Weighted linear cue combination with possibly correlated error. Vision Research 43, SALADA, M., COLGATE, J., LEE, M., AND VISHTON, P Validating a novel approach to rendering fingertip contact sensations. In Proceedings of the 10th International HAPTICS 02 Symposium, Orlando, Florida, March 2002, IEEE, SALADA, M., COLGATE, J.E., VISHTON, P., AND FRANKEL, E Two experiments on the perception of slip at the fingertip. In Proceedings of the 12th International HAPTICS 04 Symposium, Chicago, Illinois, March 2004, IEEE, SHINOHARA, M., SHIMIZU, Y., AND MOCHIZUKI, A Three-dimensional tactile display for the blind. IEEE Transactions on Rehabilitation Engineering 6, pp SRINIVASAN, M.A., WHITEHOUSE, J.M., AND LAMOTTE, R.H Tactile detection of slip: Surface microgeometry and peripheral neural codes. Journal of Neurophysiology 63,

18 Evaluation of Shear Force Display 18 SUMMERS, I., CHANTER, C., SOUTHALL, A., AND BRADY, A Results from a tactile array on the fingertip, In Eurohaptics 2001: Conference Proceedings, Birmingham, UK, July 2001, C. BABER, M. FAINT, S. WALL, AND A.M. WING, Eds., University of Birmingham, WESTLING, AND JOHANSSON, 1987; Responses in glabrous skin mechanoreceptors during precision grip in humans. Experimental Brain Research 66, WHITSEL, B.L., DREYER, D.A., HOLLINS, M., AND YOUNG, M.G The coding of direction of tactile stimulus movement: Correlative psychophysical and electrophysiological data. In Sensory functions of the skin of humans, J.R. KENSHALO, Ed. Plenum Press, New York, NY, WICHMANN, F. A., AND HILL, N. J. 2001a. The psychometric function: I. Fitting, sampling, and goodness of fit. Perception and Psychophysics 63, WICHMANN, F. A., AND HILL, N. J. 2001b. The psychometric function: II. Bootstrap-based confidence intervals and sampling. Perception and Psychophysics 63,

First Evaluation of A Novel Tactile Display Exerting Shear Force via Lateral Displacement

First Evaluation of A Novel Tactile Display Exerting Shear Force via Lateral Displacement First Evaluation of A Novel Tactile Display Exerting Shear Force via Lateral Displacement KNUT DREWING Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics MICHAEL FRITSCHI Technische Universität München REGINE

More information

Haptic Perception & Human Response to Vibrations

Haptic Perception & Human Response to Vibrations Sensing HAPTICS Manipulation Haptic Perception & Human Response to Vibrations Tactile Kinesthetic (position / force) Outline: 1. Neural Coding of Touch Primitives 2. Functions of Peripheral Receptors B

More information

VIRTUAL FIGURE PRESENTATION USING PRESSURE- SLIPPAGE-GENERATION TACTILE MOUSE

VIRTUAL FIGURE PRESENTATION USING PRESSURE- SLIPPAGE-GENERATION TACTILE MOUSE VIRTUAL FIGURE PRESENTATION USING PRESSURE- SLIPPAGE-GENERATION TACTILE MOUSE Yiru Zhou 1, Xuecheng Yin 1, and Masahiro Ohka 1 1 Graduate School of Information Science, Nagoya University Email: ohka@is.nagoya-u.ac.jp

More information

Discrimination of Virtual Haptic Textures Rendered with Different Update Rates

Discrimination of Virtual Haptic Textures Rendered with Different Update Rates Discrimination of Virtual Haptic Textures Rendered with Different Update Rates Seungmoon Choi and Hong Z. Tan Haptic Interface Research Laboratory Purdue University 465 Northwestern Avenue West Lafayette,

More information

Lecture 7: Human haptics

Lecture 7: Human haptics ME 327: Design and Control of Haptic Systems Winter 2018 Lecture 7: Human haptics Allison M. Okamura Stanford University types of haptic sensing kinesthesia/ proprioception/ force cutaneous/ tactile Related

More information

Shape Memory Alloy Actuator Controller Design for Tactile Displays

Shape Memory Alloy Actuator Controller Design for Tactile Displays 34th IEEE Conference on Decision and Control New Orleans, Dec. 3-5, 995 Shape Memory Alloy Actuator Controller Design for Tactile Displays Robert D. Howe, Dimitrios A. Kontarinis, and William J. Peine

More information

Necessary Spatial Resolution for Realistic Tactile Feeling Display

Necessary Spatial Resolution for Realistic Tactile Feeling Display Proceedings of the 2001 IEEE International Conference on Robotics & Automation Seoul, Korea May 21-26, 2001 Necessary Spatial Resolution for Realistic Tactile Feeling Display Naoya ASAMURA, Tomoyuki SHINOHARA,

More information

Proceedings of the 33rd ISR (International Symposium on Robotics) October 7 11,

Proceedings of the 33rd ISR (International Symposium on Robotics) October 7 11, Method for eliciting tactile sensation using vibrating stimuli in tangential direction : Effect of frequency, amplitude and wavelength of vibrating stimuli on roughness perception NaoeTatara, Masayuki

More information

A Pilot Study: Introduction of Time-domain Segment to Intensity-based Perception Model of High-frequency Vibration

A Pilot Study: Introduction of Time-domain Segment to Intensity-based Perception Model of High-frequency Vibration A Pilot Study: Introduction of Time-domain Segment to Intensity-based Perception Model of High-frequency Vibration Nan Cao, Hikaru Nagano, Masashi Konyo, Shogo Okamoto 2 and Satoshi Tadokoro Graduate School

More information

A Tactile Display using Ultrasound Linear Phased Array

A Tactile Display using Ultrasound Linear Phased Array A Tactile Display using Ultrasound Linear Phased Array Takayuki Iwamoto and Hiroyuki Shinoda Graduate School of Information Science and Technology The University of Tokyo 7-3-, Bunkyo-ku, Hongo, Tokyo,

More information

Selective Stimulation to Skin Receptors by Suction Pressure Control

Selective Stimulation to Skin Receptors by Suction Pressure Control Selective Stimulation to Skin Receptors by Suction Pressure Control Yasutoshi MAKINO 1 and Hiroyuki SHINODA 1 1 Department of Information Physics and Computing, Graduate School of Information Science and

More information

Effects of Longitudinal Skin Stretch on the Perception of Friction

Effects of Longitudinal Skin Stretch on the Perception of Friction In the Proceedings of the 2 nd World Haptics Conference, to be held in Tsukuba, Japan March 22 24, 2007 Effects of Longitudinal Skin Stretch on the Perception of Friction Nicholas D. Sylvester William

More information

Exploring Surround Haptics Displays

Exploring Surround Haptics Displays Exploring Surround Haptics Displays Ali Israr Disney Research 4615 Forbes Ave. Suite 420, Pittsburgh, PA 15213 USA israr@disneyresearch.com Ivan Poupyrev Disney Research 4615 Forbes Ave. Suite 420, Pittsburgh,

More information

From Encoding Sound to Encoding Touch

From Encoding Sound to Encoding Touch From Encoding Sound to Encoding Touch Toktam Mahmoodi King s College London, UK http://www.ctr.kcl.ac.uk/toktam/index.htm ETSI STQ Workshop, May 2017 Immersing a person into the real environment with Very

More information

Proprioception & force sensing

Proprioception & force sensing Proprioception & force sensing Roope Raisamo Tampere Unit for Computer-Human Interaction (TAUCHI) School of Information Sciences University of Tampere, Finland Based on material by Jussi Rantala, Jukka

More information

Texture recognition using force sensitive resistors

Texture recognition using force sensitive resistors Texture recognition using force sensitive resistors SAYED, Muhammad, DIAZ GARCIA,, Jose Carlos and ALBOUL, Lyuba Available from Sheffield Hallam University Research

More information

Touch. Touch & the somatic senses. Josh McDermott May 13,

Touch. Touch & the somatic senses. Josh McDermott May 13, The different sensory modalities register different kinds of energy from the environment. Touch Josh McDermott May 13, 2004 9.35 The sense of touch registers mechanical energy. Basic idea: we bump into

More information

A Method of Selective Stimulation to Epidermal Skin Receptors for Realistic Touch Feedback

A Method of Selective Stimulation to Epidermal Skin Receptors for Realistic Touch Feedback Proceedings of IEEE Virtual Reality '99 Conference A Method of Selective Stimulation to Epidermal Skin Receptors for Realistic Touch Feedback Naoya ASAMURA, Nozomu YOKOYAMA and Hiroyuki SHINODA Department

More information

Biomimetic Design of Actuators, Sensors and Robots

Biomimetic Design of Actuators, Sensors and Robots Biomimetic Design of Actuators, Sensors and Robots Takashi Maeno, COE Member of autonomous-cooperative robotics group Department of Mechanical Engineering Keio University Abstract Biological life has greatly

More information

Remote Tactile Transmission with Time Delay for Robotic Master Slave Systems

Remote Tactile Transmission with Time Delay for Robotic Master Slave Systems Advanced Robotics 25 (2011) 1271 1294 brill.nl/ar Full paper Remote Tactile Transmission with Time Delay for Robotic Master Slave Systems S. Okamoto a,, M. Konyo a, T. Maeno b and S. Tadokoro a a Graduate

More information

Design and Evaluation of Tactile Number Reading Methods on Smartphones

Design and Evaluation of Tactile Number Reading Methods on Smartphones Design and Evaluation of Tactile Number Reading Methods on Smartphones Fan Zhang fanzhang@zjicm.edu.cn Shaowei Chu chu@zjicm.edu.cn Naye Ji jinaye@zjicm.edu.cn Ruifang Pan ruifangp@zjicm.edu.cn Abstract

More information

Rendering Moving Tactile Stroke on the Palm Using a Sparse 2D Array

Rendering Moving Tactile Stroke on the Palm Using a Sparse 2D Array Rendering Moving Tactile Stroke on the Palm Using a Sparse 2D Array Jaeyoung Park 1(&), Jaeha Kim 1, Yonghwan Oh 1, and Hong Z. Tan 2 1 Korea Institute of Science and Technology, Seoul, Korea {jypcubic,lithium81,oyh}@kist.re.kr

More information

The Shape-Weight Illusion

The Shape-Weight Illusion The Shape-Weight Illusion Mirela Kahrimanovic, Wouter M. Bergmann Tiest, and Astrid M.L. Kappers Universiteit Utrecht, Helmholtz Institute Padualaan 8, 3584 CH Utrecht, The Netherlands {m.kahrimanovic,w.m.bergmanntiest,a.m.l.kappers}@uu.nl

More information

Wearable Haptic Display to Present Gravity Sensation

Wearable Haptic Display to Present Gravity Sensation Wearable Haptic Display to Present Gravity Sensation Preliminary Observations and Device Design Kouta Minamizawa*, Hiroyuki Kajimoto, Naoki Kawakami*, Susumu, Tachi* (*) The University of Tokyo, Japan

More information

Research Article Haptic Stylus and Empirical Studies on Braille, Button, and Texture Display

Research Article Haptic Stylus and Empirical Studies on Braille, Button, and Texture Display Hindawi Publishing Corporation Journal of Biomedicine and Biotechnology Volume 2008, Article ID 369651, 11 pages doi:10.1155/2008/369651 Research Article Haptic Stylus and Empirical Studies on Braille,

More information

A cutaneous stretch device for forearm rotational guidace

A cutaneous stretch device for forearm rotational guidace Chapter A cutaneous stretch device for forearm rotational guidace Within the project, physical exercises and rehabilitative activities are paramount aspects for the resulting assistive living environment.

More information

Ultrasound Tactile Display for Stress Field Reproduction -Examination of Non-Vibratory Tactile Apparent Movement-

Ultrasound Tactile Display for Stress Field Reproduction -Examination of Non-Vibratory Tactile Apparent Movement- Ultrasound Tactile Display for Stress Field Reproduction -Examination of Non-Vibratory Tactile Apparent Movement- Takayuki Iwamoto and Hiroyuki Shinoda Graduate School of Information Science and Technology,

More information

Haptic Discrimination of Perturbing Fields and Object Boundaries

Haptic Discrimination of Perturbing Fields and Object Boundaries Haptic Discrimination of Perturbing Fields and Object Boundaries Vikram S. Chib Sensory Motor Performance Program, Laboratory for Intelligent Mechanical Systems, Biomedical Engineering, Northwestern Univ.

More information

Salient features make a search easy

Salient features make a search easy Chapter General discussion This thesis examined various aspects of haptic search. It consisted of three parts. In the first part, the saliency of movability and compliance were investigated. In the second

More information

The Integument Laboratory

The Integument Laboratory Name Period Ms. Pfeil A# Activity: 1 Visualizing Changes in Skin Color Due to Continuous External Pressure Go to the supply area and obtain a small glass plate. Press the heel of your hand firmly against

More information

Here I present more details about the methods of the experiments which are. described in the main text, and describe two additional examinations which

Here I present more details about the methods of the experiments which are. described in the main text, and describe two additional examinations which Supplementary Note Here I present more details about the methods of the experiments which are described in the main text, and describe two additional examinations which assessed DF s proprioceptive performance

More information

Tactile Actuators Using SMA Micro-wires and the Generation of Texture Sensation from Images

Tactile Actuators Using SMA Micro-wires and the Generation of Texture Sensation from Images IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems (IROS) November -,. Tokyo, Japan Tactile Actuators Using SMA Micro-wires and the Generation of Texture Sensation from Images Yuto Takeda

More information

Speech, Hearing and Language: work in progress. Volume 12

Speech, Hearing and Language: work in progress. Volume 12 Speech, Hearing and Language: work in progress Volume 12 2 Construction of a rotary vibrator and its application in human tactile communication Abbas HAYDARI and Stuart ROSEN Department of Phonetics and

More information

The Effect of Frequency Shifting on Audio-Tactile Conversion for Enriching Musical Experience

The Effect of Frequency Shifting on Audio-Tactile Conversion for Enriching Musical Experience The Effect of Frequency Shifting on Audio-Tactile Conversion for Enriching Musical Experience Ryuta Okazaki 1,2, Hidenori Kuribayashi 3, Hiroyuki Kajimioto 1,4 1 The University of Electro-Communications,

More information

Haptic Cueing of a Visual Change-Detection Task: Implications for Multimodal Interfaces

Haptic Cueing of a Visual Change-Detection Task: Implications for Multimodal Interfaces In Usability Evaluation and Interface Design: Cognitive Engineering, Intelligent Agents and Virtual Reality (Vol. 1 of the Proceedings of the 9th International Conference on Human-Computer Interaction),

More information

Haptic presentation of 3D objects in virtual reality for the visually disabled

Haptic presentation of 3D objects in virtual reality for the visually disabled Haptic presentation of 3D objects in virtual reality for the visually disabled M Moranski, A Materka Institute of Electronics, Technical University of Lodz, Wolczanska 211/215, Lodz, POLAND marcin.moranski@p.lodz.pl,

More information

Dimensional Reduction of High-Frequency Accelerations for Haptic Rendering

Dimensional Reduction of High-Frequency Accelerations for Haptic Rendering Dimensional Reduction of High-Frequency Accelerations for Haptic Rendering Nils Landin, Joseph M. Romano, William McMahan, and Katherine J. Kuchenbecker KTH Royal Institute of Technology, Stockholm, Sweden

More information

the human chapter 1 Traffic lights the human User-centred Design Light Vision part 1 (modified extract for AISD 2005) Information i/o

the human chapter 1 Traffic lights the human User-centred Design Light Vision part 1 (modified extract for AISD 2005) Information i/o Traffic lights chapter 1 the human part 1 (modified extract for AISD 2005) http://www.baddesigns.com/manylts.html User-centred Design Bad design contradicts facts pertaining to human capabilities Usability

More information

Comparison of Skin Stretch and Vibrotactile Stimulation for Feedback of Proprioceptive Information

Comparison of Skin Stretch and Vibrotactile Stimulation for Feedback of Proprioceptive Information Comparison of Skin Stretch and Vibrotactile Stimulation for Feedback of Proprioceptive Information Karlin Bark Jason W. Wheeler Sunthar Premakumar Mark R. Cutkosky Center for Design Research Department

More information

Somatosensory Reception. Somatosensory Reception

Somatosensory Reception. Somatosensory Reception Somatosensory Reception Professor Martha Flanders fland001 @ umn.edu 3-125 Jackson Hall Proprioception, Tactile sensation, (pain and temperature) All mechanoreceptors respond to stretch Classified by adaptation

More information

TACTILE SENSING & FEEDBACK

TACTILE SENSING & FEEDBACK TACTILE SENSING & FEEDBACK Jukka Raisamo Multimodal Interaction Research Group Tampere Unit for Computer-Human Interaction Department of Computer Sciences University of Tampere, Finland Contents Tactile

More information

Perception of pitch. Importance of pitch: 2. mother hemp horse. scold. Definitions. Why is pitch important? AUDL4007: 11 Feb A. Faulkner.

Perception of pitch. Importance of pitch: 2. mother hemp horse. scold. Definitions. Why is pitch important? AUDL4007: 11 Feb A. Faulkner. Perception of pitch AUDL4007: 11 Feb 2010. A. Faulkner. See Moore, BCJ Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing, Chapter 5. Or Plack CJ The Sense of Hearing Lawrence Erlbaum, 2005 Chapter 7 1 Definitions

More information

Thresholds for Dynamic Changes in a Rotary Switch

Thresholds for Dynamic Changes in a Rotary Switch Proceedings of EuroHaptics 2003, Dublin, Ireland, pp. 343-350, July 6-9, 2003. Thresholds for Dynamic Changes in a Rotary Switch Shuo Yang 1, Hong Z. Tan 1, Pietro Buttolo 2, Matthew Johnston 2, and Zygmunt

More information

Expression of 2DOF Fingertip Traction with 1DOF Lateral Skin Stretch

Expression of 2DOF Fingertip Traction with 1DOF Lateral Skin Stretch Expression of 2DOF Fingertip Traction with 1DOF Lateral Skin Stretch Vibol Yem 1, Mai Shibahara 2, Katsunari Sato 2, Hiroyuki Kajimoto 1 1 The University of Electro-Communications, Tokyo, Japan 2 Nara

More information

Vibrotactile Apparent Movement by DC Motors and Voice-coil Tactors

Vibrotactile Apparent Movement by DC Motors and Voice-coil Tactors Vibrotactile Apparent Movement by DC Motors and Voice-coil Tactors Masataka Niwa 1,2, Yasuyuki Yanagida 1, Haruo Noma 1, Kenichi Hosaka 1, and Yuichiro Kume 3,1 1 ATR Media Information Science Laboratories

More information

III. Publication III. c 2005 Toni Hirvonen.

III. Publication III. c 2005 Toni Hirvonen. III Publication III Hirvonen, T., Segregation of Two Simultaneously Arriving Narrowband Noise Signals as a Function of Spatial and Frequency Separation, in Proceedings of th International Conference on

More information

Limitations of Vibrotactile Thresholds

Limitations of Vibrotactile Thresholds See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/327775794 Limitations of Vibrotactile Thresholds Conference Paper September 2016 CITATIONS

More information

Perception of pitch. Definitions. Why is pitch important? BSc Audiology/MSc SHS Psychoacoustics wk 5: 12 Feb A. Faulkner.

Perception of pitch. Definitions. Why is pitch important? BSc Audiology/MSc SHS Psychoacoustics wk 5: 12 Feb A. Faulkner. Perception of pitch BSc Audiology/MSc SHS Psychoacoustics wk 5: 12 Feb 2009. A. Faulkner. See Moore, BCJ Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing, Chapter 5. Or Plack CJ The Sense of Hearing Lawrence

More information

Biomimetic whiskers for shape recognition

Biomimetic whiskers for shape recognition Robotics and Autonomous Systems 55 (2007) 229 243 www.elsevier.com/locate/robot Biomimetic whiskers for shape recognition DaeEun Kim a,, Ralf Möller b a Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain

More information

Illusion of Surface Changes induced by Tactile and Visual Touch Feedback

Illusion of Surface Changes induced by Tactile and Visual Touch Feedback Illusion of Surface Changes induced by Tactile and Visual Touch Feedback Katrin Wolf University of Stuttgart Pfaffenwaldring 5a 70569 Stuttgart Germany katrin.wolf@vis.uni-stuttgart.de Second Author VP

More information

Perception of pitch. Definitions. Why is pitch important? BSc Audiology/MSc SHS Psychoacoustics wk 4: 7 Feb A. Faulkner.

Perception of pitch. Definitions. Why is pitch important? BSc Audiology/MSc SHS Psychoacoustics wk 4: 7 Feb A. Faulkner. Perception of pitch BSc Audiology/MSc SHS Psychoacoustics wk 4: 7 Feb 2008. A. Faulkner. See Moore, BCJ Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing, Chapter 5. Or Plack CJ The Sense of Hearing Lawrence Erlbaum,

More information

Reconsideration of Ouija Board Motion in Terms of Haptic Illusions (Ⅲ) -Experiment with 1-DoF Linear Rail Device-

Reconsideration of Ouija Board Motion in Terms of Haptic Illusions (Ⅲ) -Experiment with 1-DoF Linear Rail Device- Reconsideration of Ouija Board Motion in Terms of Haptic Illusions (Ⅲ) -Experiment with 1-DoF Linear Rail Device- Takahiro Shitara, Yuriko Nakai, Haruya Uematsu, Vibol Yem, and Hiroyuki Kajimoto, The University

More information

Flexible Active Touch Using 2.5D Display Generating Tactile and Force Sensations

Flexible Active Touch Using 2.5D Display Generating Tactile and Force Sensations This is the accepted version of the following article: ICIC Express Letters 6(12):2995-3000 January 2012, which has been published in final form at http://www.ijicic.org/el-6(12).htm Flexible Active Touch

More information

Haptic User Interfaces Fall Contents TACTILE SENSING & FEEDBACK. Tactile sensing. Tactile sensing. Mechanoreceptors 2/3. Mechanoreceptors 1/3

Haptic User Interfaces Fall Contents TACTILE SENSING & FEEDBACK. Tactile sensing. Tactile sensing. Mechanoreceptors 2/3. Mechanoreceptors 1/3 Contents TACTILE SENSING & FEEDBACK Jukka Raisamo Multimodal Interaction Research Group Tampere Unit for Computer Human Interaction Department of Computer Sciences University of Tampere, Finland Tactile

More information

Design of New Micro Actuator for Tactile Display

Design of New Micro Actuator for Tactile Display Proceedings of the 17th World Congress The International Federation of Automatic Control Design of New Micro Actuator for Tactile Display Tae-Heon Yang*, Sang Youn Kim**, and Dong-Soo Kwon*** * Department

More information

Dimensional Reduction of High-Frequencey Accelerations for Haptic Rendering

Dimensional Reduction of High-Frequencey Accelerations for Haptic Rendering University of Pennsylvania ScholarlyCommons Departmental Papers (MEAM) Department of Mechanical Engineering & Applied Mechanics 7-2010 Dimensional Reduction of High-Frequencey Accelerations for Haptic

More information

Comparison of Human Haptic Size Discrimination Performance in Simulated Environments with Varying Levels of Force and Stiffness

Comparison of Human Haptic Size Discrimination Performance in Simulated Environments with Varying Levels of Force and Stiffness Comparison of Human Haptic Size Discrimination Performance in Simulated Environments with Varying Levels of Force and Stiffness Gina Upperman, Atsushi Suzuki, and Marcia O Malley Mechanical Engineering

More information

Touch Perception and Emotional Appraisal for a Virtual Agent

Touch Perception and Emotional Appraisal for a Virtual Agent Touch Perception and Emotional Appraisal for a Virtual Agent Nhung Nguyen, Ipke Wachsmuth, Stefan Kopp Faculty of Technology University of Bielefeld 33594 Bielefeld Germany {nnguyen, ipke, skopp}@techfak.uni-bielefeld.de

More information

Spatial Judgments from Different Vantage Points: A Different Perspective

Spatial Judgments from Different Vantage Points: A Different Perspective Spatial Judgments from Different Vantage Points: A Different Perspective Erik Prytz, Mark Scerbo and Kennedy Rebecca The self-archived postprint version of this journal article is available at Linköping

More information

INVESTIGATION OF VHI AFFECTED BY THE DENSITY OF MECHANORECEPTIVE UNITS FOR VIRTUAL SENSATION

INVESTIGATION OF VHI AFFECTED BY THE DENSITY OF MECHANORECEPTIVE UNITS FOR VIRTUAL SENSATION INVESTIGATION OF VHI AFFECTED BY THE DENSITY OF MECHANORECEPTIVE UNITS FOR VIRTUAL SENSATION N.Rajaei 1, M.Ohka 1, T.Miyaoka 2, Hanafiah Yussof 3, Ahmad Khushairy Makhtar 3, Siti Nora Basir 3 1 Graduate

More information

Differences in Fitts Law Task Performance Based on Environment Scaling

Differences in Fitts Law Task Performance Based on Environment Scaling Differences in Fitts Law Task Performance Based on Environment Scaling Gregory S. Lee and Bhavani Thuraisingham Department of Computer Science University of Texas at Dallas 800 West Campbell Road Richardson,

More information

This is a postprint of. The influence of material cues on early grasping force. Bergmann Tiest, W.M., Kappers, A.M.L.

This is a postprint of. The influence of material cues on early grasping force. Bergmann Tiest, W.M., Kappers, A.M.L. This is a postprint of The influence of material cues on early grasping force Bergmann Tiest, W.M., Kappers, A.M.L. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 8618, 393-399 Published version: http://dx.doi.org/1.17/978-3-662-44193-_49

More information

Chapter 73. Two-Stroke Apparent Motion. George Mather

Chapter 73. Two-Stroke Apparent Motion. George Mather Chapter 73 Two-Stroke Apparent Motion George Mather The Effect One hundred years ago, the Gestalt psychologist Max Wertheimer published the first detailed study of the apparent visual movement seen when

More information

A Study of Perceptual Performance in Haptic Virtual Environments

A Study of Perceptual Performance in Haptic Virtual Environments Paper: Rb18-4-2617; 2006/5/22 A Study of Perceptual Performance in Haptic Virtual Marcia K. O Malley, and Gina Upperman Mechanical Engineering and Materials Science, Rice University 6100 Main Street, MEMS

More information

Estimating Friction Using Incipient Slip Sensing During a Manipulation Task

Estimating Friction Using Incipient Slip Sensing During a Manipulation Task Estimating Friction Using Incipient Slip Sensing During a Manipulation Task Marc R. Tremblay Mark R. Cutkosky Center for Design Research Building 2-53, Duena Street Stanford University Stanford, CA 9435-426

More information

Title: A Comparison of Different Tactile Output Devices In An Aviation Application

Title: A Comparison of Different Tactile Output Devices In An Aviation Application Page 1 of 6; 12/2/08 Thesis Proposal Title: A Comparison of Different Tactile Output Devices In An Aviation Application Student: Sharath Kanakamedala Advisor: Christopher G. Prince Proposal: (1) Provide

More information

Spatial Low Pass Filters for Pin Actuated Tactile Displays

Spatial Low Pass Filters for Pin Actuated Tactile Displays Spatial Low Pass Filters for Pin Actuated Tactile Displays Jaime M. Lee Harvard University lee@fas.harvard.edu Christopher R. Wagner Harvard University cwagner@fas.harvard.edu S. J. Lederman Queen s University

More information

MANY haptic devices used in research applications are

MANY haptic devices used in research applications are IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON HAPTICS, VOL. 6, NO. 4, OCTOBER-DECEMBER 2013 453 Human Detection and Discrimination of Tactile Repeatability, Mechanical Backlash, and Temporal Delay in a Combined Tactile-Kinesthetic

More information

Haptic Invitation of Textures: An Estimation of Human Touch Motions

Haptic Invitation of Textures: An Estimation of Human Touch Motions Haptic Invitation of Textures: An Estimation of Human Touch Motions Hikaru Nagano, Shogo Okamoto, and Yoji Yamada Department of Mechanical Science and Engineering, Graduate School of Engineering, Nagoya

More information

Response of SAI Afferents May Play a Role in the Perception of Velvet Hand Illusion

Response of SAI Afferents May Play a Role in the Perception of Velvet Hand Illusion Journal of Computer Science 6 (8): 934-939, 2010 ISSN 1549-3636 2010 Science Publications Response of SAI Afferents May Play a Role in the Perception of Velvet Hand Illusion 1 Abdullah Chami, 1 Masahiro

More information

Design of Cylindrical Whole-hand Haptic Interface using Electrocutaneous Display

Design of Cylindrical Whole-hand Haptic Interface using Electrocutaneous Display Design of Cylindrical Whole-hand Haptic Interface using Electrocutaneous Display Hiroyuki Kajimoto 1,2 1 The University of Electro-Communications 1-5-1 Chofugaoka, Chofu, Tokyo 182-8585 Japan 2 Japan Science

More information

Virtual Chromatic Percussions Simulated by Pseudo-Haptic and Vibrotactile Feedback

Virtual Chromatic Percussions Simulated by Pseudo-Haptic and Vibrotactile Feedback Virtual Chromatic Percussions Simulated by Pseudo-Haptic and Vibrotactile Feedback Taku Hachisu The University of Electro- Communications 1-5-1 Chofugaoka, Chofu, Tokyo 182-8585, Japan +81 42 443 5363

More information

Computer Haptics and Applications

Computer Haptics and Applications Computer Haptics and Applications EURON Summer School 2003 Cagatay Basdogan, Ph.D. College of Engineering Koc University, Istanbul, 80910 (http://network.ku.edu.tr/~cbasdogan) Resources: EURON Summer School

More information

Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion

Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion Vision Research 42 (2002) 2731 2735 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion Erich W. Graf *, Wendy J. Adams, Martin Lages Department

More information

Vibration Feedback Models for Virtual Environments

Vibration Feedback Models for Virtual Environments Presented at the 1998 IEEE International Conference on Robotics and Automation May 16-2, 1998, Leuven, Belgium Vibration Feedback Models for Virtual Environments Allison M. Okamura, 1,2 Jack T. Dennerlein

More information

The influence of changing haptic refresh-rate on subjective user experiences - lessons for effective touchbased applications.

The influence of changing haptic refresh-rate on subjective user experiences - lessons for effective touchbased applications. The influence of changing haptic refresh-rate on subjective user experiences - lessons for effective touchbased applications. Stuart Booth 1, Franco De Angelis 2 and Thore Schmidt-Tjarksen 3 1 University

More information

Elements of Haptic Interfaces

Elements of Haptic Interfaces Elements of Haptic Interfaces Katherine J. Kuchenbecker Department of Mechanical Engineering and Applied Mechanics University of Pennsylvania kuchenbe@seas.upenn.edu Course Notes for MEAM 625, University

More information

Comparison of Haptic and Non-Speech Audio Feedback

Comparison of Haptic and Non-Speech Audio Feedback Comparison of Haptic and Non-Speech Audio Feedback Cagatay Goncu 1 and Kim Marriott 1 Monash University, Mebourne, Australia, cagatay.goncu@monash.edu, kim.marriott@monash.edu Abstract. We report a usability

More information

A Three-Channel Model for Generating the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex in Each Eye

A Three-Channel Model for Generating the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex in Each Eye A Three-Channel Model for Generating the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex in Each Eye LAURENCE R. HARRIS, a KARL A. BEYKIRCH, b AND MICHAEL FETTER c a Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, Canada

More information

Chapter 2 Introduction to Haptics 2.1 Definition of Haptics

Chapter 2 Introduction to Haptics 2.1 Definition of Haptics Chapter 2 Introduction to Haptics 2.1 Definition of Haptics The word haptic originates from the Greek verb hapto to touch and therefore refers to the ability to touch and manipulate objects. The haptic

More information

Touching and Walking: Issues in Haptic Interface

Touching and Walking: Issues in Haptic Interface Touching and Walking: Issues in Haptic Interface Hiroo Iwata 1 1 Institute of Engineering Mechanics and Systems, University of Tsukuba, 80, Tsukuba, 305-8573 Japan iwata@kz.tsukuba.ac.jp Abstract. This

More information

Design of a Fingertip-Mounted Tactile Display with Tangential Skin Displacement Feedback

Design of a Fingertip-Mounted Tactile Display with Tangential Skin Displacement Feedback Gleeson, B.T., Horschel, S.K., and Provancher, W. R., Design of a Fingertip-Mounted Tactile Display with Tangential Skin Displacement Feedback, IEEE Transactions on Haptics, Oct-Dec, 2010 (in press). IEEE

More information

VISUAL VESTIBULAR INTERACTIONS FOR SELF MOTION ESTIMATION

VISUAL VESTIBULAR INTERACTIONS FOR SELF MOTION ESTIMATION VISUAL VESTIBULAR INTERACTIONS FOR SELF MOTION ESTIMATION Butler J 1, Smith S T 2, Beykirch K 1, Bülthoff H H 1 1 Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics, Tübingen, Germany 2 University College

More information

Tactile Illusion Caused by Tangential Skin Srain and Analysis In Terms of Skin Deformation

Tactile Illusion Caused by Tangential Skin Srain and Analysis In Terms of Skin Deformation Proceedings of Eurohaptics 28, LNCS 524, Springer-Verlag, pp. 229 237 http://www.disam.upm.es/~eurohaptics28/ Tactile Illusion Caused by Tangential Skin Srain and Analysis In Terms of Skin Deformation

More information

TOUCH screens are an indispensable part of our lives.

TOUCH screens are an indispensable part of our lives. JOURNAL OF L A T E X CLASS FILES, VOL., NO., 218 1 Tactile Masking by Electrovibration Yasemin Vardar, Member, IEEE, Burak Güçlü, and Cagatay Basdogan, Member, IEEE Abstract Future touch screen applications

More information

Figure 2. Haptic human perception and display. 2.2 Pseudo-Haptic Feedback 2. RELATED WORKS 2.1 Haptic Simulation of Tapping an Object

Figure 2. Haptic human perception and display. 2.2 Pseudo-Haptic Feedback 2. RELATED WORKS 2.1 Haptic Simulation of Tapping an Object Virtual Chromatic Percussions Simulated by Pseudo-Haptic and Vibrotactile Feedback Taku Hachisu 1 Gabriel Cirio 2 Maud Marchal 2 Anatole Lécuyer 2 Hiroyuki Kajimoto 1,3 1 The University of Electro- Communications

More information

Vibrotactile Device for Optimizing Skin Response to Vibration Abstract Motivation

Vibrotactile Device for Optimizing Skin Response to Vibration Abstract Motivation Vibrotactile Device for Optimizing Skin Response to Vibration Kou, W. McGuire, J. Meyer, A. Wang, A. Department of Biomedical Engineering, University of Wisconsin-Madison Abstract It is important to understand

More information

Haptics ME7960, Sect. 007 Lect. 7: Device Design II

Haptics ME7960, Sect. 007 Lect. 7: Device Design II Haptics ME7960, Sect. 007 Lect. 7: Device Design II Spring 2011 Prof. William Provancher University of Utah Salt Lake City, UT USA We would like to acknowledge the many colleagues whose course materials

More information

Arbitrating Multimodal Outputs: Using Ambient Displays as Interruptions

Arbitrating Multimodal Outputs: Using Ambient Displays as Interruptions Arbitrating Multimodal Outputs: Using Ambient Displays as Interruptions Ernesto Arroyo MIT Media Laboratory 20 Ames Street E15-313 Cambridge, MA 02139 USA earroyo@media.mit.edu Ted Selker MIT Media Laboratory

More information

Development of Thermal Displays for Haptic Interfaces

Development of Thermal Displays for Haptic Interfaces Development of Thermal Displays for Haptic Interfaces by Anshul Singhal B.Tech. in Production and Industrial Engineering Indian Institute of Technology Delhi, 2012 Submitted to the Department of Mechanical

More information

Haptic Identification of Stiffness and Force Magnitude

Haptic Identification of Stiffness and Force Magnitude Haptic Identification of Stiffness and Force Magnitude Steven A. Cholewiak, 1 Hong Z. Tan, 1 and David S. Ebert 2,3 1 Haptic Interface Research Laboratory 2 Purdue University Rendering and Perceptualization

More information

Feeding human senses through Immersion

Feeding human senses through Immersion Virtual Reality Feeding human senses through Immersion 1. How many human senses? 2. Overview of key human senses 3. Sensory stimulation through Immersion 4. Conclusion Th3.1 1. How many human senses? [TRV

More information

Building a Cognitive Model of Tactile Sensations Based on Vibrotactile Stimuli

Building a Cognitive Model of Tactile Sensations Based on Vibrotactile Stimuli Building a Cognitive Model of Tactile Sensations Based on Vibrotactile Stimuli Yuichi Muramatsu and Mihoko Niitsuma Department of Precision Mechanics Chuo University Tokyo, Japan Abstract We investigated

More information

Touch & Haptics. Touch & High Information Transfer Rate. Modern Haptics. Human. Haptics

Touch & Haptics. Touch & High Information Transfer Rate. Modern Haptics. Human. Haptics Touch & Haptics Touch & High Information Transfer Rate Blind and deaf people have been using touch to substitute vision or hearing for a very long time, and successfully. OPTACON Hong Z Tan Purdue University

More information

MGL Avionics Autopilot. Servo. Specifications & Installation Manual. Last Update: 20 October Disclaimer:

MGL Avionics Autopilot. Servo. Specifications & Installation Manual. Last Update: 20 October Disclaimer: MGL Avionics Autopilot Servo Specifications & Installation Manual Last Update: 20 October 2010 Disclaimer: MGL Avionics should not be held responsible for errors or omissions in this document. Usage of

More information

Feelable User Interfaces: An Exploration of Non-Visual Tangible User Interfaces

Feelable User Interfaces: An Exploration of Non-Visual Tangible User Interfaces Feelable User Interfaces: An Exploration of Non-Visual Tangible User Interfaces Katrin Wolf Telekom Innovation Laboratories TU Berlin, Germany katrin.wolf@acm.org Peter Bennett Interaction and Graphics

More information

SenseMaker IST Martin McGinnity University of Ulster Neuro-IT, Bonn, June 2004 SenseMaker IST Neuro-IT workshop June 2004 Page 1

SenseMaker IST Martin McGinnity University of Ulster Neuro-IT, Bonn, June 2004 SenseMaker IST Neuro-IT workshop June 2004 Page 1 SenseMaker IST2001-34712 Martin McGinnity University of Ulster Neuro-IT, Bonn, June 2004 Page 1 Project Objectives To design and implement an intelligent computational system, drawing inspiration from

More information

The Persistence of Vision in Spatio-Temporal Illusory Contours formed by Dynamically-Changing LED Arrays

The Persistence of Vision in Spatio-Temporal Illusory Contours formed by Dynamically-Changing LED Arrays The Persistence of Vision in Spatio-Temporal Illusory Contours formed by Dynamically-Changing LED Arrays Damian Gordon * and David Vernon Department of Computer Science Maynooth College Ireland ABSTRACT

More information

Perception of Curvature and Object Motion Via Contact Location Feedback

Perception of Curvature and Object Motion Via Contact Location Feedback Perception of Curvature and Object Motion Via Contact Location Feedback William R. Provancher, Katherine J. Kuchenbecker, Günter Niemeyer, and Mark R. Cutkosky Stanford University Dexterous Manipulation

More information

2. Introduction to Computer Haptics

2. Introduction to Computer Haptics 2. Introduction to Computer Haptics Seungmoon Choi, Ph.D. Assistant Professor Dept. of Computer Science and Engineering POSTECH Outline Basics of Force-Feedback Haptic Interfaces Introduction to Computer

More information