FACE-SPECIFIC RESPONSES FROM THE HUMAN INFERIOR OCCIPITO-TEMPORAL CORTEX

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "FACE-SPECIFIC RESPONSES FROM THE HUMAN INFERIOR OCCIPITO-TEMPORAL CORTEX"

Transcription

1 Pergamon Neuroscience Vol. 77, No. 1, pp , 1997 Copyright 1997 IBRO. Published by Elsevier Science Ltd Printed in Great Britain PII: S (96) /97 $ FACE-SPECIFIC RESPONSES FROM THE HUMAN INFERIOR OCCIPITO-TEMPORAL CORTEX M. SAMS,* J. K. HIETANEN, R. HARI,* R. J. ILMONIEMI* and O. V. LOUNASMAA* *Low Temperature Laboratory, Helsinki University of Technology, Espoo, Finland Institute of Biomedicine, Department of Physiology, P.O. Box 9, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland Abstract Whole-head neuromagnetic responses were recorded from seven subjects to pictures of faces and to various control stimuli. Four subjects displayed signals specific to faces. The combination of functional information from magnetoencephalography and anatomical data from magnetic resonance images suggests that the face-specific activity was generated in the inferior occipitotemporal cortex. All four subjects showed the face-specific response in the right hemisphere, one of them also in the left. Our results, together with recent positron emission tomography and lesion studies, suggest a right-hemisphere preponderance of face processing in the inferior occipitotemporal cortex. Copyright 1997 IBRO. Published by Elsevier Science Ltd. Key words: magnetoencephalography, face, visual cortex, evoked responses, perception, human. Processing of facial images has been studied extensively during the past few years. Neuropsychological models have emphasized the unique nature and sequence of different stages in this process (e.g., Ref. 9), while neurophysiological recordings have provided evidence for the existence of localized brain areas related to facial perception and recognition. 17,21,39 This multidisciplinary approach is hoped to be successful in constructing anatomo-functional models of cerebral face processing. Many different experimental methods have been employed: measurements of performance and reaction time, 8,13,42,43 tachistoscopic presentation of face stimuli to divided visual fields, 20,25,36 investigations of symptoms in patients having (prosopagnostic) difficulties in perceiving and recognizing faces after local brain injuries, 11,12,28 measurements of visual evoked potentials 6,21,22,23 and magnetic fields 27 to face stimuli, and measurements of regional cerebral blood flow with positron emission tomography (PET) during facial image processing. 39 In addition, single-unit recordings on monkeys have revealed cell populations in the temporal cortex responding selectively to faces. 14,19,32,33 To whom correspondence should be addressed. Present address: Department of Psychology, University of Tampere, Tampere, Finland. Present address: BioMag Laboratory, Medical Engineering Centre, Helsinki University Central Hospital, Helsinki, Finland. Abbreviations: ECD, equivalent current dipole; EEG, electroencephalography; MEG, magnetoencephalography; MRI, magnetic resonance imaging; PET, positron emission tomography. 49 Magnetoencephalography (MEG), a method for studying the neural basis of cognitive brain functions non-invasively, is based on recordings of the weak magnetic fields produced by electric currents flowing in active neurons (for a comprehensive review of the method, see Ref. 16). MEG differs from electroencephalography (EEG) in its selectivity to superficial, tangentially oriented neural currents. This means that MEG sees mainly the fissural cortex and, therefore, less neuronal activity than EEG. MEG offers a temporal resolution in the millisecond range which makes it useful in studying the timecourse of stimulus-evoked brain responses. In addition, because the intervening cerebral and extracerebral tissues are practically transparent to magnetic fields, whereas electric recordings are often smeared by inhomogeneities in them, it is often easier to calculate the locus of the activated brain area on the basis of MEG rather than EEG recordings. 16 In a previous study from this laboratory, 27 MEG was applied to identify face-responsive brain areas. Three sites outside the occipital visual cortex were observed to become active, two of them exclusively by viewing faces: an area near the occipitotemporal junction and another in the mid-temporal lobe. In this early study, MEG signals were recorded with a 24-channel magnetometer that covered an area 12.5 cm in diameter. We decided to extend and confirm the previous results with our 122-channel neuromagnetometer. This device covers the whole scalp and thus offers the possibility of studying interand intrahemispheric differences in the magnetic fields during a single recording session.

2 50 M. Sams et al. Fig. 1. Examples of pictures used in the experiment. From left to right: a face, the same face pointillized, an everyday object and a sphere. Fig. 2. Whole-head 122-channel neuromagnetic responses to faces (continuous lines) and pointillized faces from subject S3. The head is viewed form the top (the nose is pointing up), and in each response pair the upper trace illustrates the field derivative along the latitude and the lower trace along the longitude. The face-specific response is best seen in the channel pair over the right occipital cortex (framed). The thin vertical line is drawn at the peak of the face-specific response. (Insert) The channel pair showing the face-specific response is depicted on an expanded scale in the center. The occipital dipolar magnetic field pattern at the peak of the face-specific response is illustrated in the upper part; the center of the white arrow indicates the approximate location of the source for this response; the head is viewed from the right and slightly behind. The isocontours are separated by 30 ft. The lower part shows the field pattern at the same latency for pointillized faces. In this case there is very weak indication of activity in the right visual cortex. EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURES Seven subjects with normal vision were studied (one left-handed, three females, four males, years old). During the recordings, the subject sat in a magnetically shielded room with the head supported against the helmetshaped magnetometer. Four different stimulus categories were presented (see Fig. 1): (i) 23 faces of persons working in the laboratory; (ii) pointillized (Adobe Photoshop ) versions of these 23 faces; the altered figures did not look like faces at all, but their mean luminance was preserved; (iii) 23 images of everyday objects such as a watering can, a watch, scissors etc.; and (iv) a white sphere on a grey background. All

3 Face-specific response 51 stimulus categories were equiprobable (P=0.25) and they were presented in random order within one sequence. The subjects were instructed to fixate on a cross in the center of the image, avoid eye movements during stimuli and concentrate on looking at the pictures without any other specific task. The pictures, 3.1 in height and 2.6 in width, were presented against grey background for 500 ms, once per second, on a computer screen located outside the darkened magnetically shielded room. The subjects viewed them through a hole via a mirror system. The magnetic brain signals were recorded with the Neuromag-122 system, 1 which has 122 planar first-order SQUID (Superconducting QUantum Interference Device) gradiometers covering the whole head. Each sensor block contains a pair of independent units that measure two orthogonal tangential derivatives of the magnetic field component B z, normal to the helmet surface at the sensor location. With this instrument, brain activity produces the largest signal just above the source, where the field gradient has its maximum. The two derivatives measured at each site also give the direction of the source current. The signals were analogically bandpass-filtered ( Hz), digitized at 0.4 khz and averaged on-line. The 900-ms analysis period included a 100-ms prestimulus baseline. A minimum of 120 responses were averaged for each stimulus category. To check the replicability of the data, the experiment was performed twice for each subject. A vertical electro-oculogram was used to reject data contaminated by eye movements or blinks. Three marker coils were attached to the scalp, and their positions in the head coordinate frame were measured with a three-dimensional digitizer. The coordinate system was specified by the two periauricular points and the nasion. The head position with respect to the sensor array was then determined by feeding current to the marker coils when the subject was seated under the magnetometer. To identify neural sources underlying the evoked magnetic responses, equivalent current dipoles (ECDs), each best describing the measured field pattern at a given time instant, were extracted by a least-squares search. After digital low-pass filtering at 30 Hz, single ECDs were found using a subset of channels around the one showing the largest signal. The goodness-of-fit (g) of the source model was also calculated. 24 Only ECDs explaining more than 80% of the field variance during the face-specific response were accepted. In one subject (S3), the ECD locations were related to brain anatomy obtained from magnetic resonance images. The magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) coordinate system was aligned with the help of oil capsules attached to the nasion and to the two preauricular points before MRI acquisition. These marker locations were picked from the images. The volume conductor sphere was fitted on the basis of the magnetic resonance images to the local curvature of the surface of the brain over the source area. RESULTS Figure 2 shows the neuromagnetic responses of subject S3 to faces (continuous lines) and pointillized faces (dashed lines). The largest signals to both stimuli, as also to everyday objects and spheres, were recorded over the occipital cortex; the frontal and frontotemporal areas showed only negligible responses. Responses to faces and pointillized faces differed systematically over the right occipital cortex (framed channel pair and inset) from 120 ms onwards; the difference was largest around 160 ms. The occipital magnetic field pattern at the peak of the face Fig. 3. Neuromagnetic signals of four subjects over the posterior part of the left and right hemispheres. Traces from two consecutive recordings are superimposed. The responses to face stimuli are presented with thick continuous lines. The thinner continuous curves indicate responses to pointillized faces, the thinnest short dashed lines responses to everyday objects and longer dashed lines responses to spheres. response is shown in the upper part of the inset. The arrow depicts the surface projection of the ECD. The field pattern drawn at the same latency for the pointillized faces (lower part of the inset) suggested very weak activity over the occipital cortex. Responses to the two stimulus categories also differed at the posterior channels (Fig. 2). It is suggested that this difference reflects the differential processing of simple visual features. Response differences were also found between faces and other control stimuli. These differences were not, however, systematic but were different for various control stimulus categories. Of the seven subjects studied, four showed clear and replicable face-specific responses. The responses from these subjects are depicted in Fig. 3. Subject S1, who is right handed, had a face-specific response over both hemispheres but the signal was clearly larger over the left hemisphere. The other three subjects produced a face-specific signal only over the right hemisphere; the responses peaked at ms. The other stimulus categories elicited only small signals at about the same latency. Note the good replicability of the face-specific responses. Figure 4 (upper part) shows the source location for the face-specific response of subject S3, projected on her MRI surface rendering. This ECD, pointing towards the occipitoparietal area, accounted for 84% of the field variance. MRI slices cut through the source are illustrated in the bottom row. The Talairach 41 coordinates of the source are 9-c-G, agreeing with the location of the fusiform gyrus.

4 52 M. Sams et al. Fig. 4. (Top, from left to right) Right, back and top views of MRI surface renderings of subject S3. The source area (white dot) of the face-specific response is projected on to the surface of the brain along the viewing direction. (Bottom) MRI slices at the source level of the face-specific response. The line in the sagittal section shows the direction of the ECD. Figure 5 illustrates the calculated source locations and directions for the face-specific responses for subjects S1 S4. The corresponding goodness-of-fits were 89, 87, 84 and 83%. The right-hemisphere response of subject S1 (Fig. 3) was not adequately explained by a single ECD. In all subjects, ECDs point towards the centroparietal or occipitoparietal scalp. The sources were in the occipital regions, but clearly outside the primary projection areas. We did not find face-specific responses in more anterior parts of the brain. Faces, pointillized faces (Fig. 2) and the other picture categories elicited many other MEG deflections at ms. These responses were predominantly recorded over the back of the head, evidently arising from occipitoparietal areas. Because the fixation point was at the center of the images, stimuli activated the visual cortex in a very complex way. Therefore, it was not possible to determine the location of the ECDs for these different signals, which certainly reflect the mixed activation of several cytoarchitectonic areas. DISCUSSION The present study reports replicable face-specific neuromagnetic responses in four of seven normal human subjects. The combination of functional information from MEG and anatomical data from MRI suggests that the face-specific activity is generated in the inferior occipitotemporal cortex. Very similar responses occurring at approximately the same latency have been reported in intracranial recordings, 3 evoked-potential recordings 21 and in a separate MEG experiment conducted in our laboratory. 15 The source location of the response agrees with that found in other recent studies. 3,10,15,18,34,39 Face-specific responses peaked at ms, in accordance with evoked potential data 21 and subdural recordings. 3 Single units in the temporal lobe of the macaque monkey (STPa) reach their maximum firing rate on average 180 ms after face stimuli, 30 whereas the responses from V1 have a mean latency of about 50 ms. 37 The first human MEG responses, probably originating from the striatal and extra-

5 Face-specific response 53 Fig. 5. Horizontal (xy), sagittal (yz) and coronal (xz) views illustrating the sources of face-specific responses in four subjects. The schematic head (radius=7 cm) is shown to give an impression of the approximate source locations in the brain. striatal areas, peak at ms. 2 The rather long peak latency and the source location suggest that the face-specific signal reflects activation of brain mechanisms involved in complex visuoperceptual processing of the facial image. The source locations of the face-specific responses varied across the subjects. The common feature was that all ECDs point towards the centroparietal or occipitoparietal scalp and that the sources were in the posterior brain regions, outside the primary projection areas. In S3, whose magnetic resonance images were available, the ECD was located in the area of the fusiform gyrus, in the occipitotemporal sulcus. However, as suggested by previous studies, rather extensive areas in the gyral and sulcal cortices are activated by face stimuli. 34 When this activity is modeled by a single dipole, its location denotes a center of gravity of the activated area. In the other subjects, the right-hemisphere sources were more superior and located rather anteriorly (S4) or posteriorly (S2). This variability might be due to individual differences in the locations of the facespecific cortical areas. 3 Additional variability might be caused by simultaneous activity of more than one adjacent face-specific area, whose relative contributions might vary (cf. Ref. 39). Meaningful control stimuli, comparable in their complexity to faces, typically elicit smaller and later electrical evoked responses than faces Similarly, our control stimuli weakly activated the right occipital cortex at ms (Fig. 2, Fig. 3). These findings suggest that even non-face stimuli can activate the face neurons, although less intensively. However, recent subdural recordings 3 imply that the inferior occipital cortex contains areas which are very specific to faces and that other stimulus types may elicit responses at the same latency, but at different cortical locations. One possible explanation for the absence of facespecific responses in three of our seven subjects is source orientation. If the source is close to radial, the signal cannot be recorded by MEG. In addition, face-specific neurons may be located rather deeply, 3 resulting in signals too weak to be picked up. Jeffreys 21 identified an evoked potential to faces in eight of nine subjects, but because the responses were small, the conclusions were based on recordings on only three subjects. Jeffreys and Tukmachi 23 screened each new volunteer in order to obtain subjects with distinct vertex positive peaks, which they have demonstrated to be sensitive to faces. A previous face study from this laboratory 27 suggested two face-specific areas, one near the occipitotemporal junction and another in the middle temporal lobe. The response in the occipitotemporal junction peaked at ms and was on average 38 mm beneath the skull. The signal is probably analogous to the face-specific response of the present study, which actually suggests an origin in the inferior occipitotemporal cortex. The present study did not reveal face-specific responses in the midtemporal areas. Neuronal activity in the middle temporal gyrus may be modulated during identification of facial emotions. 29 It is possible that the subjects in our previous study paid attention to the emotionality of the faces. The polarity of the face-specific response in subdural recordings 3 suggests source currents oriented towards the parietal lobe and vertex. A similar direction was obtained for the present ECDs. This kind of current source appears as a scalp positivity close to the vertex, as was found in EEG recordings Lateralization of functions involved in face processing is still a controversial issue. 4,5 In subdural recordings, Allison et al. 3 found face-specific signals from the fusiform and inferior temporal gyri of both

6 54 M. Sams et al. hemispheres. Similarly, cortical volumes activated by face stimuli in a recent functional MRI study were not different in the two hemispheres. 34 On the other hand, prosopagnostic patients studied recently with MRI and PET showed no involvement of the left hemisphere. 40 Sergent et al. 39 suggested that three areas of the right hemisphere play a crucial role in face processing: (i) the ventral part of the right occipital cortex is involved in perceptual operations, (ii) the more anterior ventral regions of the right hemisphere relate perceptual information to stored biographical memories, and (iii) the anterior temporal cortex of both hemispheres acts as a depository of biographical memories. The authors emphasized the role of the right hemisphere in face recognition and considered the role of the left hemisphere as complementary. In the present study, three subjects demonstrating the face-specific response showed it unilaterally in the right hemisphere and in only one was the signal bilateral. Our subjects were looking at the faces with no particular task. In studies reporting bilateral activation during face recognition, the subjects have performed some type of stimulus discrimination. 3,17,39 We suggest that the present type of passive viewing task favors the global, configurational processing of facial features, suggested to be a function of the right hemisphere. 7,26,31,35,38 Our conclusion is that the present results, together with lesion data and PET studies, suggest a dominance of the right inferior temporal cortex in global visuoperceptual face processing. Acknowledgements This study was supported by the Academy of Finland. We thank Veikko Jousmäki and Satu Tissari for help in the analysis of MRI data. REFERENCES 1. Ahonen A. I., Hämäläinen M. S., Kajola M. J., Knuutila J. E. T., Laine P. P., Lounasmaa O. V., Parkkonen L. T., Simola J. T. and Tesche C. D. (1993) 122-channel SQUID instrument for investigating the magnetic signals from the human brain. Phys. Scripta T49, Aine C., Bodis-Wollner I. and George J. (1990) Generators of visually evoked neuromagnetic responses. Spatialfrequency segregation and evidence for multiple sources. In Advances in Neurology (ed. Sato S.), pp Raven, New York. 3. Allison T., Ginter H., McCarthy G., Norbe A., Puce A., Luby M. and Spencer D. (1994) Face recognition in human extrastriate cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 71, Benton A. L. (1985) Facial recognition In Studies in Neuropsychology: Selected Papers of Arthur Benton (eds Costa L. and Spreen O.), pp Oxford University Press, New York. 5. Benton A. L. (1990) Facial recognition Cortex 26, Bötzel K. and Grüsser O.-J. (1987) Potentials evoked by face and nonface stimuli in the human electroencephalogram. Perception 16, A Bradshaw J. L. and Sherlock D. (1982) Bugs and faces in the two visual fields: the analytic/holistic processing dichotomy and task sequencing. Cortex 18, Bruce V., Doyle A., Dench N. and Burton M. (1991) Remembering facial configurations. Cognition 38, Bruce V. and Young A. (1986) Understanding face recognition. Br. J. Psychol. 77, Clark V., Parasuraman R., Keil K., Maisog J., Ungerleider L. and Haxby J. (1995) FMRI studies of attention to color and face identity. Human Brain Mapping Suppl. 1, De Haan E. H. F., Young A. W. and Newcombe F. (1992) Neuropsychological impairment of face recognition units. Q. Jl exp. Psychol. 44, De Renzi E., Faglioni P., Grossi D. and Nichelli P. (1991) Apperceptive and associative forms of prosopagnosia. Cortex 27, Ellis A. W., Young A. W. and Flude B. M. (1990) Repetition priming and face processing: priming occurs within the system that responds to the identity of a face. Q. Jl exp. Psychol. 39A, Gross C. G. (1992) Representation of visual stimuli in inferior temporal cortex. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 335, Halgren E., Raij T., Marinkovic K., Jousmäki V. and Hari R. (1995) Magnetic fields evoked by faces in the human brain: 1. Topography and source locations. Soc. Neurosci. Abstr. 211, Hämäläinen M., Hari R., Ilmoniemi R. J., Knuutila J. and Lounasmaa O. V. (1993) Magnetoencephalography theory, instrumentation, and applications to noninvasive studies of the working human brain. Rev. mod. Phys. 65, Haxby J. V., Grady C. L., Horowitz B., Salerno J., Ungerleider L. G., Mishkin M. and Schapiro M. B. (1993) Dissociation of object and spatial visual processing pathways in human extrastriate cortex. In Functional Organization of the Human Visual Cortex (eds Gulyas B., Ottoson D. and Roland P. E.), pp Pergamon, Oxford. 18. Haxby J., Ungleider L., Horwitz B., Rapoport S. and Grady C. (1995) Hemispheric differences in neural systems for face working memory: a PET rcbf study. Human Brain Mapping 3, Hietanen J. K., Perrett D. I., Oram M. W., Benson P. J. and Dittrich W. H. (1992) The effects of lighting conditions on responses of cells selective for face views in the macaque temporal cortex. Expl Brain Res. 89, Hilliard R. D. (1973) Hemispheric laterality effects on a facial recognition task in normal subjects. Cortex 9, Jeffreys D. A. (1989) A face-responsive potential recorded from the human scalp. Expl Brain Res. 78, Jeffreys D. A. (1993) The influence of stimulus orientation on the vertex positive scalp potential evoked by faces. Expl Brain Res. 96, Jeffreys D. A. and Tukmachi E. S. A. (1992) The vertex-positive scalp potential evoked by faces and by objects. Expl Brain Res. 91,

7 Face-specific response Kaukoranta E., Hari R., Hämäläinen M. and Huttunen J. (1986) Cerebral magnetic fields evoked by peroneal nerve stimulation. Somatosensory Res. 3, Klein D., Moscovitch M. and Vigna C. (1976) Attentional mechanisms and perceptual asymmetries in tachistoscopic recognition of words and faces. Neuropsychology 14, Levy J., Trevarthen C. and Sperry R. W. (1972) Perception of bilateral chimeric figures following hemispheric deconnexion. Brain 95, Lu S. T., Hämäläinen M., Hari R., Ilmoniemi R., Lounasmaa O. V., Sams M. and Vilkman V. (1991) Seeing faces activates three separate areas outside the occipital visual cortex in man. Neuroscience 43, Newcombe F., Young A. W. and de Haan E. H. F. (1989) Prosopagnosia, and object agnosia without covert recognition. Neuropsychology 27, Ojemann J. G., Ojemann G. A. and Lettich E. (1992) Neuronal activity related to faces and matching in human right nondominant temporal cortex. Brain 115, Oram M. W. and Perrett D. I. (1992) Time course of neural responses discriminating different views of the face and head. J. Neurophysiol. 68, Parkin A. J. and Williamson P. (1987) Cerebral lateralization at different stages of facial processing. Cortex 23, Perrett D. I., Hietanen J. K., Oram M. W. and Benson P. J. (1992) Organization and function of cells responsive to faces in the temporal cortex. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 335, Perrett D. I., Oram M. W., Harries M. H., Bevan R., Hietanen J. K., Benson P. J. and Thomas S. (1991) Viewer-centred and object-centred encoding of heads by cells in the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesus monkey. Expl Brain Res. 86, Puce A., Allison T., Gore J. C. and McGarthy G. (1995) Face-sensitive regions in human extrastriate cortex studied by functional MRI. J. Neurophysiol. 74, Rapcsak S. Z., Polster M. R., Comer J. F. and Rubens A. B. (1994) False recognition and misidentification of faces following right hemisphere damage. Cortex 30, Rizzolatti G., Umiltà C. and Berlucchi G. (1971) Opposite superiorities of the right and left cerebral hemispheres in discriminative reaction time to physiognomical and alphabetical material. Brain 94, Robinson D. L. and Rugg M. D. (1988) Latencies of visually responsive neurons in various regions of the rhesus monkey brain and their relation to human visual responses. Biol. Psychol. 26, Sergent J. (1984) An investigation into component and configural processes underlying face perception. Br. J. Psychol. 75, Sergent J., Ohta S. and MacDonald B. (1992) Functional neuroanatomy of face and object processing. Brain 115, Sergent J. and Signoret J. L. (1991) Outstanding issues in the study of prosopagnosia. J. clin. exp. Neuropsychol. 13, Talairach J. and Tournoux P. (1988) Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain. George Thieme, Stuttgart. 42. Valentine T. (1991) A unified account of the effects of distinctiveness, inversion and race in face recognition. Q. J. exp. Psychol. 43A, Young A. W., Ellis A. W. and Flude B. M. (1988) Accessing stored information about familiar people. Psychol. Res. 50, (Accepted 16 July 1996)

Distributed representation of objects in the human ventral visual pathway (face perception functional MRI object recognition)

Distributed representation of objects in the human ventral visual pathway (face perception functional MRI object recognition) Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 96, pp. 9379 9384, August 1999 Neurobiology Distributed representation of objects in the human ventral visual pathway (face perception functional MRI object recognition)

More information

Cognitive Response Profile of the Human Fusiform Face Area as Determined by MEG

Cognitive Response Profile of the Human Fusiform Face Area as Determined by MEG Cognitive Response Profile of the Human Fusiform Face Area as Determined by MEG Eric Halgren 1,2, Tommi Raij 3, Ksenija Marinkovic 1,2, Veikko Jousmäki 3 and Riitta Hari 3 1 INSERM E9926, Marseilles, France,

More information

Holistic Processing of Faces: Learning Effects with Mooney Faces

Holistic Processing of Faces: Learning Effects with Mooney Faces Holistic Processing of Faces: Learning Effects with Mooney Faces Marianne Latinus and Margot J. Taylor* Abstract & The specialness of faces is seen in the face inversion effect, which disrupts the configural,

More information

HBM2006: MEG/EEG Brain mapping course MEG/EEG instrumentation and experiment design. Florence, June 11, 2006

HBM2006: MEG/EEG Brain mapping course MEG/EEG instrumentation and experiment design. Florence, June 11, 2006 HBM2006: MEG/EEG Brain mapping course MEG/EEG instrumentation and experiment design Florence, June 11, 2006 Lauri Parkkonen Brain Research Unit Low Temperature Laboratory Helsinki University lauri@neuro.hut.fi

More information

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007 AUDITORY EVOKED MAGNETIC FIELDS AND LOUDNESS IN RELATION TO BANDPASS NOISES

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007 AUDITORY EVOKED MAGNETIC FIELDS AND LOUDNESS IN RELATION TO BANDPASS NOISES 19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007 AUDITORY EVOKED MAGNETIC FIELDS AND LOUDNESS IN RELATION TO BANDPASS NOISES PACS: 43.64.Ri Yoshiharu Soeta; Seiji Nakagawa 1 National

More information

Prosopagnosia and structural encoding of faces: Evidence from event-related potentials

Prosopagnosia and structural encoding of faces: Evidence from event-related potentials Cognitive neuroscience 10, 255±259 (1999) EVENT-RELATED brain potentials (ERPs) were recorded in response to unfamiliar faces and to houses from a severely prosopagnosic patient (PHD) and 24 control subjects.

More information

Magnetoencephalography and Auditory Neural Representations

Magnetoencephalography and Auditory Neural Representations Magnetoencephalography and Auditory Neural Representations Jonathan Z. Simon Nai Ding Electrical & Computer Engineering, University of Maryland, College Park SBEC 2010 Non-invasive, Passive, Silent Neural

More information

The Effect of Face Inversion on Activity in Human Neural Systems for Face and Object Perception

The Effect of Face Inversion on Activity in Human Neural Systems for Face and Object Perception Neuron, Vol. 22, 189 199, January, 1999, Copyright 1999 by Cell Press The Effect of Face Inversion on Activity in Human Neural Systems for Face and Object Perception James V. Haxby,* Leslie G. Ungerleider,*

More information

Parvocellular layers (3-6) Magnocellular layers (1 & 2)

Parvocellular layers (3-6) Magnocellular layers (1 & 2) Parvocellular layers (3-6) Magnocellular layers (1 & 2) Dorsal and Ventral visual pathways Figure 4.15 The dorsal and ventral streams in the cortex originate with the magno and parvo ganglion cells and

More information

Normal and abnormal face selectivity of the M170 response in developmental prosopagnosics

Normal and abnormal face selectivity of the M170 response in developmental prosopagnosics Neuropsychologia 43 (2005) 2125 2136 Normal and abnormal face selectivity of the M170 response in developmental prosopagnosics Alison M. Harris, Bradley C. Duchaine, Ken Nakayama Vision Science Laboratory,

More information

KYMATA DATASET 3.01: README

KYMATA DATASET 3.01: README KYMATA DATASET 3.01: README Kymata s information processing pathways are generated from electromagnetic measurements of the human cortex. These raw measurements are available for download from https://kymata-atlas.org/datasets.

More information

Electrophysiological Studies of Human Face Perception. I: Potentials Generated in Occipitotemporal Cortex by Face and Non-face Stimuli

Electrophysiological Studies of Human Face Perception. I: Potentials Generated in Occipitotemporal Cortex by Face and Non-face Stimuli Electrophysiological Studies of Human Face Perception. I: Potentials Generated in Occipitotemporal Cortex by Face and Non-face Stimuli This and the following two papers describe event-related potentials

More information

Chapter 8: Perceiving Motion

Chapter 8: Perceiving Motion Chapter 8: Perceiving Motion Motion perception occurs (a) when a stationary observer perceives moving stimuli, such as this couple crossing the street; and (b) when a moving observer, like this basketball

More information

PHYSICAL METHODS, INSTRUMENTS AND MEASUREMENTS Vol. IV - SQUIDs in Neuro-and Cardiomagnetism - Olli V. Lounasmaa, Heikki Seppä

PHYSICAL METHODS, INSTRUMENTS AND MEASUREMENTS Vol. IV - SQUIDs in Neuro-and Cardiomagnetism - Olli V. Lounasmaa, Heikki Seppä SQUIDS IN NEURO- AND CARDIOMAGNETISM Olli V. Lounasmaa Low Temperature Laboratory, Helsinki University of Technology, 02015 HUT, Finland Heikki Seppä Measurement Technology, VTT Automation, 02015 VTT,

More information

Structural Encoding of Human and Schematic Faces: Holistic and Part-Based Processes

Structural Encoding of Human and Schematic Faces: Holistic and Part-Based Processes Structural Encoding of Human and Schematic Faces: Holistic and Part-Based Processes Noam Sagiv 1 and Shlomo Bentin Abstract & The range of specificity and the response properties of the extrastriate face

More information

Face Perception. The Thatcher Illusion. The Thatcher Illusion. Can you recognize these upside-down faces? The Face Inversion Effect

Face Perception. The Thatcher Illusion. The Thatcher Illusion. Can you recognize these upside-down faces? The Face Inversion Effect The Thatcher Illusion Face Perception Did you notice anything odd about the upside-down image of Margaret Thatcher that you saw before? Can you recognize these upside-down faces? The Thatcher Illusion

More information

Image-Invariant Responses in Face-Selective Regions Do Not Explain the Perceptual Advantage for Familiar Face Recognition

Image-Invariant Responses in Face-Selective Regions Do Not Explain the Perceptual Advantage for Familiar Face Recognition Cerebral Cortex February 2013;23:370 377 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhs024 Advance Access publication February 17, 2012 Image-Invariant Responses in Face-Selective Regions Do Not Explain the Perceptual Advantage

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION a b STS IOS IOS STS c "#$"% "%' STS posterior IOS dorsal anterior ventral d "( "& )* e f "( "#$"% "%' "& )* Supplementary Figure 1. Retinotopic mapping of the non-lesioned hemisphere. a. Inflated 3D representation

More information

Human Extrastriate Visual Cortex and the Perception of Faces, Words, Numbers, and Colors

Human Extrastriate Visual Cortex and the Perception of Faces, Words, Numbers, and Colors Human Extrastriate Visual Cortex and the Perception of Faces, Words, Numbers, and Colors Truett Allison, Gregory McCarthy, Anna Nobre, Aim Puce, and Aysenil Belger Neuropsychology Laboratory, VA Medical

More information

Tilburg University. Haptic face recognition and prosopagnosia Kilgour, A.R.; de Gelder, Bea; Bertelson, P. Published in: Neuropsychologia

Tilburg University. Haptic face recognition and prosopagnosia Kilgour, A.R.; de Gelder, Bea; Bertelson, P. Published in: Neuropsychologia Tilburg University Haptic face recognition and prosopagnosia Kilgour, A.R.; de Gelder, Bea; Bertelson, P. Published in: Neuropsychologia Publication date: 2004 Link to publication Citation for published

More information

Mapping face categorization in the human ventral occipitotemporal cortex with direct neural intracranial recordings

Mapping face categorization in the human ventral occipitotemporal cortex with direct neural intracranial recordings Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. ISSN 0077-8923 ANNALS OF THE NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES Issue: The Year in Cognitive Neuroscience REVIEW Mapping face categorization in the human ventral occipitotemporal cortex

More information

Supplementary Figure 1

Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1 Left aspl Right aspl Detailed description of the fmri activation during allocentric action observation in the aspl. Averaged activation (N=13) during observation of the allocentric

More information

Topographical Analysis of Motion-Triggered Visual-Evoked Potentials in Man

Topographical Analysis of Motion-Triggered Visual-Evoked Potentials in Man Topographical Analysis of Motion-Triggered Visual-Evoked Potentials in Man Yasushi Nakamura and Kenji Ohtsuka Department of Ophthalmology, School of Medicine, Sapporo Medical University, Sapporo, Japan

More information

Neural Coding of Multiple Stimulus Features in Auditory Cortex

Neural Coding of Multiple Stimulus Features in Auditory Cortex Neural Coding of Multiple Stimulus Features in Auditory Cortex Jonathan Z. Simon Neuroscience and Cognitive Sciences Biology / Electrical & Computer Engineering University of Maryland, College Park Computational

More information

Rapid Face-Selective Adaptation of an Early Extrastriate Component in MEG

Rapid Face-Selective Adaptation of an Early Extrastriate Component in MEG Cerebral Cortex January 2007;17:63--70 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj124 Advance Access publication January 25, 2006 Rapid Face-Selective Adaptation of an Early Extrastriate Component in MEG Alison Harris and

More information

Stimulus-dependent position sensitivity in human ventral temporal cortex

Stimulus-dependent position sensitivity in human ventral temporal cortex Stimulus-dependent position sensitivity in human ventral temporal cortex Rory Sayres 1, Kevin S. Weiner 1, Brian Wandell 1,2, and Kalanit Grill-Spector 1,2 1 Psychology Department, Stanford University,

More information

The Physiology of the Senses Lecture 3: Visual Perception of Objects

The Physiology of the Senses Lecture 3: Visual Perception of Objects The Physiology of the Senses Lecture 3: Visual Perception of Objects www.tutis.ca/senses/ Contents Objectives... 2 What is after V1?... 2 Assembling Simple Features into Objects... 4 Illusory Contours...

More information

A Three-Channel Model for Generating the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex in Each Eye

A Three-Channel Model for Generating the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex in Each Eye A Three-Channel Model for Generating the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex in Each Eye LAURENCE R. HARRIS, a KARL A. BEYKIRCH, b AND MICHAEL FETTER c a Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, Canada

More information

The fusiform face area is not sufficient for face recognition: Evidence from a patient with dense prosopagnosia and no occipital face area

The fusiform face area is not sufficient for face recognition: Evidence from a patient with dense prosopagnosia and no occipital face area Neuropsychologia 44 (2006) 594 609 The fusiform face area is not sufficient for face recognition: Evidence from a patient with dense prosopagnosia and no occipital face area Jennifer K.E. Steeves a,, Jody

More information

Selective visual streaming in face recognition: evidence from developmental prosopagnosia

Selective visual streaming in face recognition: evidence from developmental prosopagnosia Cognitive Neuroscience 10, 823±827 (1999) COMPUTATIONAL considerations suggest that ef cient face identi cation requires the categorization and exclusive streaming of previously encoded face visual primitives

More information

Development of a Time-Restricted Region-Suppressed ER-SAM Beamformer and its Application to an Auditory Evoked Field Study

Development of a Time-Restricted Region-Suppressed ER-SAM Beamformer and its Application to an Auditory Evoked Field Study Development of a Time-Restricted Region-Suppressed ER-SAM Beamformer and its Application to an Auditory Evoked Field Study by Daniel Davis Eugene Wong A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements

More information

Chapter 73. Two-Stroke Apparent Motion. George Mather

Chapter 73. Two-Stroke Apparent Motion. George Mather Chapter 73 Two-Stroke Apparent Motion George Mather The Effect One hundred years ago, the Gestalt psychologist Max Wertheimer published the first detailed study of the apparent visual movement seen when

More information

The visual and oculomotor systems. Peter H. Schiller, year The visual cortex

The visual and oculomotor systems. Peter H. Schiller, year The visual cortex The visual and oculomotor systems Peter H. Schiller, year 2006 The visual cortex V1 Anatomical Layout Monkey brain central sulcus Central Sulcus V1 Principalis principalis Arcuate Lunate lunate Figure

More information

Invariant Object Recognition in the Visual System with Novel Views of 3D Objects

Invariant Object Recognition in the Visual System with Novel Views of 3D Objects LETTER Communicated by Marian Stewart-Bartlett Invariant Object Recognition in the Visual System with Novel Views of 3D Objects Simon M. Stringer simon.stringer@psy.ox.ac.uk Edmund T. Rolls Edmund.Rolls@psy.ox.ac.uk,

More information

from signals to sources asa-lab turnkey solution for ERP research

from signals to sources asa-lab turnkey solution for ERP research from signals to sources asa-lab turnkey solution for ERP research asa-lab : turnkey solution for ERP research Psychological research on the basis of event-related potentials is a key source of information

More information

Orientation-sensitivity to facial features explains the Thatcher illusion

Orientation-sensitivity to facial features explains the Thatcher illusion Journal of Vision (2014) 14(12):9, 1 10 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/14/12/9 1 Orientation-sensitivity to facial features explains the Thatcher illusion Department of Psychology and York Neuroimaging

More information

Lauri Parkkonen. Jyväskylä Summer School 2013

Lauri Parkkonen. Jyväskylä Summer School 2013 Jyväskylä Summer School 2013 COM7: Electromagnetic Signals from The Human Brain: Fundamentals and Analysis (TIEJ659) Pre-processing of MEG data Lauri Parkkonen Dept. Biomedical Engineering and Computational

More information

Vision V Perceiving Movement

Vision V Perceiving Movement Vision V Perceiving Movement Overview of Topics Chapter 8 in Goldstein (chp. 9 in 7th ed.) Movement is tied up with all other aspects of vision (colour, depth, shape perception...) Differentiating self-motion

More information

Domain-Specificity versus Expertise in Face Processing

Domain-Specificity versus Expertise in Face Processing Domain-Specificity versus Expertise in Face Processing Dan O Shea and Peter Combs 18 Feb 2008 COS 598B Prof. Fei Fei Li Inferotemporal Cortex and Object Vision Keiji Tanaka Annual Review of Neuroscience,

More information

Vision V Perceiving Movement

Vision V Perceiving Movement Vision V Perceiving Movement Overview of Topics Chapter 8 in Goldstein (chp. 9 in 7th ed.) Movement is tied up with all other aspects of vision (colour, depth, shape perception...) Differentiating self-motion

More information

Haptic study of three-dimensional objects activates extrastriate visual areas

Haptic study of three-dimensional objects activates extrastriate visual areas Neuropsychologia 40 (2002) 1706 1714 Haptic study of three-dimensional objects activates extrastriate visual areas Thomas W. James, G. Keith Humphrey, Joseph S. Gati, Philip Servos, Ravi S. Menon, Melvyn

More information

The parallel visual motion inputs into areas VI and V5 of human cerebral cortex

The parallel visual motion inputs into areas VI and V5 of human cerebral cortex Brain (1995), 118, 1375-1394 The parallel visual motion inputs into areas VI and V5 of human cerebral cortex D. H. ffytche 1, C. N. Guy 2 and S. Zeki 1 department of Anatomy, University College London

More information

PERCEIVING MOTION CHAPTER 8

PERCEIVING MOTION CHAPTER 8 Motion 1 Perception (PSY 4204) Christine L. Ruva, Ph.D. PERCEIVING MOTION CHAPTER 8 Overview of Questions Why do some animals freeze in place when they sense danger? How do films create movement from still

More information

Budapest University of Technology and Economics Department of Cognitive Science Psychology PhD School

Budapest University of Technology and Economics Department of Cognitive Science Psychology PhD School Budapest University of Technology and Economics Department of Cognitive Science Psychology PhD School Németh Kornél The possible subtypes of the developmental prosopagnosia in the light of the neuropsychological,

More information

The Representation of Parts and Wholes in Faceselective

The Representation of Parts and Wholes in Faceselective University of Pennsylvania ScholarlyCommons Cognitive Neuroscience Publications Center for Cognitive Neuroscience 5-2008 The Representation of Parts and Wholes in Faceselective Cortex Alison Harris University

More information

Chapter 3: Psychophysical studies of visual object recognition

Chapter 3: Psychophysical studies of visual object recognition BEWARE: These are preliminary notes. In the future, they will become part of a textbook on Visual Object Recognition. Chapter 3: Psychophysical studies of visual object recognition We want to understand

More information

Methods. Experimental Stimuli: We selected 24 animals, 24 tools, and 24

Methods. Experimental Stimuli: We selected 24 animals, 24 tools, and 24 Methods Experimental Stimuli: We selected 24 animals, 24 tools, and 24 nonmanipulable object concepts following the criteria described in a previous study. For each item, a black and white grayscale photo

More information

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Lecture 4: Data analysis I Lesson Title 1 Introduction 2 Structure and Function of the NS 3 Windows to the Brain 4 Data analysis 5 Data analysis II 6 Single neuron

More information

A Sequence of Object-Processing Stages Revealed by fmri in the Human Occipital Lobe

A Sequence of Object-Processing Stages Revealed by fmri in the Human Occipital Lobe Human Brain Mapping 6:316 328(1998) A Sequence of Object-Processing Stages Revealed by fmri in the Human Occipital Lobe Kalanit Grill-Spector, 1 Tammar Kushnir, 2 Talma Hendler, 2 Shimon Edelman, 3 Yacov

More information

A Cortical Network for Face Perception

A Cortical Network for Face Perception A Cortical Network for Face Perception Alumit Ishai Institute of Neuroradiology, University of Zurich, Switzerland Address for correspondence: Alumit Ishai, PhD Professor of Cognitive Neuroscience University

More information

- Faces - A Special Problem of Object Recognition

- Faces - A Special Problem of Object Recognition - Faces - A Special Problem of Object Recognition Lesson II: Perception module 10 Perception.10. 1 Why are faces interesting? A face provides some of the most important cues about someone s identity Facial

More information

Seeing face-like objects: an event-related potential study Owen Churches a,b, Simon Baron-Cohen a and Howard Ring b

Seeing face-like objects: an event-related potential study Owen Churches a,b, Simon Baron-Cohen a and Howard Ring b Cognitive neuroscience and neuropsychology 1 Seeing face-like objects: an event-related potential study Owen Churches a,b, Simon Baron-Cohen a and Howard Ring b The N17 event-related potential component

More information

Lecture 4 Foundations and Cognitive Processes in Visual Perception From the Retina to the Visual Cortex

Lecture 4 Foundations and Cognitive Processes in Visual Perception From the Retina to the Visual Cortex Lecture 4 Foundations and Cognitive Processes in Visual Perception From the Retina to the Visual Cortex 1.Vision Science 2.Visual Performance 3.The Human Visual System 4.The Retina 5.The Visual Field and

More information

Let s face it: It s a cortical network

Let s face it: It s a cortical network Target Article Let s face it: It s a cortical network www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg NeuroImage 40 (2008) 415 419 Alumit Ishai Institute of Neuroradiology, University of Zurich, Winterthurerstrasse 190,

More information

A Revised Neural Framework for Face Processing

A Revised Neural Framework for Face Processing ANNUAL REVIEWS Further Click here to view this article's online features: Download figures as PPT slides Navigate linked references Download citations Explore related articles Search keywords A Revised

More information

GROUPING BASED ON PHENOMENAL PROXIMITY

GROUPING BASED ON PHENOMENAL PROXIMITY Journal of Experimental Psychology 1964, Vol. 67, No. 6, 531-538 GROUPING BASED ON PHENOMENAL PROXIMITY IRVIN ROCK AND LEONARD BROSGOLE l Yeshiva University The question was raised whether the Gestalt

More information

Bodies are Represented as Wholes Rather Than Their Sum of Parts in the Occipital-Temporal Cortex

Bodies are Represented as Wholes Rather Than Their Sum of Parts in the Occipital-Temporal Cortex Cerebral Cortex February 2016;26:530 543 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhu205 Advance Access publication September 12, 2014 Bodies are Represented as Wholes Rather Than Their Sum of Parts in the Occipital-Temporal

More information

Object Perception. 23 August PSY Object & Scene 1

Object Perception. 23 August PSY Object & Scene 1 Object Perception Perceiving an object involves many cognitive processes, including recognition (memory), attention, learning, expertise. The first step is feature extraction, the second is feature grouping

More information

Neural representations of faces and limbs neighbor in human high-level visual cortex: evidence for a new organization principle

Neural representations of faces and limbs neighbor in human high-level visual cortex: evidence for a new organization principle Psychological Research (2013) 77:74 97 DOI 10.1007/s00426-011-0392-x REVIEW Neural representations of faces and limbs neighbor in human high-level visual cortex: evidence for a new organization principle

More information

Processing streams PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 10. Neurophysiology

Processing streams PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 10. Neurophysiology Processing streams PSY 310 Greg Francis Lecture 10 A continuous surface infolded on itself. Neurophysiology We are working under the following hypothesis What we see is determined by the pattern of neural

More information

Title Observation of a Mirror Reflection. original author and source are cred

Title Observation of a Mirror Reflection.   original author and source are cred Title Asymmetric Activation of the Primar Observation of a Mirror Reflection Tominaga, Wataru; Matsubayashi, Jun Author(s) Matsuhashi, Masao; Mima, Tatsuya; F Mitani, Akira Citation PloS one (2011), 6(11)

More information

Faces and objects in macaque cerebral cortex

Faces and objects in macaque cerebral cortex Faces and objects in macaque cerebral cortex Doris Y Tsao 1,2,Winrich A Freiwald 3 5,Tamara A Knutsen 1,Joseph B Mandeville 1 & Roger B H Tootell 1,6 How are different object categories organized by the

More information

Lecture 5. The Visual Cortex. Cortical Visual Processing

Lecture 5. The Visual Cortex. Cortical Visual Processing Lecture 5 The Visual Cortex Cortical Visual Processing 1 Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN) LGN is located in the Thalamus There are two LGN on each (lateral) side of the brain. Optic nerve fibers from eye

More information

SQUID - Superconducting QUantum Interference Device. Introduction History Operation Applications

SQUID - Superconducting QUantum Interference Device. Introduction History Operation Applications SQUID - Superconducting QUantum Interference Device Introduction History Operation Applications Introduction Very sensitive magnetometer Superconducting quantum interference device based on quantum effects

More information

Brain and Art. Guiomar Niso. December 15, Guiomar Niso C3GI 2017

Brain and Art. Guiomar Niso. December 15, Guiomar Niso C3GI 2017 Brain and Art Guiomar Niso December 15, 2017 Guiomar Niso C3GI 2017 Santiago Ramón y Cajal Guiomar Niso C3GI 2017 2 Santiago Ramón y Cajal Premio Nobel 1906 Guiomar Niso C3GI 2017 3 Human Brain In the

More information

BME 3113, Dept. of BME Lecture on Introduction to Biosignal Processing

BME 3113, Dept. of BME Lecture on Introduction to Biosignal Processing What is a signal? A signal is a varying quantity whose value can be measured and which conveys information. A signal can be simply defined as a function that conveys information. Signals are represented

More information

Multi-channel SQUID-based Ultra-Low Field Magnetic Resonance Imaging in Unshielded Environment

Multi-channel SQUID-based Ultra-Low Field Magnetic Resonance Imaging in Unshielded Environment Multi-channel SQUID-based Ultra-Low Field Magnetic Resonance Imaging in Unshielded Environment Andrei Matlashov, Per Magnelind, Shaun Newman, Henrik Sandin, Algis Urbaitis, Petr Volegov, Michelle Espy

More information

iris pupil cornea ciliary muscles accommodation Retina Fovea blind spot

iris pupil cornea ciliary muscles accommodation Retina Fovea blind spot Chapter 6 Vision Exam 1 Anatomy of vision Primary visual cortex (striate cortex, V1) Prestriate cortex, Extrastriate cortex (Visual association coretx ) Second level association areas in the temporal and

More information

Evaluation Method of Magnetic Sensors Using the Calibrated Phantom for Magnetoencephalography

Evaluation Method of Magnetic Sensors Using the Calibrated Phantom for Magnetoencephalography J. Magn. Soc. Jpn., 41, 7-74 (217) Evaluation Method of Magnetic Sensors Using the Calibrated Phantom for Magnetoencephalography D. Oyama, Y. Adachi, and G. Uehara Applied Electronics Laboratory,

More information

780. Biomedical signal identification and analysis

780. Biomedical signal identification and analysis 780. Biomedical signal identification and analysis Agata Nawrocka 1, Andrzej Kot 2, Marcin Nawrocki 3 1, 2 Department of Process Control, AGH University of Science and Technology, Poland 3 Department of

More information

THE EFFECTS OF ROTATION AND INVERSION ON

THE EFFECTS OF ROTATION AND INVERSION ON Q0421 CN4500 / Jan 7, 02 (Mon)/ [17 pages, 0 tables, 8 figures, 1 footnotes] Edited from Disk. COGNITIVE NEUROPSYCHOLOGY, 2002, 19 (1), 31 47 THE EFFECTS OF ROTATION AND INVERSION ON FACE PROCESSING IN

More information

Beyond Blind Averaging Analyzing Event-Related Brain Dynamics

Beyond Blind Averaging Analyzing Event-Related Brain Dynamics Beyond Blind Averaging Analyzing Event-Related Brain Dynamics Scott Makeig Swartz Center for Computational Neuroscience Institute for Neural Computation University of California San Diego La Jolla, CA

More information

7Motion Perception. 7 Motion Perception. 7 Computation of Visual Motion. Chapter 7

7Motion Perception. 7 Motion Perception. 7 Computation of Visual Motion. Chapter 7 7Motion Perception Chapter 7 7 Motion Perception Computation of Visual Motion Eye Movements Using Motion Information The Man Who Couldn t See Motion 7 Computation of Visual Motion How would you build a

More information

Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion

Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion Vision Research 42 (2002) 2731 2735 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion Erich W. Graf *, Wendy J. Adams, Martin Lages Department

More information

Received 28 September 1999; accepted 15 October 1999

Received 28 September 1999; accepted 15 October 1999 COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE NEUROREPORT The N7 occipito-temporal component is delayed and enhanced to inverted faces but not to inverted objects: an electrophysiological account of face-speci c processes in

More information

Neuron, volume 57 Supplemental Data

Neuron, volume 57 Supplemental Data Neuron, volume 57 Supplemental Data Measurements of Simultaneously Recorded Spiking Activity and Local Field Potentials Suggest that Spatial Selection Emerges in the Frontal Eye Field Ilya E. Monosov,

More information

Maps in the Brain Introduction

Maps in the Brain Introduction Maps in the Brain Introduction 1 Overview A few words about Maps Cortical Maps: Development and (Re-)Structuring Auditory Maps Visual Maps Place Fields 2 What are Maps I Intuitive Definition: Maps are

More information

Un Approccio Sistemistico allo Studio delle Neuroscienze

Un Approccio Sistemistico allo Studio delle Neuroscienze Un Approccio Sistemistico allo Studio delle Neuroscienze Domenico Prattichizzo Dipartimento di Ingegneria dell Informazione Universita di Siena CIRA Settembre 2005 Tropea 0 Workshop su Robotica e Neuroscienze

More information

M R I Physics Course. Jerry Allison Ph.D., Chris Wright B.S., Tom Lavin B.S., Nathan Yanasak Ph.D. Department of Radiology Medical College of Georgia

M R I Physics Course. Jerry Allison Ph.D., Chris Wright B.S., Tom Lavin B.S., Nathan Yanasak Ph.D. Department of Radiology Medical College of Georgia M R I Physics Course Jerry Allison Ph.D., Chris Wright B.S., Tom Lavin B.S., Nathan Yanasak Ph.D. Department of Radiology Medical College of Georgia M R I Physics Course Magnetic Resonance Imaging Spatial

More information

NeuroImage 56 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

NeuroImage 56 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage: NeuroImage 56 (2011) 2356 2363 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect NeuroImage journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Differential selectivity for dynamic versus static information in face-selective

More information

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Cogn Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 June 23.

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Cogn Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 June 23. NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: J Cogn Neurosci. 2010 January ; 22(1): 203 211. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21203. Perception of Face Parts and Face Configurations: An

More information

EEG frequency tagging to study active and passive rhythmic movements

EEG frequency tagging to study active and passive rhythmic movements EEG frequency tagging to study active and passive rhythmic movements Dissertation presented by Aurore NIEUWENHUYS for obtaining the Master s degree in Biomedical Engineering Supervisor(s) André MOURAUX,

More information

Spectro-Temporal Methods in Primary Auditory Cortex David Klein Didier Depireux Jonathan Simon Shihab Shamma

Spectro-Temporal Methods in Primary Auditory Cortex David Klein Didier Depireux Jonathan Simon Shihab Shamma Spectro-Temporal Methods in Primary Auditory Cortex David Klein Didier Depireux Jonathan Simon Shihab Shamma & Department of Electrical Engineering Supported in part by a MURI grant from the Office of

More information

VISION. John Gabrieli Melissa Troyer 9.00

VISION. John Gabrieli Melissa Troyer 9.00 VISION John Gabrieli Melissa Troyer 9.00 Objectives Purposes of vision Problems that the visual system has to overcome Neural organization of vision Human Perceptual Abilities Detect a candle, 30 miles

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

Surround suppression effect in human early visual cortex contributes to illusory contour processing: MEG evidence.

Surround suppression effect in human early visual cortex contributes to illusory contour processing: MEG evidence. Kanizsa triangle (Kanizsa, 1955) Surround suppression effect in human early visual cortex contributes to illusory contour processing: MEG evidence Boris Chernyshev Laboratory of Cognitive Psychophysiology

More information

III: Vision. Objectives:

III: Vision. Objectives: III: Vision Objectives: Describe the characteristics of visible light, and explain the process by which the eye transforms light energy into neural. Describe how the eye and the brain process visual information.

More information

Fusiform Face Area in Chess Expertise

Fusiform Face Area in Chess Expertise Fusiform Face Area in Chess Expertise Merim Bilalić (merim.bilalic@med.uni-tuebingen.de) Department of Neuroradiology, Hoppe-Seyler Str. 2 Tübingen, 72076, Germany Abstract The ability to recognize faces

More information

Stochastic resonance of the visually evoked potential

Stochastic resonance of the visually evoked potential PHYSICAL REVIEW E VOLUME 59, NUMBER 3 MARCH 1999 Stochastic resonance of the visually evoked potential R. Srebro* and P. Malladi Department of Ophthalmology and Department of Biomedical Engineering, University

More information

Chapter Six Chapter Six

Chapter Six Chapter Six Chapter Six Chapter Six Vision Sight begins with Light The advantages of electromagnetic radiation (Light) as a stimulus are Electromagnetic energy is abundant, travels VERY quickly and in fairly straight

More information

fmri-derived cortical maps for haptic shape, texture, and hardness

fmri-derived cortical maps for haptic shape, texture, and hardness Cognitive Brain Research 12 (2001) 307 313 www.elsevier.com/ locate/ bres Research report fmri-derived cortical maps for haptic shape, texture, and hardness * a b P. Servos, S. Lederman, D. Wilson, J.

More information

Simultaneous Multi-Slice (Slice Accelerated) Diffusion EPI

Simultaneous Multi-Slice (Slice Accelerated) Diffusion EPI Simultaneous Multi-Slice (Slice Accelerated) Diffusion EPI Val M. Runge, MD Institute for Diagnostic and Interventional Radiology Clinics for Neuroradiology and Nuclear Medicine University Hospital Zurich

More information

Event-Related Potential and Functional MRI Measures of Face-Selectivity are Highly Correlated: A Simultaneous ERP-fMRI Investigation

Event-Related Potential and Functional MRI Measures of Face-Selectivity are Highly Correlated: A Simultaneous ERP-fMRI Investigation r Human Brain Mapping 000:000 000 (2010) r Event-Related Potential and Functional MRI Measures of Face-Selectivity are Highly Correlated: A Simultaneous ERP-fMRI Investigation Boaz Sadeh, 1 Ilana Podlipsky,

More information

Residual fmri sensitivity for identity changes in acquired prosopagnosia

Residual fmri sensitivity for identity changes in acquired prosopagnosia ORIGINAL RESEARCH ARTICLE published: 18 October 2013 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00756 Residual fmri sensitivity for identity changes in acquired prosopagnosia Christopher J. Fox 1 *, Giuseppe Iaria 2, Bradley

More information

Motor Imagery based Brain Computer Interface (BCI) using Artificial Neural Network Classifiers

Motor Imagery based Brain Computer Interface (BCI) using Artificial Neural Network Classifiers Motor Imagery based Brain Computer Interface (BCI) using Artificial Neural Network Classifiers Maitreyee Wairagkar Brain Embodiment Lab, School of Systems Engineering, University of Reading, Reading, U.K.

More information

Embodiment illusions via multisensory integration

Embodiment illusions via multisensory integration Embodiment illusions via multisensory integration COGS160: sensory systems and neural coding presenter: Pradeep Shenoy 1 The illusory hand Botvinnik, Science 2004 2 2 This hand is my hand An illusion of

More information

Segmentation of Cerebral Gyri in the Sectioned Images by Referring to Volume Model

Segmentation of Cerebral Gyri in the Sectioned Images by Referring to Volume Model ORIGINAL ARTICLE Basic Medical Sciences DOI: 10.3346/jkms.2010.25.12.1710 J Korean Med Sci 2010; 25: 1710-1715 Segmentation of Cerebral Gyri in the Sectioned Images by Referring to Volume Model Jin Seo

More information

Motion-reversal Visual Evoked Responses

Motion-reversal Visual Evoked Responses Physiol. Res. 41: 369-373, 1992 Motion-reversal Visual Evoked Responses M. KUBA, NAOTO TOYONAGA1, Z. KUBOVÁ Medical Faculty of Charles University, Department of Pathophysiology and Department of Physiology,

More information

Electrophysiological correlates of purely temporal figure ground segregation

Electrophysiological correlates of purely temporal figure ground segregation Vision Research 43 (2003) 2583 2589 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Electrophysiological correlates of purely temporal figure ground segregation Farid I. Kandil *, Manfred Fahle Human Neurobiology, University

More information

Non Invasive Brain Computer Interface for Movement Control

Non Invasive Brain Computer Interface for Movement Control Non Invasive Brain Computer Interface for Movement Control V.Venkatasubramanian 1, R. Karthik Balaji 2 Abstract: - There are alternate methods that ease the movement of wheelchairs such as voice control,

More information