Temporal Modulation Transfer Functions for Tonal Stimuli: Gated versus Continuous Conditions

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Temporal Modulation Transfer Functions for Tonal Stimuli: Gated versus Continuous Conditions"

Transcription

1 Auditory Neuroscience, Vol. 3(4), pp Reprints available directly from the publisher Photocopying permitted by license only 1997 OPA (Overseas Publishers Association) Amsterdam B.V. Published in The Netherlands by Harwood Academic Publishers Printed in Malaysia Temporal Modulation Transfer Functions for Tonal Stimuli: Gated versus Continuous Conditions WILLIAM A. YOST* and STANLEY SHEFT Parmly Hearing Institute, Loyola University, 6525 N. Sheridan Rd Chicago IL (Received 12 January 1996; Accepted 2 September 1996) Temporal Modulation Transfer Functions (TMTFs) were obtained for tonal carriers with frequencies of 500, 1000, and 4000 Hz. The carrier was either on continuously or gated. In the continuous-carrier condition the duration of the modulation was 125 or 500 msec, and in a gated-carrier condition the entire stimulus duration was 125 or 500 msec. Data were collected from six listeners using an adaptive, two-alternative, forced-choice procedure. The lowest thresholds were obtained for modulation rates between 4 and 8 Hz in the continuous-carrier condition and between 16 and 64 Hz in the gated-carrier condition. Several different processing strategies were used to try to account for the shape of the tonal TMTFs. The threshold changes with carrier frequency in the tonal TMTFs at high rates of modulation were best accounted for by assuming that listeners can detect the sidebands which fall outside of the critical band centered at the carrier frequency. The low- and mid-rate regions of the tonal TMTF were best described in a qualitative manner by a process that accumulates information based on the number of cycles of envelope fluctuations. However, this process could not quantitatively explain the tonal TMTF data in the low- to mid-rate regions nor could it account for the differences between tonal and noise TMTFs. Keywords: Temporal modulation transfer function (TMTF), sinusoidal amplitude modulation (SAM), envelope fluctuations, adaptation, multiple looks INTRODUCTION The function relating thresholds for detecting sinusoidal am plitude m odulation (SAM) to the rate of modulation has been called a Temporal Modulation Transfer Function (TMTF). A number of psychophysical studies have measured the TMTF for noise carriers (e.g., Rodenburg, 1977; Viemeister, 1979; Sheft and Yost, 1990; and Eddins, 1993 for reviews), but there are fewer data which describe TMTFs for tonal carriers (see Kohlrausch, 1993 for a discussion of tonal TM TF studies). In a study by Zwicker (1952), the modulated tones were presented continuously and a method-of-adjustment procedure was used to estimate SAM detection thresholds. These data suggest that the auditory system is most sensitive to SAM when the Corresponding author. Tel.: (312) Fax: (312) WYOST@LUC.EDU. 401

2 402 W. A. YOST and S. SHEFT tones are m odulated at approxim ately 4 Hz. Reisz (1928), also using a continuous stimulus, established that for two beating tones the perception of best- beats occurs at approximately 3 to 4 Hz. Results from these two studies have been used to suggest that the auditory system is most sensitive to modulation rates of 3 to 4 Hz (see Viemeister, 1979). Although we are unaware of any recent studies of tonal TMTFs, several different lines of research have used SAM o f tonal carriers. In recent studies of M odulation Detection Interference (e.g., Yost and Sheft, 1989; Hall and Grose, 1990; M oore et al., 1991; Y ost and Sheft, 1994), the SAM thresholds for gated tones are higher than those reported by Zwicker for continuous tones. In other studies using gated tonal carriers, modulation thresholds from only some of the listeners were greater than the Zwicker results (e.g., M oore and Sek, 1992 and 1995; Edwards and V iem eister, 1994a and b). Viemeister (1979), Forest and Green (1987) and Sheft and Yost (1990) have all shown that SAM detection thresholds at low modulation rates are higher for gated than for continuous noise carriers. The loss in sensitivity at low modulation rates in the gated-noise conditions results in the lowest m odulation detection thresholds being obtained at rates considerably higher than 3-4 Hz. Sheft and Yost (1990) also showed that the shape of the TMTFs for noise depends on stimulus duration. There is no current study comparing tonal TMTFs with gated and continuous carriers and as a function of duration. Since both of these variables affect noise TMTFs and there is some evidence of an effect o f carrier gating on tonal TM TFs, w e determ ined tonal TM TFs as a function of duration and gated versus continuous presentation. In general the TMTFs for tones have three regions based on modulation rate. In the high-rate region, the sidebands due to SAM become detectable leading to m odulation thresholds decreasing as rate increases. The rate at which sidebands are just detectable depends on the carrier frequency. Because the width of the critical band increases with frequency, a higher SAM rate is needed for sideband detection as the carrier frequency is increased. In the m id-rate region several percepts occur. For rates above about 50 Hz at high modulation depths, a complex pitch equal to the SAM rate can be detected. Also, in this region the roughness of the tem poral fluctuations is detectable but disappears at low modulation depths and high rates. In the m id-rate region, thresholds tend to increase as rate increases. In the low-rate region at high modulation depths there is a clear waxing and waning of loudness. That is, the percept is no longer that of a stable sound but one of a sound of a particular pitch whose loudness is changing over time. At these low rates, thresholds either remain somewhat constant as a function of modulation rate or increase as rate decreases, especially when the rate is less than 4 Hz. Some of the literature cited above suggests that the modulation rates defining these three regions of the TM TF depend on whether the tones are gated or continuous. A m plitude m odulated tones are often used to obtained neural TMTFs for different locations in the auditory nervous system (e.g., Schreiner and Urbus, 1988; Jorris and Yin, 1992; Frisina et al., 1996). Most of the physiological studies of tonal TM TFs use gated signals, yet com parisons are often m ade to psychophysical data obtained from studies using continuous carriers. Thus, docum entation of differences between TMTFs obtained with gated and continuous carriers would provide useful inform ation when com paring physiological and psychophysical measures. METHODS The stimuli are described by the following equation: [1 + msin(27tfm)][cos(27ifc)]/[(l + m2/2 )^ ], (1) where m (0<m <l) is the depth of modulation, fm is the modulation rate (2, 4, 8, 16, 32, 64, and 128 Hz), and fc is the carrier frequency (500, 1000, and 4000 Hz). The carriers were presented at 70 db SPL. The carrier was either gated on and off in each observation interval or was on continuously during a block of trials. Stimulus durations of 125 and 500 msec were used in the gated condition. The modulation had a duration of either 125 or 500 msec in the continuous condition. In the gated conditions, the waveforms were shaped with a 5-msec raised cosine. Table I describes the basic stimulus conditions.

3 TONAL TMTFs 403 TABLE I Stimulus conditions and use of listeners. Labels SI to S6 identify subjects who participated in each condition Duration Carrier Frequencies 500 Hz 1000 Hz 4000 Hz Gated 125 msec Modulation rates of 8,16,32,64,128 Hz Carrier (S1-S3) (S1-S3) (S1-S3) 500 msec Modulation rates of 2,4,8,16,32,64,128 Hz (S1-S3) (S1-S3) (S1-S6) 125 msec Modulation rates of 8,16,32,64,128 Hz Continuous (S1-S3) (S1-S3) (S1-S3) Carrier 500 msec Modulation rates of 2,4,8,16,32,64,128 Hz (S1-S3) (S1-S3) (S1-S6) The thresholds (expressed as 20 log m) were obtained with a two-alternative, forced-choice adaptive procedure. The two observation intervals followed a 500-msec warning interval and were separated by 500 msec. Feedback was provided after each trial in the 50-trial block. In the gated conditions, the signal interval contained the m odulated stimulus and the non-signal interval contained the unmodulated carrier. In the continuous condition, the carrier was on continuously and was modulated only during the signal interval. The Levitt (1971) two-down, one-up method was used to estimate the 70.7% point on the psychometric function. The step size in terms of 20 log m was 4 db until the second reversal and then it was reduced to 2 db. At least ten reversals of modulation depth (20 log m) were required before a threshold for a block was calculated. Excluding the first four reversals, the threshold for any 50 trial block was estimated by averaging the modulation depths of the remaining reversals. At least four thresholds were estimated for each condition. If the four threshold estimates were within ±3 db of each other, then the final threshold was the average of these four thresholds. If the thresholds were not within ±3 db of each other, then additional thresholds were obtained until four consecutive thresholds were within the ±3 db range. The four that were within this range were then averaged for the final threshold estimate. For the majority of the conditions a final threshold was obtained with six or fewer threshold estimates. Six listeners with normal thresholds of hearing were used in the study. Data for all conditions were obtained from three listeners (SI, S2, S3). Data for the 4000-Hz carrier, the gated and continuous conditions at 500 msec were obtained for the three other listeners (S4, S5, S6). See Table I for a description of the assignment of listeners to the various conditions. The stimuli were presented diotically over TDH-49 headphones mounted in MX/41 AR cushions to the listeners seated in a soundproof booth. RESULTS The average TMTFs are shown in Figures 1-4. Figure 1 shows the data for the gated conditions, Figure 2 the data for the continuous conditions, Figure 3 a replotting of the data for the 500-msec conditions, and Figure 4 a replotting of the data for the 125-msec conditions. W ith the exception of many of the data for the 125-msec condition, the TMTFs can be divided into the three general regions described in the Introduction. The divisions are general descriptions of the TMTFs and often depend on carrier gating and duration. For the 500-msec conditions, the low-rate region extends up to 8 Hz for continuous carriers and up to 64 Hz for gated carriers. In the low-rate region thresholds decrease with increasing modulation rates. The low- rate region is more evident with the 500-msec than with the 125-msec durations. For the 500-msec conditions, somewhat independent of whether or not the tones were gated or continuous, the mid-rate region extended up to the highest rate (128 Hz) tested for the 4000-Hz carrier, to about 64 Hz for the 1000-Hz carrier, and to about 32 Hz for the 500-Hz carrier. There

4 404 W. A. YOST and S. SHEFT Gated so o o (N Modulation Rate (Hz) msec, 500-Hz 125-msec, 1000-Hz 125-msec, 4000-Hz A 500-msec, 500-Hz 500-msec, 1000-Hz - F I 500-msec 4000-Hz FIGURE 1 Average AM detection thresholds for the gated conditions as a function of modulation rate (in Hz). Data are shown for 125-msec (closed symbols) and 500-msec conditions (open symbols) for three carrier frequencies (500 Hz, triangles; 1000 Hz, circles; and 4000 Hz, squares). was little evidence of a mid-rate region for 125-msec durations except perhaps for the 4000-H z carrier, where the mid-rate region again extended to 128 Hz. In this mid-rate region, thresholds increase with increasing m odulation rate. The high-rate region, where thresholds decrease with increasing rate, was clear for the 500 and 1000-Hz carriers, but did not occur for the 4000-Hz carrier. For the 125-msec gated conditions, there is no break in the shape of the TMTFs as a function o f rate, except for the 4000-Hz carrier. In addition: 1) thresholds are lower for the continuous than for the gated conditions, especially at low modulation rates; 2) the thresholds are lower for the 500-msec conditions than for the 125-msec conditions, especially at low modulation rates; 3) in the low- and mid-rate regions, thresholds are lowest for modulation rates between 4 and 8 Hz in the 500-msec continuous conditions, and they are lowest for modulation rates of Hz for the 500-msec gated conditions; 4) although the slope of the TM TF from mid to high rates is difficult to estimate for many conditions, it is between 2.5 and 4.5 db/octave in the 500-msec conditions, while the slope on the low-rate side of the 500-msec TMTFs is less than 3 db/octave for the continuous conditions and closer to 3 db/octave for the gated conditions. For all conditions shown in Figures 1-4 the standard error of the mean computed for the six listeners ranged from 0.9 to 1.7 db. For each condition all listener s

5 TONAL TMTFs 405 Continuous 125-msec, 500-Hz 125-msec, 1000-Hz 125-msec, 4000-Hz r- 500-msec, 500-Hz - Q 500-msec, 1000-Hz n 500-msec, 4000-Hz FIGURE 2 Average AM detection thresholds for the continuous conditions as a function of modulation rate (in Hz). Data are shown for 125-msec (closed symbols) and 500-msec conditions (open symbols) for three carrier frequencies (500 Hz, triangles; 1000 Hz, circles; and 4000 Hz, squares). thresholds changed in approximately the same way with modulation rate. The major difference among listeners was in their overall sensitivity for detecting modulation. DISCUSSION As pointed out by many others (e.g., Zwicker, 1952 and Viemeister, 1979), the drop in thresholds at high rates of modulation is most likely the result of sideband detection. Listeners can discriminate the modulated from the unmodulated stimulus by listening for the presence of the sidebands rather than detecting the temporal fluctuations in the envelope. If the critical band at 500 Hz is slightly narrower than 100 Hz, then thresholds for both 64-Hz and 128-Hz rates of modulation should reflect sideband detection. A critical band at 1000 Hz of approximately 120 Hz rate allows for sideband detected with 128-Hz SAM. If the critical band at 4000 Hz is much wider than 128 Hz, then sideband detection would not be expected at any of the rates used. These predictions are reflected in the data of Figures 1-4. The rise in thresholds with increasing modulation rate in the mid-rate region of the TMTF has usually been attributed to a lowpass filter stage for processing temporal envelopes (see Viemeister, 1979; Sheft and Yost, 1990 for a complete discussion of this lowpass filter assumption). The integration time of the lowpass

6 406 W. A. YOST and S. SHEFT 500-msec. 10 -J r : ' i t * : i n Modulation Rate (Hz) -A Cont., 500-Hz Cont., 1000-Hz Cont, 4000-Hz A Gated, 500-Hz O Gated, 1000-Hz Gated, 4000-Hz FIGURE 3 Average AM detection thresholds for the 500-msec conditions as a function of modulation rate (in Hz). Data are shown for gated (open symbols) and continuous conditions (filled symbols) for three carrier frequencies (500 Hz, triangles; 1000 Hz, circles; and 4000 Hz, squares). filter sets the m odulation rate at w hich threshold begins to rise with increasing m odulation rate; the number of filter stages or the order of the filter determines the rate at which threshold increases. Since the TMTFs are clearly not like the output of a lowpass filter, but more like the output of a bandpass filter, it is clear that either a lowpass filter is an inappropriate model for these tonal TMTFs or that processes in addition to lowpass filtering are involved. Sheft and Yost (1-990) and, earlier, V iem eister (1979) discussed a few possible explanations for the low-rate region of noise carrier TM TFs. However, none of these explanations could account for their data. Less attention has been paid to the low-rate region for tonal carriers. The data from Figure 1 clearly indicate a low-rate region for 500-msec gated tonal carriers. For the 4000-Hz carrier, where sideband detection does not occur, the TMTF is clearly bandpass. We will next investigate a few mechanisms that might account for the bandpass shape of the TMTFs: adaptation of various forms, and lateral inhibition. Adaptation Viemeister (1979) suggested that losses in modulation sensitivity at low AM rates with a gated noise carrier might be due to short-term neural adaptation. He proposed that the modulation of the neural response by adaptation might interfere with the detection of m odulation. W ith the largest change in the neural response

7 TONAL TMTFs msec Modulation Rate (Hz) A Cont., 500-Hz 9 Cont., 1000-Hz Cont., 4000-Hz A Gated, 500-Hz O Gated, 1000-Hz Gated, 4000-Hz FIGURE 4 Average AM detection thresholds for the 125-msec conditions as a function of modulation rate (in Hz). Data are shown for gated (open symbols) and continuous conditions (filled symbols) for three carrier frequencies (500 Hz, triangles; 1000 Hz, circles; and 4000 Hz, squares). by adaptation occurring near stimulus onset, the interference w ould be greatest at low m odulation rates where few cycles of modulation are present. Sheft and Yost (1990) evaluated the effect of adding a 500-msec unmodulated forward carrier fringe on the detection of low rate SAM with a noise carrier. The addition of the 500-msec fringe did not elim inate the highpass segment of the TMTF. Since 500 msec is much longer than the time course of neural measures, these results suggested that m echanism s other than short-term neural adaptation are required to account for the bandpass characteristics of the TMTF. Sheft and Yost (1990) suggested that the highpass segment of the gated noise-carrier TM TF may reflect involvem ent o f an adaptation process slow er than short-term neural adaptation. Neural channels selective to AM rate may be the site of one such process. However, attempts by Sheft and Yost (1990) failed to confirm this concept. In addition, there is no convincing argument to limit the effect of this form of adaptation to low AM rates. In general, the fact that tonal TMTFs from both gated and continuous carrier conditions are bandpass leaves consideration of adaptation by itself an inadequate explanation for the bandpass shape of the tonal TMTF. Lateral Inhibition Procedures similar to the ones used to obtain auditory TMTFs have been used in vision to obtain temporal

8 408 W. A. YOST and S. SHEFT modulation transfer functions (see Comsweet, 1970). Gated conditions are almost always used to estimate the visual TMTFs, and the shape of the modulation transfer functions are bandpass. Lateral inhibition is a common process used to explain the bandpass characteristics of the visual TM TFs. The TMTF is treated as a transfer function and its inverse Fourier transform (assuming in our case a zero phase-function) is used to generate a temporal weighting function (an impulse response). Figure 5 shows the tem poral weighting function generated from the inverse Fourier transform of the TMTF obtained with the 500-msec, 4000-Hz tonal carriers in the gated condition (see Fig. 1). The 4000-Hz carrier TM TF was used since it appears to be unaffected by sideband detection. The negative portion of the tem poral weighting function is viewed in vision as indicating a lateral inhibitory mechanism in temporal processing. By convolving a stimulus with this temporal weighting function, one can m ake predictions regarding tem poral resolution. Although such temporal weighting functions have been suggested for visual processing, their use in audition is more limited. Plack and M oore (1990) have assumed a temporal weighting function derived from a variety of different tem poral procedures, but these temporal weighting functions do not show evidence of a lateral inhibitory mechanism. Gilkey and Robinson Temporal Weighting Function 1 <D = 0.8 f- 0.6 < o 0.4 o> N E o 0 z Time - msec. FIGURE 5 Temporal weighting function obtained for the inverse Fourier Transform of the tonal TMTF for the 4000-Hz carrier in the gated condition. See Text for additional information. (1986) derived tem poral weighting functions for detecting tones in frozen noise, and some of these temporal weighting functions did go negative indicating the possibility of a lateral inhibitory mechanism. Since the shape of the TMTFs differ as a function of carrier frequency, duration, and with carrier gating, each condition would require a different weighting function. In vision, a single w eighting function is usually m easured, and there is no a priori reason why different conditions in audition should yield different temporal weighting functions. In the auditory system, unlike in vision, there is no known physiological mechanism for the form of lateral inhibition illustrated in Figure 5. In applying the weighting function, one would also have to deal with the issue of causality, since the weighting function extends to both positive and negative time. M ultiple-looks Perhaps listeners use the modulation cycle as the basis for a decision that modulation has occurred (as described by Viemeister,1979). The more cycles that occur, the more looks the listener has at this decision variable, and perform ance improves as the num ber of looks (number of cycles) increases. Thus, with only a few looks the performance is poor and it improves as the number of modulation cycles increases, explaining the drop in thresholds in the low-rate region as modulation rate increases. Using the number of envelope periods is also one way o f equating the 125-msec and 500-msec conditions. If number of envelope periods is a reasonable way of accounting for the TMTF then one might expect TMTFs for the 125- and 500-msec durations to be similar at least in the low- and mid-rate regions (where sideband detection is not possible). Figure 6 shows the TMTFs for the gated (on top) and continuous (on bottom) conditions plotted as a function of the number of envelope periods (that is, dividing rate by 2 for the 500-msec data and dividing rate by 8 for the 125-msec data of Figures 1 and 2). In the low-rate region the TMTFs tend to align fairly well for the gated conditions, but with more spread for the continuous conditions. Thus, there might be some validity in attributing the shape of the TM TF in the low-rate region, at least

9 TONAL TMTFs 409 Gated I i I r Number of Modulation Periods 125-msec, 500-Hz 125-msec, 1000-Hz 125-msec, 4000-Hz -A 500-msec, 500-Hz O 500-msec, 1000-Hz Q 500-msec, 4000-Hz Continuous A 125-msec, 500-Hz 125-msec, 1000-Hz 125-msec, 4000-Hz A 500-msec, 500-Hz O 500-msec, 1000-Hz -^3 500-msec, 4000-Hz FIGURE 6 The TMTFs of Figures 1 and 2 are replotted as a function of the number of modulation periods. That is, modulation rate was divided by 2 for the 500-msec conditions and by 8 for the 125-msec conditions. The gated conditions are shown on top and the continuous conditions are shown on the bottom.

10 410 W. A. YOST and S. SHEFT for the gated condition, to a process based on accumulating inform ation from each cycle of m odulation. However, such a simple multiple-looks explanation would not predict the bandpass shape of the TMTF, since performance should continue improving as the number of looks increases. To use such an explanation to describe the low-and m id-rate regions of the TM TFs, we have adopted aspects of the model proposed by H after and his colleagues (see Yost and Hafter, 1987 or Hafter et al., 1988). Hafter et al. (1988) developed the model to account for click-lateralization thresholds. We are adopting the model to account for the TMTFs. The m ajor assumption of our im plementation of this m odel is that when m odulation rate increases, the listener has a greater number of looks to determine if the tone was modulated. For the present derivation we assume that each observation of the envelope period is an independent event and the measure of the listener s performance is proportional to the independent variable (e.g., M oore and Sek, 1992, have shown that d' is proportional to m2 for SAM tones). W ith these assum ptions, m odulation thresholds in terms of m are predicted to decrease at a rate equal to the square root of the number of observations of the envelope period (1/N0 5, where N is the num ber of observations). If we assum e, as H after has, that there is a com pressive property operating such that each successive observation contributes progressively less to the decision, then the decline in threshold is proportional to 1/N 5k, where 0 < k < 1. W e assume further (as has Hafter) that k is a function of the rate of modulation, in that k becomes smaller as the rate increases, such that at fast rates the observations come so close together there is no accum ulation of information due to multiple observations. The assumption of the compressive function appears reasonable in that if the period of modulation is too short then some form of integration might make it difficult to determine the envelope period. This is similar to the rationale given by Hafter in his application o f the model to interaural difference thresholds for repeated binaural clicks. This function for accum ulating inform ation as modulation rate increases has a bandpass shape, first increasing and then decreasing as N increases. In order to provide some insight about this relationship, we used a sim ple exponential to define the relationship between k and the modulation rate; that is, k = exp(-cr), where c is a constant 0 < c > 1, and R is the modulation rate [R = fm in equation 1)]. Therefore, this suggests that thresholds would change as: l/n '5[exP(~cR)l (2) If we assume that performance (e.g., d') is proportional to m2 as suggested by Moore and Sek, (1992), then 20 log m is related to N by 10 log(l/na5[exp(-cr)]). Figure 7 shows a family of curves (with c as the parameter from.005 to.03 in.005 steps) plotted in terms of 20 log m versus N. The curves for 500-msec duration are on top and those for 125-msec duration are on the bottom. These curves have several qualitative similarities to the tonal TMTFs. The curves are bandpass in shape as are the TMTFs. For both the predictions and the actual TMTFs there is a larger change in performance as a function of N for 500 msec than for 125 msec. For any one value of c, the peak in the predicted function shifts to a lower number of modulation periods as the duration is lowered from 500 to 125 msec. This is like the peak sensitivity in the TMTF shifting to a lower modulation rate when the duration is lowered. The slopes on the low- and high rate sides are 1,5-dB per octave (or slightly less) for both the 500-msec predictions.1 Even at a qualitative level this approach cannot account for the differences in the TMTFs due to carrier frequency and carrier gating. However, there is one assum ption that can be m ade that will qualitatively m ake the predictions fit with some of the changes in the TMTFs between gated and continuous presentations. One might assume that in the continuous conditions the change from the continuous to a modulated tone and the change back from modulation to a continuous tone serve as two more transitions in 1 Moore and Sek (1992) showed that d' is proportional to m2 for gated tones in the low-rate region of the TMTF. If performance is also proportional to 1/N0'5, then one might predict that the rise in sensitivity with increasing rate in the low-rate region would be 1.5 db per doubling of the number of observations. However, if performance is proportional to 1/Nk05, as we suggest, then the rise in sensitivity with number of observations depends on k as well, and would be between 1.5 and 3 db per doubling of observations. Thus, there is a qualitative agreement between these calculations and the obtained TMTFs.

11 TONAL TMTFs m sec. 125 m sec. FIGURE 7 The predictions based on the equation 201og(l/Na5"xp<-cR)1) plotted against N, where N is the number of modulation periods, R is modulate rate, and c a constant which is the parameter on the figure with c ranging from (top curve) to 0.03 (bottom curve) m steps. Functions for the 500-msec duration are shown on top and those for 125-msec duration are shown on the bottom.

12 412 W. A. YOST and S. SHEFT level that might be used to define two more modulation cycles. By adding 2 to each N of the predictions shown in Figure 7, the peak will shift to a lower number of modulation periods as it does in the TMTFs when the conditions change from gated to continuous. Even though there are a number of qualitative similarities between the curves of Figure 7 and the data of Figure 6, there are many quantitative discrepancies. The maximum change in performance for the predictions is less than 6 db for the 500-msec conditions and less than 4 db for the 125-msec conditions. This is 10 or more db less than obtained in the data. As N increases the predicted perform ance can never be worse than that at N = 1. The TMTFs clearly show that this is not true in the mid-rate region. The slopes of the low- and high-rate regions are never more than 1.5-dB per octave in the predicted functions, yet these slopes can be more then 3-dB per octave in the obtained TM TFs. The shape of the predicted functions with 125-msec duration is always bandpass, whereas the TMTFs for 125 msec are not always bandpass. Also, for the continuous conditions for 125-msec stimuli, the different thresholds for the different carrier frequencies in the low rate region of the TM TF is not accounted for by this model. As Figure 6 indicates, performance is not exactly the same for both durations when considered on the basis of the number of modulation periods. Thus, the model based on accumulating information over the num ber o f m odulation periods does not appear to quantitatively account for the shapes of the TM TFs. However, the qualitative com parisons suggest that there is some validity to a consideration that processing may be partially based on accumulating information over m odulation periods in the manner suggested, especially in the low-rate region. In addition, we assumed that k = exp(-cr ) to describe how k might change with modulation rate. W e have no prior reason for using this exponential relationship expect for the fact that k decreases as R increases which is the desired relationship. O ther relationships m ight provide a better account of the data, but at the moment we have no basis for choosing any particular one. W e have been unable to develop a single model which accounts for the tonal TMTFs and which would be compatible with noise-carrier TMTFs. This may be because the TMTF, especially the tonal TMTF, may not reflect a single process. It is already acknowledged that there are at least two processes involved with tonal TMTFs, one that operates in the high-rate region that takes into account sideband detection, and another that covers the low- and mid-rate regions. It might also be that the low- and mid-rate regions depend on more than one process. After all, as was discussed in the Introduction, the percept varies with modulation rate. These percepts also interact with carrier frequency and duration. For instance, the saliency of the pitch of SAM tones depends on the carrier frequency (see Cohen et al., 1995, for a recent discussion o f the laws of complex pitch) and duration. Roughness depends on duration (see Terhardt, 1968). If these percepts are used by listeners for AM detection, then one might expect that more than one process might be involved in determining a tonal TMTF. ADDITIONAL DISCUSSION The TMTFs obtained in this study differ from those reported by Zwicker (1952) and to some extent from those reported by Viemeister (1979). There appear to be two major differences: the thresholds obtained by Zwicker are 5 to 10 db lower than those obtained in this study (and also by Viemeister, 1979) at the comparable sound levels; and the TMTFs obtained by Zwicker and Viemeister (1979) clearly have their minimum at a modulation rate of 4 Hz, with the minimum for our data are at a slightly higher modulation rate. There are major procedural differences between Zwicker s and our study which might help explain the differences in the TMTFs. Zwicker used a method of adjustment procedure (Bekesy tracking) with continuously modulated tones. That is, the listeners adjusted the depth of a continuously modulated tone until they no longer detected modulation. Perhaps Zwicker s procedure is a more sensitive psychophysical procedure for measuring tonal TMTFs than the method of constant stimulus used in this study (see Viemeister, 1979). Our method was very close to that used by Viemeister (1979) and he found the peak to be at 4 Hz for the continuous condition, not at 8 Hz. There were twelve TMTFs obtained in this study for the continuous condition at 500 msec (3 listeners for 500-Hz

13 TONAL TMTFs 413 carrier, 3 for 1000-Hz carrier, and 6 for 4000-Hz carrier, see Table I). Of these twelve TMTFs, six had best sensitivity at 4 Hz, four had best sensitivity at 8 Hz, and two had equal thresholds at 4 and 8 Hz. The peak sensitivity for two of TMTFs was much greater at 8 Hz than it was for the other TMTFs. Thus, on average the best sensitivity in the TMTF was at 8 Hz, but more listeners generated TMTFs in the continuous case (at 500 msec) with a peak at 4 Hz. A major empirical finding of this study is that the modulation rate at which modulation-detection thresholds are lowest is higher in the gated than in the continuous condition and the rate for best sensitivity depends on the duration of the carrier. This finding of best sensitivity at higher rates for the gated than for the continuous condition for tonal TMTFs has also been found for the TMTFs with noise carriers (Viemeister, 1979; Sheft and Yost, 1990). There is evidence of this same trend in the literature on tonal modulation (e.g., Yost and Sheft, 1989; Green and Nguyen, 1988; Hall and Grose, 1990; Moore et al., 1991; Yost and Sheft, 1994), but other studies have found that this trend is listener dependent (Moore and Sek, 1992, 1995; Edwards and Viemeister, 1994a,b). It is difficult to compare across studies since tonal TM TFs depend on many stimulus conditions (modulation rate, carrier frequency, duration, gated versus continuous presentation and overall level are the major variables for tonal TMTFs). Also Moore and Sek (1995) suggested that detecting modulation might depend on different strategies adopted by listeners in determining what to listen for in a modulation detection task. The large effects many of these variables have on tonal TMTFs was one of our motivations for the present study comparing several of these variables with the same set of listeners. This comparison mirrors the work with noise carriers showing that the rate at which best sensitivity occurs is higher for gated than for continuous presentations. As such, the dictum that best modulation detection occurs at 3-4 Hz may only apply to'continu- ous stimuli (see Zwicker and Fasti, 1990). A comparison of the data from this and other studies of AM detection with tonal carriers (e.g., Yost and Sheft, 1989; Green and Nguyen, 1988; Hall and Grose, 1990; Moore et a l, 1991; Yost and Sheft, 1994) with the data from the study of AM detection with noise earners (Rodenburg, 1977; Viemeister, 1979; Forest and Green, 1987; Sheft and Yost, 1990) suggests that the form of the TMTF for the two types of carriers is different. The noise carrier TMTFs appear to be much more lowpass than the bandpass characteristics of the tonal TMTF, although the noise carrier TMTFs is bandpass in shape for gated conditions (Viemeister, 1979; Sheft and Yost, 1990). Viemeister (1979), Forest and Green (1987), and Sheft and Yost (1990) have all offered explanations of the noise-carrier TMTF that involve assumptions of lowpass filtering. Since the tonal-carrier TMTFs are more bandpass than lowpass, processes in addition to lowpass filtering appear needed to account for the tonal data. However, none of the models discussed in this paper appear capable of accounting for all of the tonal TMTF data and for the differences between tonal and noise TMTFs. Thus, a single comprehensive model of TMTFs is still not available. Acknowledgements We would like to thank the faculty of the Parmly Hearing Institute for their many valuable comments on this work and paper. This research was supported by a Program Project Grant from the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders, NIH and a grant from the Air Force Office of Scientific Research. References Cohen, M. A., Grossberg, S. and Wyse, L. L. (1995) A spectral network model for pitch perception, J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 98, Cornsweet, T. (1970) Visual Perception, Academic Press, New York. Eddins, D. A. (1993) Amplitude modulation detection of narrowband noise: Effects of absolute bandwidth and frequency region, J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 93, Edwards, B. W. and Viemeister, N. E. (1994a) Modulation detection and discrimination with 3-component signals, J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 95, Edwards, B. W. and Viemeister, N. E. (1994b) Frequency modulation versus amplitude modulation: Evidence for a second frequency modulation coding mechanism, J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 96, Forest, T. G. and Green, D. M. (1987) Detection of partially filled gaps in noise and the temporal modulation transfer function, J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 82, Frisina, R. P., Karcich, K. J., Tracy, T. C., Sullivan, D. M. and Walton, J. P. (1996) Preservation of amplitude modulation coding in the presence of background noise by the Chinchilla auditory nerve fibers, J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 99,

14 414 W. A. YOST and S. SHEFT Gilkey, R. H. and Robinson, D. E. (1986) Models of auditory masking: A molecular psychophysical approach, J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 79, Green, D. M. and Nguyen, Q. T. (1988) Profile analysis: Detecting dynamic spectral changes, Hear. Res. 32, Hafter, E. R., Buell, T. N., and Richards, V. M. (1988) Onset-coding in lateralization: Its form, site, and function. In: Auditory Functions: Neurobiological Bases o f Hearing, G. M. Edelman, W. E. Gall and W. M. Cowan, eds., John Wiley Sons, New York, Hall, J. W. and Grose, J. H. (1990) Consequences of temporal asynchrony for modulation detection interference (MDI), J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 90, Jorris, P. X. and Yin, T. C. T. (1992) Responses to amplitudemodulated tones in the auditory nerve of the cat, J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 91, Kohlrausch, A. (1993) Comment on temporal modulation transfer functions in patients with cochlear implants (J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 91, , 1992), J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 93, Levitt, H. (1971) Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics, J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 49, Moore, B. C. J., Glasberg, B. R., Gaut, T. and Child, T. (1991) Across-channel masking of changes in modulation depth for amplitude- and frequency-modulated signals, Q. J. Exp. Psychol., 43A, Moore, B. C. J. and Sek, A. (1992) Detection of combined frequency and amplitude modulation, J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 92, Moore, B. C. J. and Sek, A. (1995) Effect of carrier frequency, modulation rate, modulation waveform on the detection of modulation and the discrimination of modulation type (amplitude modulation versus frequency modulation), J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 97, Plack, C. J. and Moore, B. C. J. (1990) Temporal window shape as a function of frequency and level, J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 87, Riesz, R. R. (1928) Differential intensity sensitivity of the ear for pure tones, Phys. Rev., 31, Rodenburg, M. (1977) Investigation of temporal effects with amplitude modulated signals. In: Psychophysics and Physiology of Hearing, E. F. Evans and J. P. Wilson, eds., Academic, London, Sheft, S. and Yost, W. A. (1990) Temporal integration in amplitude modulation detection, J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 88, Schreiner, C. E. and Urbus, J. V. (1988) Representation of amplitude modulation in the auditory cortex of the cat II: Comparison between cortical fields, Hear. Res., 32, Terhardt, E. (1968) Uberdie durch amplitudenmodulierte Sinustone hervorgerufene Horempfmdung, Acoustica, 20, Viemeister, N. F. (1979) Temporal modulation transfer functions based upon modulation thresholds, J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 66, Yost, W. A. and Hafter, E. (1987) Lateralization of Simple Stimuli. In: Directional Hearing, W. A. Yost and G. Gourevitch, eds., Springer-Verlag, New York, Yost, W. A. and Sheft, S. (1989) Across-critical-band processing of amplitude-modulated tones, J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 85, Yost, W. A. and Sheft, S. (1994) Modulation detection interference: Across-spectral processing and sound source determination, Hear. Res., 79, Zwicker, E. (1952) Die grenzen der horbarkeit der amplitudenmodulation und der frequenzenmodulation eines tones, Acustica, 2, AB125-AB133. Zwicker, E. and Fasti, H. (1990) Psychoacoustics: Facts and Models, Springer-Verlag, Berlin.

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007 19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007 MODELING SPECTRAL AND TEMPORAL MASKING IN THE HUMAN AUDITORY SYSTEM PACS: 43.66.Ba, 43.66.Dc Dau, Torsten; Jepsen, Morten L.; Ewert,

More information

The role of intrinsic masker fluctuations on the spectral spread of masking

The role of intrinsic masker fluctuations on the spectral spread of masking The role of intrinsic masker fluctuations on the spectral spread of masking Steven van de Par Philips Research, Prof. Holstlaan 4, 5656 AA Eindhoven, The Netherlands, Steven.van.de.Par@philips.com, Armin

More information

Tone-in-noise detection: Observed discrepancies in spectral integration. Nicolas Le Goff a) Technische Universiteit Eindhoven, P.O.

Tone-in-noise detection: Observed discrepancies in spectral integration. Nicolas Le Goff a) Technische Universiteit Eindhoven, P.O. Tone-in-noise detection: Observed discrepancies in spectral integration Nicolas Le Goff a) Technische Universiteit Eindhoven, P.O. Box 513, NL-5600 MB Eindhoven, The Netherlands Armin Kohlrausch b) and

More information

Interaction of Object Binding Cues in Binaural Masking Pattern Experiments

Interaction of Object Binding Cues in Binaural Masking Pattern Experiments Interaction of Object Binding Cues in Binaural Masking Pattern Experiments Jesko L.Verhey, Björn Lübken and Steven van de Par Abstract Object binding cues such as binaural and across-frequency modulation

More information

Complex Sounds. Reading: Yost Ch. 4

Complex Sounds. Reading: Yost Ch. 4 Complex Sounds Reading: Yost Ch. 4 Natural Sounds Most sounds in our everyday lives are not simple sinusoidal sounds, but are complex sounds, consisting of a sum of many sinusoids. The amplitude and frequency

More information

Modeling auditory processing of amplitude modulation II. Spectral and temporal integration Dau, T.; Kollmeier, B.; Kohlrausch, A.G.

Modeling auditory processing of amplitude modulation II. Spectral and temporal integration Dau, T.; Kollmeier, B.; Kohlrausch, A.G. Modeling auditory processing of amplitude modulation II. Spectral and temporal integration Dau, T.; Kollmeier, B.; Kohlrausch, A.G. Published in: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America DOI: 10.1121/1.420345

More information

Research Note MODULATION TRANSFER FUNCTIONS: A COMPARISON OF THE RESULTS OF THREE METHODS

Research Note MODULATION TRANSFER FUNCTIONS: A COMPARISON OF THE RESULTS OF THREE METHODS Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, Volume 33, 390-397, June 1990 Research Note MODULATION TRANSFER FUNCTIONS: A COMPARISON OF THE RESULTS OF THREE METHODS DIANE M. SCOTT LARRY E. HUMES Division of

More information

Perception of pitch. Definitions. Why is pitch important? BSc Audiology/MSc SHS Psychoacoustics wk 4: 7 Feb A. Faulkner.

Perception of pitch. Definitions. Why is pitch important? BSc Audiology/MSc SHS Psychoacoustics wk 4: 7 Feb A. Faulkner. Perception of pitch BSc Audiology/MSc SHS Psychoacoustics wk 4: 7 Feb 2008. A. Faulkner. See Moore, BCJ Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing, Chapter 5. Or Plack CJ The Sense of Hearing Lawrence Erlbaum,

More information

Spectral and temporal processing in the human auditory system

Spectral and temporal processing in the human auditory system Spectral and temporal processing in the human auditory system To r s t e n Da u 1, Mo rt e n L. Jepsen 1, a n d St e p h a n D. Ew e r t 2 1Centre for Applied Hearing Research, Ørsted DTU, Technical University

More information

COM325 Computer Speech and Hearing

COM325 Computer Speech and Hearing COM325 Computer Speech and Hearing Part III : Theories and Models of Pitch Perception Dr. Guy Brown Room 145 Regent Court Department of Computer Science University of Sheffield Email: g.brown@dcs.shef.ac.uk

More information

2920 J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 102 (5), Pt. 1, November /97/102(5)/2920/5/$ Acoustical Society of America 2920

2920 J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 102 (5), Pt. 1, November /97/102(5)/2920/5/$ Acoustical Society of America 2920 Detection and discrimination of frequency glides as a function of direction, duration, frequency span, and center frequency John P. Madden and Kevin M. Fire Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders,

More information

AUDL GS08/GAV1 Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Loudness & Temporal resolution

AUDL GS08/GAV1 Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Loudness & Temporal resolution AUDL GS08/GAV1 Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear Loudness & Temporal resolution Absolute thresholds & Loudness Name some ways these concepts are crucial to audiologists Sivian & White (1933) JASA

More information

Modeling auditory processing of amplitude modulation I. Detection and masking with narrow-band carriers Dau, T.; Kollmeier, B.; Kohlrausch, A.G.

Modeling auditory processing of amplitude modulation I. Detection and masking with narrow-band carriers Dau, T.; Kollmeier, B.; Kohlrausch, A.G. Modeling auditory processing of amplitude modulation I. Detection and masking with narrow-band carriers Dau, T.; Kollmeier, B.; Kohlrausch, A.G. Published in: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America

More information

Perception of pitch. Importance of pitch: 2. mother hemp horse. scold. Definitions. Why is pitch important? AUDL4007: 11 Feb A. Faulkner.

Perception of pitch. Importance of pitch: 2. mother hemp horse. scold. Definitions. Why is pitch important? AUDL4007: 11 Feb A. Faulkner. Perception of pitch AUDL4007: 11 Feb 2010. A. Faulkner. See Moore, BCJ Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing, Chapter 5. Or Plack CJ The Sense of Hearing Lawrence Erlbaum, 2005 Chapter 7 1 Definitions

More information

Perception of pitch. Definitions. Why is pitch important? BSc Audiology/MSc SHS Psychoacoustics wk 5: 12 Feb A. Faulkner.

Perception of pitch. Definitions. Why is pitch important? BSc Audiology/MSc SHS Psychoacoustics wk 5: 12 Feb A. Faulkner. Perception of pitch BSc Audiology/MSc SHS Psychoacoustics wk 5: 12 Feb 2009. A. Faulkner. See Moore, BCJ Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing, Chapter 5. Or Plack CJ The Sense of Hearing Lawrence

More information

Acoustics, signals & systems for audiology. Week 9. Basic Psychoacoustic Phenomena: Temporal resolution

Acoustics, signals & systems for audiology. Week 9. Basic Psychoacoustic Phenomena: Temporal resolution Acoustics, signals & systems for audiology Week 9 Basic Psychoacoustic Phenomena: Temporal resolution Modulating a sinusoid carrier at 1 khz (fine structure) x modulator at 100 Hz (envelope) = amplitudemodulated

More information

Temporal resolution AUDL Domain of temporal resolution. Fine structure and envelope. Modulating a sinusoid. Fine structure and envelope

Temporal resolution AUDL Domain of temporal resolution. Fine structure and envelope. Modulating a sinusoid. Fine structure and envelope Modulating a sinusoid can also work this backwards! Temporal resolution AUDL 4007 carrier (fine structure) x modulator (envelope) = amplitudemodulated wave 1 2 Domain of temporal resolution Fine structure

More information

Results of Egan and Hake using a single sinusoidal masker [reprinted with permission from J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 22, 622 (1950)].

Results of Egan and Hake using a single sinusoidal masker [reprinted with permission from J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 22, 622 (1950)]. XVI. SIGNAL DETECTION BY HUMAN OBSERVERS Prof. J. A. Swets Prof. D. M. Green Linda E. Branneman P. D. Donahue Susan T. Sewall A. MASKING WITH TWO CONTINUOUS TONES One of the earliest studies in the modern

More information

Estimating critical bandwidths of temporal sensitivity to low-frequency amplitude modulation

Estimating critical bandwidths of temporal sensitivity to low-frequency amplitude modulation Estimating critical bandwidths of temporal sensitivity to low-frequency amplitude modulation Allison I. Shim a) and Bruce G. Berg Department of Cognitive Sciences, University of California, Irvine, Irvine,

More information

Exploiting envelope fluctuations to achieve robust extraction and intelligent integration of binaural cues

Exploiting envelope fluctuations to achieve robust extraction and intelligent integration of binaural cues The Technology of Binaural Listening & Understanding: Paper ICA216-445 Exploiting envelope fluctuations to achieve robust extraction and intelligent integration of binaural cues G. Christopher Stecker

More information

Distortion products and the perceived pitch of harmonic complex tones

Distortion products and the perceived pitch of harmonic complex tones Distortion products and the perceived pitch of harmonic complex tones D. Pressnitzer and R.D. Patterson Centre for the Neural Basis of Hearing, Dept. of Physiology, Downing street, Cambridge CB2 3EG, U.K.

More information

THE MATLAB IMPLEMENTATION OF BINAURAL PROCESSING MODEL SIMULATING LATERAL POSITION OF TONES WITH INTERAURAL TIME DIFFERENCES

THE MATLAB IMPLEMENTATION OF BINAURAL PROCESSING MODEL SIMULATING LATERAL POSITION OF TONES WITH INTERAURAL TIME DIFFERENCES THE MATLAB IMPLEMENTATION OF BINAURAL PROCESSING MODEL SIMULATING LATERAL POSITION OF TONES WITH INTERAURAL TIME DIFFERENCES J. Bouše, V. Vencovský Department of Radioelectronics, Faculty of Electrical

More information

A cat's cocktail party: Psychophysical, neurophysiological, and computational studies of spatial release from masking

A cat's cocktail party: Psychophysical, neurophysiological, and computational studies of spatial release from masking A cat's cocktail party: Psychophysical, neurophysiological, and computational studies of spatial release from masking Courtney C. Lane 1, Norbert Kopco 2, Bertrand Delgutte 1, Barbara G. Shinn- Cunningham

More information

Spectro-Temporal Methods in Primary Auditory Cortex David Klein Didier Depireux Jonathan Simon Shihab Shamma

Spectro-Temporal Methods in Primary Auditory Cortex David Klein Didier Depireux Jonathan Simon Shihab Shamma Spectro-Temporal Methods in Primary Auditory Cortex David Klein Didier Depireux Jonathan Simon Shihab Shamma & Department of Electrical Engineering Supported in part by a MURI grant from the Office of

More information

A CLOSER LOOK AT THE REPRESENTATION OF INTERAURAL DIFFERENCES IN A BINAURAL MODEL

A CLOSER LOOK AT THE REPRESENTATION OF INTERAURAL DIFFERENCES IN A BINAURAL MODEL 9th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, -7 SEPTEMBER 7 A CLOSER LOOK AT THE REPRESENTATION OF INTERAURAL DIFFERENCES IN A BINAURAL MODEL PACS: PACS:. Pn Nicolas Le Goff ; Armin Kohlrausch ; Jeroen

More information

THE PERCEPTION OF ALL-PASS COMPONENTS IN TRANSFER FUNCTIONS

THE PERCEPTION OF ALL-PASS COMPONENTS IN TRANSFER FUNCTIONS PACS Reference: 43.66.Pn THE PERCEPTION OF ALL-PASS COMPONENTS IN TRANSFER FUNCTIONS Pauli Minnaar; Jan Plogsties; Søren Krarup Olesen; Flemming Christensen; Henrik Møller Department of Acoustics Aalborg

More information

Binaural Mechanisms that Emphasize Consistent Interaural Timing Information over Frequency

Binaural Mechanisms that Emphasize Consistent Interaural Timing Information over Frequency Binaural Mechanisms that Emphasize Consistent Interaural Timing Information over Frequency Richard M. Stern 1 and Constantine Trahiotis 2 1 Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering and Biomedical

More information

AN AUDITORILY MOTIVATED ANALYSIS METHOD FOR ROOM IMPULSE RESPONSES

AN AUDITORILY MOTIVATED ANALYSIS METHOD FOR ROOM IMPULSE RESPONSES Proceedings of the COST G-6 Conference on Digital Audio Effects (DAFX-), Verona, Italy, December 7-9,2 AN AUDITORILY MOTIVATED ANALYSIS METHOD FOR ROOM IMPULSE RESPONSES Tapio Lokki Telecommunications

More information

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics Volume 19, 2013 http://acousticalsociety.org/ ICA 2013 Montreal Montreal, Canada 2-7 June 2013 Psychological and Physiological Acoustics Session 1pPPb: Psychoacoustics

More information

Psycho-acoustics (Sound characteristics, Masking, and Loudness)

Psycho-acoustics (Sound characteristics, Masking, and Loudness) Psycho-acoustics (Sound characteristics, Masking, and Loudness) Tai-Shih Chi ( 冀泰石 ) Department of Communication Engineering National Chiao Tung University Mar. 20, 2008 Pure tones Mathematics of the pure

More information

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007 AUDITORY EVOKED MAGNETIC FIELDS AND LOUDNESS IN RELATION TO BANDPASS NOISES

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007 AUDITORY EVOKED MAGNETIC FIELDS AND LOUDNESS IN RELATION TO BANDPASS NOISES 19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007 AUDITORY EVOKED MAGNETIC FIELDS AND LOUDNESS IN RELATION TO BANDPASS NOISES PACS: 43.64.Ri Yoshiharu Soeta; Seiji Nakagawa 1 National

More information

Phase and Feedback in the Nonlinear Brain. Malcolm Slaney (IBM and Stanford) Hiroko Shiraiwa-Terasawa (Stanford) Regaip Sen (Stanford)

Phase and Feedback in the Nonlinear Brain. Malcolm Slaney (IBM and Stanford) Hiroko Shiraiwa-Terasawa (Stanford) Regaip Sen (Stanford) Phase and Feedback in the Nonlinear Brain Malcolm Slaney (IBM and Stanford) Hiroko Shiraiwa-Terasawa (Stanford) Regaip Sen (Stanford) Auditory processing pre-cosyne workshop March 23, 2004 Simplistic Models

More information

INTRODUCTION. Address and author to whom correspondence should be addressed. Electronic mail:

INTRODUCTION. Address and author to whom correspondence should be addressed. Electronic mail: Detection of time- and bandlimited increments and decrements in a random-level noise Michael G. Heinz Speech and Hearing Sciences Program, Division of Health Sciences and Technology, Massachusetts Institute

More information

The role of distortion products in masking by single bands of noise Heijden, van der, M.L.; Kohlrausch, A.G.

The role of distortion products in masking by single bands of noise Heijden, van der, M.L.; Kohlrausch, A.G. The role of distortion products in masking by single bands of noise Heijden, van der, M.L.; Kohlrausch, A.G. Published in: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America DOI: 10.1121/1.413801 Published:

More information

Effect of fast-acting compression on modulation detection interference for normal hearing and hearing impaired listeners

Effect of fast-acting compression on modulation detection interference for normal hearing and hearing impaired listeners Effect of fast-acting compression on modulation detection interference for normal hearing and hearing impaired listeners Yi Shen a and Jennifer J. Lentz Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, Indiana

More information

III. Publication III. c 2005 Toni Hirvonen.

III. Publication III. c 2005 Toni Hirvonen. III Publication III Hirvonen, T., Segregation of Two Simultaneously Arriving Narrowband Noise Signals as a Function of Spatial and Frequency Separation, in Proceedings of th International Conference on

More information

DETERMINATION OF EQUAL-LOUDNESS RELATIONS AT HIGH FREQUENCIES

DETERMINATION OF EQUAL-LOUDNESS RELATIONS AT HIGH FREQUENCIES DETERMINATION OF EQUAL-LOUDNESS RELATIONS AT HIGH FREQUENCIES Rhona Hellman 1, Hisashi Takeshima 2, Yo^iti Suzuki 3, Kenji Ozawa 4, and Toshio Sone 5 1 Department of Psychology and Institute for Hearing,

More information

An unnatural test of a natural model of pitch perception: The tritone paradox and spectral dominance

An unnatural test of a natural model of pitch perception: The tritone paradox and spectral dominance An unnatural test of a natural model of pitch perception: The tritone paradox and spectral dominance Richard PARNCUTT, University of Graz Amos Ping TAN, Universal Music, Singapore Octave-complex tone (OCT)

More information

Monaural and binaural processing of fluctuating sounds in the auditory system

Monaural and binaural processing of fluctuating sounds in the auditory system Monaural and binaural processing of fluctuating sounds in the auditory system Eric R. Thompson September 23, 2005 MSc Thesis Acoustic Technology Ørsted DTU Technical University of Denmark Supervisor: Torsten

More information

ANALYSIS AND EVALUATION OF IRREGULARITY IN PITCH VIBRATO FOR STRING-INSTRUMENT TONES

ANALYSIS AND EVALUATION OF IRREGULARITY IN PITCH VIBRATO FOR STRING-INSTRUMENT TONES Abstract ANALYSIS AND EVALUATION OF IRREGULARITY IN PITCH VIBRATO FOR STRING-INSTRUMENT TONES William L. Martens Faculty of Architecture, Design and Planning University of Sydney, Sydney NSW 2006, Australia

More information

AUDL GS08/GAV1 Auditory Perception. Envelope and temporal fine structure (TFS)

AUDL GS08/GAV1 Auditory Perception. Envelope and temporal fine structure (TFS) AUDL GS08/GAV1 Auditory Perception Envelope and temporal fine structure (TFS) Envelope and TFS arise from a method of decomposing waveforms The classic decomposition of waveforms Spectral analysis... Decomposes

More information

A Pole Zero Filter Cascade Provides Good Fits to Human Masking Data and to Basilar Membrane and Neural Data

A Pole Zero Filter Cascade Provides Good Fits to Human Masking Data and to Basilar Membrane and Neural Data A Pole Zero Filter Cascade Provides Good Fits to Human Masking Data and to Basilar Membrane and Neural Data Richard F. Lyon Google, Inc. Abstract. A cascade of two-pole two-zero filters with level-dependent

More information

Document Version Publisher s PDF, also known as Version of Record (includes final page, issue and volume numbers)

Document Version Publisher s PDF, also known as Version of Record (includes final page, issue and volume numbers) A quantitative model of the 'effective' signal processing in the auditory system. II. Simulations and measurements Dau, T.; Püschel, D.; Kohlrausch, A.G. Published in: Journal of the Acoustical Society

More information

Perception of low frequencies in small rooms

Perception of low frequencies in small rooms Perception of low frequencies in small rooms Fazenda, BM and Avis, MR Title Authors Type URL Published Date 24 Perception of low frequencies in small rooms Fazenda, BM and Avis, MR Conference or Workshop

More information

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE Copyright SFA - InterNoise 2000 1 inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering 27-30 August 2000, Nice, FRANCE I-INCE Classification: 6.1 AUDIBILITY OF COMPLEX

More information

Technical University of Denmark

Technical University of Denmark Technical University of Denmark Masking 1 st semester project Ørsted DTU Acoustic Technology fall 2007 Group 6 Troels Schmidt Lindgreen 073081 Kristoffer Ahrens Dickow 071324 Reynir Hilmisson 060162 Instructor

More information

Preface A detailed knowledge of the processes involved in hearing is an essential prerequisite for numerous medical and technical applications, such a

Preface A detailed knowledge of the processes involved in hearing is an essential prerequisite for numerous medical and technical applications, such a Modeling auditory processing of amplitude modulation Torsten Dau Preface A detailed knowledge of the processes involved in hearing is an essential prerequisite for numerous medical and technical applications,

More information

The effect of noise fluctuation and spectral bandwidth on gap detection

The effect of noise fluctuation and spectral bandwidth on gap detection The effect of noise fluctuation and spectral bandwidth on gap detection Joseph W. Hall III, 1,a) Emily Buss, 1 Erol J. Ozmeral, 2 and John H. Grose 1 1 Department of Otolaryngology Head & Neck Surgery,

More information

Hearing and Deafness 2. Ear as a frequency analyzer. Chris Darwin

Hearing and Deafness 2. Ear as a frequency analyzer. Chris Darwin Hearing and Deafness 2. Ear as a analyzer Chris Darwin Frequency: -Hz Sine Wave. Spectrum Amplitude against -..5 Time (s) Waveform Amplitude against time amp Hz Frequency: 5-Hz Sine Wave. Spectrum Amplitude

More information

A Neural Edge-Detection Model for Enhanced Auditory Sensitivity in Modulated Noise

A Neural Edge-Detection Model for Enhanced Auditory Sensitivity in Modulated Noise A Neural Edge-etection odel for Enhanced Auditory Sensitivity in odulated Noise Alon Fishbach and Bradford J. ay epartment of Biomedical Engineering and Otolaryngology-HNS Johns Hopkins University Baltimore,

More information

E ects of masker frequency and duration in forward masking: further evidence for the in uence of peripheral nonlinearity

E ects of masker frequency and duration in forward masking: further evidence for the in uence of peripheral nonlinearity Hearing Research 150 (2000) 258^266 www.elsevier.com/locate/heares E ects of masker frequency and duration in forward masking: further evidence for the in uence of peripheral nonlinearity a Andrew J. Oxenham

More information

Machine recognition of speech trained on data from New Jersey Labs

Machine recognition of speech trained on data from New Jersey Labs Machine recognition of speech trained on data from New Jersey Labs Frequency response (peak around 5 Hz) Impulse response (effective length around 200 ms) 41 RASTA filter 10 attenuation [db] 40 1 10 modulation

More information

Signals & Systems for Speech & Hearing. Week 6. Practical spectral analysis. Bandpass filters & filterbanks. Try this out on an old friend

Signals & Systems for Speech & Hearing. Week 6. Practical spectral analysis. Bandpass filters & filterbanks. Try this out on an old friend Signals & Systems for Speech & Hearing Week 6 Bandpass filters & filterbanks Practical spectral analysis Most analogue signals of interest are not easily mathematically specified so applying a Fourier

More information

Measuring the critical band for speech a)

Measuring the critical band for speech a) Measuring the critical band for speech a) Eric W. Healy b Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Arnold School of Public Health, University of South Carolina, Columbia, South Carolina 29208

More information

Physiological evidence for auditory modulation filterbanks: Cortical responses to concurrent modulations

Physiological evidence for auditory modulation filterbanks: Cortical responses to concurrent modulations Physiological evidence for auditory modulation filterbanks: Cortical responses to concurrent modulations Juanjuan Xiang a) Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, University of Maryland, College

More information

Chapter 12. Preview. Objectives The Production of Sound Waves Frequency of Sound Waves The Doppler Effect. Section 1 Sound Waves

Chapter 12. Preview. Objectives The Production of Sound Waves Frequency of Sound Waves The Doppler Effect. Section 1 Sound Waves Section 1 Sound Waves Preview Objectives The Production of Sound Waves Frequency of Sound Waves The Doppler Effect Section 1 Sound Waves Objectives Explain how sound waves are produced. Relate frequency

More information

Citation for published version (APA): Lijzenga, J. (1997). Discrimination of simplified vowel spectra Groningen: s.n.

Citation for published version (APA): Lijzenga, J. (1997). Discrimination of simplified vowel spectra Groningen: s.n. University of Groningen Discrimination of simplified vowel spectra Lijzenga, Johannes IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please

More information

Across frequency processing with time varying spectra

Across frequency processing with time varying spectra Bachelor thesis Across frequency processing with time varying spectra Handed in by Hendrike Heidemann Study course: Engineering Physics First supervisor: Prof. Dr. Jesko Verhey Second supervisor: Prof.

More information

You know about adding up waves, e.g. from two loudspeakers. AUDL 4007 Auditory Perception. Week 2½. Mathematical prelude: Adding up levels

You know about adding up waves, e.g. from two loudspeakers. AUDL 4007 Auditory Perception. Week 2½. Mathematical prelude: Adding up levels AUDL 47 Auditory Perception You know about adding up waves, e.g. from two loudspeakers Week 2½ Mathematical prelude: Adding up levels 2 But how do you get the total rms from the rms values of two signals

More information

Detection of Tones in Reproducible Noises: Prediction of Listeners Performance in Diotic and Dichotic Conditions

Detection of Tones in Reproducible Noises: Prediction of Listeners Performance in Diotic and Dichotic Conditions Detection of Tones in Reproducible Noises: Prediction of Listeners Performance in Diotic and Dichotic Conditions by Junwen Mao Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor

More information

Binaural Hearing. Reading: Yost Ch. 12

Binaural Hearing. Reading: Yost Ch. 12 Binaural Hearing Reading: Yost Ch. 12 Binaural Advantages Sounds in our environment are usually complex, and occur either simultaneously or close together in time. Studies have shown that the ability to

More information

Enhancing and unmasking the harmonics of a complex tone

Enhancing and unmasking the harmonics of a complex tone Enhancing and unmasking the harmonics of a complex tone William M. Hartmann a and Matthew J. Goupell Department of Physics and Astronomy, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan 48824 Received

More information

Influence of fine structure and envelope variability on gap-duration discrimination thresholds Münkner, S.; Kohlrausch, A.G.; Püschel, D.

Influence of fine structure and envelope variability on gap-duration discrimination thresholds Münkner, S.; Kohlrausch, A.G.; Püschel, D. Influence of fine structure and envelope variability on gap-duration discrimination thresholds Münkner, S.; Kohlrausch, A.G.; Püschel, D. Published in: Journal of the Acoustical Society of America DOI:

More information

AUDITORY ILLUSIONS & LAB REPORT FORM

AUDITORY ILLUSIONS & LAB REPORT FORM 01/02 Illusions - 1 AUDITORY ILLUSIONS & LAB REPORT FORM NAME: DATE: PARTNER(S): The objective of this experiment is: To understand concepts such as beats, localization, masking, and musical effects. APPARATUS:

More information

An auditory model that can account for frequency selectivity and phase effects on masking

An auditory model that can account for frequency selectivity and phase effects on masking Acoust. Sci. & Tech. 2, (24) PAPER An auditory model that can account for frequency selectivity and phase effects on masking Akira Nishimura 1; 1 Department of Media and Cultural Studies, Faculty of Informatics,

More information

Signals, Sound, and Sensation

Signals, Sound, and Sensation Signals, Sound, and Sensation William M. Hartmann Department of Physics and Astronomy Michigan State University East Lansing, Michigan Л1Р Contents Preface xv Chapter 1: Pure Tones 1 Mathematics of the

More information

FFT 1 /n octave analysis wavelet

FFT 1 /n octave analysis wavelet 06/16 For most acoustic examinations, a simple sound level analysis is insufficient, as not only the overall sound pressure level, but also the frequency-dependent distribution of the level has a significant

More information

Preeti Rao 2 nd CompMusicWorkshop, Istanbul 2012

Preeti Rao 2 nd CompMusicWorkshop, Istanbul 2012 Preeti Rao 2 nd CompMusicWorkshop, Istanbul 2012 o Music signal characteristics o Perceptual attributes and acoustic properties o Signal representations for pitch detection o STFT o Sinusoidal model o

More information

Pattern Recognition. Part 6: Bandwidth Extension. Gerhard Schmidt

Pattern Recognition. Part 6: Bandwidth Extension. Gerhard Schmidt Pattern Recognition Part 6: Gerhard Schmidt Christian-Albrechts-Universität zu Kiel Faculty of Engineering Institute of Electrical and Information Engineering Digital Signal Processing and System Theory

More information

The role of fine structure in bilateral cochlear implantation

The role of fine structure in bilateral cochlear implantation Acoustics Research Institute Austrian Academy of Sciences The role of fine structure in bilateral cochlear implantation Laback, B., Majdak, P., Baumgartner, W. D. Interaural Time Difference (ITD) Sound

More information

MUS 302 ENGINEERING SECTION

MUS 302 ENGINEERING SECTION MUS 302 ENGINEERING SECTION Wiley Ross: Recording Studio Coordinator Email =>ross@email.arizona.edu Twitter=> https://twitter.com/ssor Web page => http://www.arts.arizona.edu/studio Youtube Channel=>http://www.youtube.com/user/wileyross

More information

I. INTRODUCTION. NL-5656 AA Eindhoven, The Netherlands. Electronic mail:

I. INTRODUCTION. NL-5656 AA Eindhoven, The Netherlands. Electronic mail: Binaural processing model based on contralateral inhibition. II. Dependence on spectral parameters Jeroen Breebaart a) IPO, Center for User System Interaction, P.O. Box 513, NL-5600 MB Eindhoven, The Netherlands

More information

ABSTRACT. Title of Document: SPECTROTEMPORAL MODULATION LISTENERS. Professor, Dr.Shihab Shamma, Department of. Electrical Engineering

ABSTRACT. Title of Document: SPECTROTEMPORAL MODULATION LISTENERS. Professor, Dr.Shihab Shamma, Department of. Electrical Engineering ABSTRACT Title of Document: SPECTROTEMPORAL MODULATION SENSITIVITY IN HEARING-IMPAIRED LISTENERS Golbarg Mehraei, Master of Science, 29 Directed By: Professor, Dr.Shihab Shamma, Department of Electrical

More information

Acoustics, signals & systems for audiology. Week 4. Signals through Systems

Acoustics, signals & systems for audiology. Week 4. Signals through Systems Acoustics, signals & systems for audiology Week 4 Signals through Systems Crucial ideas Any signal can be constructed as a sum of sine waves In a linear time-invariant (LTI) system, the response to a sinusoid

More information

Introduction to cochlear implants Philipos C. Loizou Figure Captions

Introduction to cochlear implants Philipos C. Loizou Figure Captions http://www.utdallas.edu/~loizou/cimplants/tutorial/ Introduction to cochlear implants Philipos C. Loizou Figure Captions Figure 1. The top panel shows the time waveform of a 30-msec segment of the vowel

More information

Modulation analysis in ArtemiS SUITE 1

Modulation analysis in ArtemiS SUITE 1 02/18 in ArtemiS SUITE 1 of ArtemiS SUITE delivers the envelope spectra of partial bands of an analyzed signal. This allows to determine the frequency, strength and change over time of amplitude modulations

More information

Modelling the sensation of fluctuation strength

Modelling the sensation of fluctuation strength Product Sound Quality and Multimodal Interaction: Paper ICA016-113 Modelling the sensation of fluctuation strength Alejandro Osses Vecchi (a), Rodrigo García León (a), Armin Kohlrausch (a,b) (a) Human-Technology

More information

Neural Processing of Amplitude-Modulated Sounds: Joris, Schreiner and Rees, Physiol. Rev. 2004

Neural Processing of Amplitude-Modulated Sounds: Joris, Schreiner and Rees, Physiol. Rev. 2004 Neural Processing of Amplitude-Modulated Sounds: Joris, Schreiner and Rees, Physiol. Rev. 2004 Richard Turner (turner@gatsby.ucl.ac.uk) Gatsby Computational Neuroscience Unit, 02/03/2006 As neuroscientists

More information

ECMA TR/105. A Shaped Noise File Representative of Speech. 1 st Edition / December Reference number ECMA TR/12:2009

ECMA TR/105. A Shaped Noise File Representative of Speech. 1 st Edition / December Reference number ECMA TR/12:2009 ECMA TR/105 1 st Edition / December 2012 A Shaped Noise File Representative of Speech Reference number ECMA TR/12:2009 Ecma International 2009 COPYRIGHT PROTECTED DOCUMENT Ecma International 2012 Contents

More information

The Effect of Frequency Shifting on Audio-Tactile Conversion for Enriching Musical Experience

The Effect of Frequency Shifting on Audio-Tactile Conversion for Enriching Musical Experience The Effect of Frequency Shifting on Audio-Tactile Conversion for Enriching Musical Experience Ryuta Okazaki 1,2, Hidenori Kuribayashi 3, Hiroyuki Kajimioto 1,4 1 The University of Electro-Communications,

More information

Structure of Speech. Physical acoustics Time-domain representation Frequency domain representation Sound shaping

Structure of Speech. Physical acoustics Time-domain representation Frequency domain representation Sound shaping Structure of Speech Physical acoustics Time-domain representation Frequency domain representation Sound shaping Speech acoustics Source-Filter Theory Speech Source characteristics Speech Filter characteristics

More information

Human Auditory Periphery (HAP)

Human Auditory Periphery (HAP) Human Auditory Periphery (HAP) Ray Meddis Department of Human Sciences, University of Essex Colchester, CO4 3SQ, UK. rmeddis@essex.ac.uk A demonstrator for a human auditory modelling approach. 23/11/2003

More information

Psychoacoustic Cues in Room Size Perception

Psychoacoustic Cues in Room Size Perception Audio Engineering Society Convention Paper Presented at the 116th Convention 2004 May 8 11 Berlin, Germany 6084 This convention paper has been reproduced from the author s advance manuscript, without editing,

More information

Intensity Discrimination and Binaural Interaction

Intensity Discrimination and Binaural Interaction Technical University of Denmark Intensity Discrimination and Binaural Interaction 2 nd semester project DTU Electrical Engineering Acoustic Technology Spring semester 2008 Group 5 Troels Schmidt Lindgreen

More information

The psychoacoustics of reverberation

The psychoacoustics of reverberation The psychoacoustics of reverberation Steven van de Par Steven.van.de.Par@uni-oldenburg.de July 19, 2016 Thanks to Julian Grosse and Andreas Häußler 2016 AES International Conference on Sound Field Control

More information

Measurement of the binaural auditory filter using a detection task

Measurement of the binaural auditory filter using a detection task Measurement of the binaural auditory filter using a detection task Andrew J. Kolarik and John F. Culling School of Psychology, Cardiff University, Tower Building, Park Place, Cardiff CF1 3AT, United Kingdom

More information

Predicting discrimination of formant frequencies in vowels with a computational model of the auditory midbrain

Predicting discrimination of formant frequencies in vowels with a computational model of the auditory midbrain F 1 Predicting discrimination of formant frequencies in vowels with a computational model of the auditory midbrain Laurel H. Carney and Joyce M. McDonough Abstract Neural information for encoding and processing

More information

SOUND QUALITY EVALUATION OF FAN NOISE BASED ON HEARING-RELATED PARAMETERS SUMMARY INTRODUCTION

SOUND QUALITY EVALUATION OF FAN NOISE BASED ON HEARING-RELATED PARAMETERS SUMMARY INTRODUCTION SOUND QUALITY EVALUATION OF FAN NOISE BASED ON HEARING-RELATED PARAMETERS Roland SOTTEK, Klaus GENUIT HEAD acoustics GmbH, Ebertstr. 30a 52134 Herzogenrath, GERMANY SUMMARY Sound quality evaluation of

More information

Predictions of diotic tone-in-noise detection based on a nonlinear optimal combination of energy, envelope, and fine-structure cues

Predictions of diotic tone-in-noise detection based on a nonlinear optimal combination of energy, envelope, and fine-structure cues Predictions of diotic tone-in-noise detection based on a nonlinear optimal combination of energy, envelope, and fine-structure cues Junwen Mao Department of Electrical and Computer Engineering, University

More information

HCS 7367 Speech Perception

HCS 7367 Speech Perception HCS 7367 Speech Perception Dr. Peter Assmann Fall 212 Power spectrum model of masking Assumptions: Only frequencies within the passband of the auditory filter contribute to masking. Detection is based

More information

Modulation frequency and orientation tuning of second-order texture mechanisms

Modulation frequency and orientation tuning of second-order texture mechanisms Arsenault et al. Vol. 16, No. 3/March 1999/J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 427 Modulation frequency and orientation tuning of second-order texture mechanisms A. Serge Arsenault and Frances Wilkinson Department of Psychology,

More information

A psychoacoustic-masking model to predict the perception of speech-like stimuli in noise q

A psychoacoustic-masking model to predict the perception of speech-like stimuli in noise q Speech Communication 40 (2003) 291 313 www.elsevier.com/locate/specom A psychoacoustic-masking model to predict the perception of speech-like stimuli in noise q James J. Hant *, Abeer Alwan Speech Processing

More information

Experiments in two-tone interference

Experiments in two-tone interference Experiments in two-tone interference Using zero-based encoding An alternative look at combination tones and the critical band John K. Bates Time/Space Systems Functions of the experimental system: Variable

More information

Standard Octaves and Sound Pressure. The superposition of several independent sound sources produces multifrequency noise: i=1

Standard Octaves and Sound Pressure. The superposition of several independent sound sources produces multifrequency noise: i=1 Appendix C Standard Octaves and Sound Pressure C.1 Time History and Overall Sound Pressure The superposition of several independent sound sources produces multifrequency noise: p(t) = N N p i (t) = P i

More information

Imperfect pitch: Gabor s uncertainty principle and the pitch of extremely brief sounds

Imperfect pitch: Gabor s uncertainty principle and the pitch of extremely brief sounds Psychon Bull Rev (2016) 23:163 171 DOI 10.3758/s13423-015-0863-y BRIEF REPORT Imperfect pitch: Gabor s uncertainty principle and the pitch of extremely brief sounds I-Hui Hsieh 1 & Kourosh Saberi 2 Published

More information

An introduction to physics of Sound

An introduction to physics of Sound An introduction to physics of Sound Outlines Acoustics and psycho-acoustics Sound? Wave and waves types Cycle Basic parameters of sound wave period Amplitude Wavelength Frequency Outlines Phase Types of

More information

I. INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 110 (3), Pt. 1, Sep /2001/110(3)/1628/13/$ Acoustical Society of America

I. INTRODUCTION J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 110 (3), Pt. 1, Sep /2001/110(3)/1628/13/$ Acoustical Society of America On the upper cutoff frequency of the auditory critical-band envelope detectors in the context of speech perception a) Oded Ghitza Media Signal Processing Research, Agere Systems, Murray Hill, New Jersey

More information

Communication Engineering Prof. Surendra Prasad Department of Electrical Engineering Indian Institute of Technology, Delhi

Communication Engineering Prof. Surendra Prasad Department of Electrical Engineering Indian Institute of Technology, Delhi Communication Engineering Prof. Surendra Prasad Department of Electrical Engineering Indian Institute of Technology, Delhi Lecture - 16 Angle Modulation (Contd.) We will continue our discussion on Angle

More information

Pre- and Post Ringing Of Impulse Response

Pre- and Post Ringing Of Impulse Response Pre- and Post Ringing Of Impulse Response Source: http://zone.ni.com/reference/en-xx/help/373398b-01/svaconcepts/svtimemask/ Time (Temporal) Masking.Simultaneous masking describes the effect when the masked

More information

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics Volume 19, 2013 http://acousticalsociety.org/ ICA 2013 Montreal Montreal, Canada 2-7 June 2013 Psychological and Physiological Acoustics Session 3pPP: Multimodal Influences

More information