Influence of visual constraints in the trajectory formation of grasping movements

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Influence of visual constraints in the trajectory formation of grasping movements"

Transcription

1 Neuroscience Letters 401 (2006) Influence of visual constraints in the trajectory formation of grasping movements R. Palluel-Germain a,, F. Boy a, J.P. Orliaguet a, Y. Coello b a Laboratoire de Psychologie et NeuroCognition (LPNC), CNRS UMR 5105, Université Pierre Mendès France, Grenoble II, 1251, Avenue Centrale BP 47, Grenoble Cedex 9, France b Laboratoire URECA, Université Charles de Gaulle, Lille, France Received 18 November 2005; received in revised form 8 February 2006; accepted 24 February 2006 Abstract The main objective of the present study is to show that the visual context can influence the trajectory formation of grasping movements. We asked participants to reach and grasp a cylinder disposed at three different positions: 20, 0 and 20 of eccentricity with respect to the midsagittal axis. Grasping movements were performed in a direct and in an indirect visual feedback condition (i.e., controlled through a vertical video display). Results revealed that for grasping movements directed toward objects located at 20 and 0, path curvatures of the wrist, the thumb and the index finger were significantly straighter in the indirect visual feedback condition. However, no significant difference concerning hand path curvature was observed when the movement was directed toward the object located at 20. This suggests that grasping movements controlled through a remote visual feedback tend to be planned in extrinsic space and that the effect of the visual context on movement planning appears to be not isotropic over the workspace Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Natural grasp; Motor planning; Visual feedback; Video-controlled movements; Context effect The paths taken by the hand in goal-directed movements are generally viewed as containing information about how movements are planned and controlled (for a review see [4]). For instance, considering pointing movements, it has been shown that hand paths were roughly straight whatever the direction of the movement [16]. This extrinsic stability suggests that manual goal-directed movements are coded as a displacement along a straight line between the initial position of the hand and the goal of the movement (extrinsic space planning) and that the appropriate covariation of the joint angles is specified at some later stage [7,23,26,27]. Alternatively, some other studies observed that the shape of the hand movement varied as a function of the movement direction, thus implying that the movement is not programmed to follow a straight line in the extrinsic space [1,5,14,17]. This last result is compatible with the idea that the motor system plans movement in joint coordinates (intrinsic space planning). Movements in extrinsic space are therefore curved simply as a result of the non-linear relation between cartesian coordinates and joint angle coordinates [1]. Corresponding author. Tel.: ; fax: address: richard.palluel@upmf-grenoble.fr (R. Palluel-Germain). These controversial data were reconciled by studies showing that the features of a trajectory could depend on the execution constraints imposed to the movement. Desmurget et al. [3], for instance, observed that planar (2D) pointings evidenced straight hand paths whereas trajectory curvature of three-dimensional (3D) pointings depended on movement direction. This suggests that planar movements are planned in extrinsic space and that differently, 3D movements are planned in intrinsic space. Interestingly, Palluel-Germain et al. [18] showed recently that not only physical constraints but also the visual context can affect trajectory formation. Indeed, three-dimensional pointing movements executed in a situation of remote visual control (i.e., with the use of a video-system) exhibited roughly straight paths whatever the movement direction. The authors then concluded that in this particular situation subjects estimated the desired trajectory by using hand and target position displayed on the screen, with the consequence that reaching movements are planned and controlled in extrinsic space. Therefore, it seems that in a remote control situation, the constraints arising from the visual feedback surpass the biomechanical constraints thereby leading to modifications in the shape of the hand displacement. The present study was designed to test this hypothesis by analysing grasping movements i.e., /$ see front matter 2006 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved. doi: /j.neulet

2 98 R. Palluel-Germain et al. / Neuroscience Letters 401 (2006) movements in which the hand path is strongly compelled by both task and biomechanical constraints. Indeed, several studies [5,6,10,19,22] showed that the trajectories of grasping movements are influenced, at least in part, by the final posture reached by the arm with respect to the properties of the object (localisation, orientation, shape). For example, when subjects were asked to reach and grasp cylindrical objects placed at different locations, Paulignan et al. [19] reported that the configuration reached by the arm was stereotyped for a given position of the object. More specifically the hand trajectory exhibited curved path that changed significantly as a function of the object position. Such results suggest that the hand path, in grasping movements, is widely determined by the constraints of the task [5,10,19,22,24]. Therefore, if goal-directed movements carried out in an indirect visual feedback situation tend to be planned and controlled in extrinsic space, we expect that even grasping movements should exhibit roughly straight hand paths whatever the object s position. Moreover, as the visual feedback is available during the entire movement, it is thus difficult to disentangle whether the observed modifications of the path shape result from the influence of either planning or feedback control processes. Consequently, to determine the part played by planning processes, the shape of the hand trajectory was assessed right at the start of the movement. In order to test these hypotheses, we asked participants to carry out grasping movements in two conditions of visual feedback: either under an indirect visual feedback (i.e., controlled through a vertical video-screen) or in a direct visual feedback condition. Moreover, movements were executed toward objects placed at different locations. After providing informed consent, eight subjects (18 25 years) participated in this experiment. All subjects were right handed, and naïve as to the purpose of the experiment. None of them reported any sensory or motor deficits, and all had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. The apparatus is shown in Fig. 1. Subjects sat in front of an untextured white table positioned slightly above the navel and maintained their trunk straight. Each trial started by placing the tip of the thumb and index finger in contact on the top of a support (1 cm diameter and 5 cm in height) aligned with the midsagittal plane. Subjects were instructed to hold the remaining fingers clenched into the palm and to keep the lower arm and the hand on the table surface. In this configuration, the thumb and the index finger were approximately at 15 cm from the body. After a go instruction from the experimenter, subjects were asked to reach and grasp a cylinder (diameter 2.5 cm, height 1.5 cm) disposed one at a time at three fixed locations: 20 on the left of subject s sagittal axis ( 20 ), 0 (along the midsagittal axis) and 20 on the right (see Fig. 1). The distance between the starting position and the object locations was 25 cm. Subjects were instructed to pick up the cylinder with the thumb and the index finger. No instruction was given as concern the movement speed, the movement path and the contact positions of the digits on the cylinder. Having grasped the cylinder, subjects lifted it a few centimetres off the table. For every session the subject s left hand remained on his/her left thigh. Each subject performed grasping movements in an indirect visual feedback condition (IVF) and Fig. 1. Experimental conditions. (A) Indirect visual feedback condition. Cylinder, hand position and movement were visible on the video screen. An opaque cloth suspended between the screen and the subject prevented the direct vision of the hand. (B) Direct visual feedback condition. The cylinder was directly visible in the workspace. Subjects began each trial by positioning the tip of the thumb and index finger in contact on the top of a support (1 cm diameter and 5 cm in height) aligned with the midsagittal plane. Subjects were asked to reach and grasp a cylinder (diameter 2.5 cm, height 1.5 cm) disposed at 25 cm from the starting position. Three objects locations were used: 0 (plain circles), 20 and 20 (dotted circles) centred on the subject s body axis. in a direct visual feedback condition (DVF). In the IVF condition participants were seated in front of a colour video screen (0.55 m 0.40 m) located approximately 0.50 m from the head. A Sony video camera placed 1.10 m above the table recorded arm displacement (1:1 spatial relationship) and transmitted in real time movement images on the screen. The starting position of the hand, the cylinder location and the displacement of the hand were continuously available on the video screen. On the video display the hand starting position and the cylinder located at 0 were in alignment with the subject s midsagittal axis. An opaque cloth placed between the screen and the subject prevented the direct vision of the hand. In the DVF condition participants performed the same task in the absence of the occluder

3 R. Palluel-Germain et al. / Neuroscience Letters 401 (2006) (see Fig. 1B). The IVF and the DVF conditions were performed in two independent sessions that were counterbalanced across participants. In each experimental condition the 30 movements (3 object positions 10 trials) were carried out in a random order. The positional data of the movements were recorded using the CMS movement registration system (manufacturer: ZEBRIS, Isny, Germany). In this measuring system the positions of miniature ultrasonic transmitters were sampled at a frequency of 100 Hz at a spatial resolution of about 0.1 mm. Two markers were stuck on the tips of the thumb and index finger, respectively. Another one was placed on the radius process of the wrist. Positional data were filtered using a second-order Butterworth dual-pass filter (cut-off frequency: 10 Hz). Movement onset was defined as the time when the thumb speed first exceeded 3 cm/s. The movement endpoint was determined visually on the curve of the thumb and index finger distance, as the point where this distance stopped decreasing (i.e., the time when the fingers came into contact with the object). Therefore, the period following the contact with the object, during which the cylinder was lifted, was not considered. In order to assess movement curvature we first calculated the Path Curvature Index (PCI) [1]. For each trial, the maximum distance between the trajectory and the straight line joining the starting- and the end-point was divided by the amplitude of the movement. Therefore, this measure increases with curvature (a straight line would have a null PCI value). To calculate average paths for each marker position and each condition, the starting position of each trajectory was first realigned with the mean starting position. The trajectories were then resampled at 50 evenly spaced points over the duration of the movement. Finally, averaging these resampled trajectories allowed to compute mean trajectories. We also calculated the maximum distance (MD) between the trajectory and the straight line joining the startingand the end-point (negative value for deviations to the left of the straight line). In addition, in order to determine whether a difference in curvature was present at the beginning of the movement, we measured the distance between the path and the straight line 150 ms after movement onset. Finally, movement time was analysed. For the movement time (MT) we performed a two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with repeated measures on the visual feedback (IVF and DVF) and the object position ( 20, 0 and 20 ). For the other parameters a two within-subject (visual feedback conditions) 3 (marker positions) 3 (object positions) ANOVA was conducted. For each analysis a significance level of 0.05 was chosen. For comparison of each marker a Tukey HSD test was used as a post hoc test. Fig. 2B shows the average hand paths for each condition. Each trajectory represents the average of 10 replications for all 8 subjects (80 paths). We can observe that hand-paths tend to be straighter in the IVF than in the DVF condition only for movements executed toward the 20 and the 0 directions. The statistical analyses confirm this observation. PCI was less pronounced in IVF than in DVF condition, F(1, 7) = 11.38, p < There was also a significant main effect of the object position, F(2, 14) = 17.01, p < 0.001, and a significant interaction between the object position and the visual feedback situation, F(2, 14) = 16.91, p < To investigate this interaction further, we then used simple effects analysis to compare the two visual feedback conditions for each object position and each marker (see Fig. 2A). When the object was at 20, a significant difference between the two visual feedback conditions was observed for the three marker positions: trajectories were straighter in the IVF condition (wrist: F(1, 7) = 39.81, p < 0.001; thumb: F(1, 7) = 24.47, p < 0.01; index finger: F(1, 7) = 25.98, p < 0.01). The same effects were found for movements directed at the object located at 0 (wrist: F(1, 7) = 10.49, p < 0.02; thumb: F(1, 7) = 8.29, p < 0.03; index finger: F(1, 7) = 6.35, p < 0.04). Conversely, when the object was positioned at 20, differences between IVF and DVF conditions were not significant whatever the marker position (wrist: F(1, 7) = 1.90, p > 0.05; thumb: F(1, 7) = 2.80, p > 0.05; index finger: F(1, 7) = 1.78, p > 0.05). The post hoc analysis revealed that in each condition of visual feedback, trajectories of thumb and index finger were straighter than the trajectory of the wrist. No significant difference was noted between trajectory curvatures of thumb and index finger. We can notice that, when the object was positioned at 20, the maximum distance (MD) between the trajectory and the straight line joining the starting and the end point was on average 10 and 39 mm for the IVF and DVF conditions, respectively, F(1, 7) = 17.20, p < For movements directed at the object located at 0, MD was on average 7 mm for the IVF condition and 28 mm for the DVF condition, F(1, 7) = 8.71, p < Conversely, no significant difference was noted between IVF and DVF condition when the object was positioned at 20 : MD was on average 19 mm for the IVF condition and 28 mm for the DVF condition, F(1, 7) = 1.43, p > Therefore, as observed in Fig. 2A, in the IVF condition trajectories tended to be curved leftward with respect to the sagittal axis when the object was positioned at 20, whereas they tended to be curved rightward when the object was positioned at 0 and at 20. The effect of the visual feedback on trajectory curvature was present from the beginning of the movement. The perpendicular distance of paths from a straight line connecting start- and endpoints 150 ms after movement onset was indeed less important in the IVF condition when the object was positioned at 20 (wrist: F(1, 7) = 9.25, p < 0.02; thumb: F(1, 7) = 9.87, p < 0.02; index finger: F(1, 7) = 10.44, p < 0.02) and at 0 (wrist: F(1, 7) = 7.30, p < 0.05; thumb: F(1, 7) = 7.18, p < 0.05; index finger: F(1, 7) = 6.43, p < 0.01). No significant difference was observed for the object located at 20 (all ps > 0.05). As concerned MT no significant difference was observed between the IVF (1171 ms) and the DVF conditions (892 ms), F(1, 7) = 3.38, p > 0.05 and between the object positions, F(2, 14) = 1.23, p > A significant interaction was noted between the object position and the visual feedback, F(2, 14) = 4.29, p < The analysis of the simple effects showed that MT was longer in the IVF situation for the 20 position (IVF: 1241 ms, DVF: 876 ms, F(1, 7) = 6.49, p < 0.05). No significant difference was observed for the 0 position (IVF: 1180 ms, DVF: 882 ms, F(1, 7) = 2.47, p > 0.05) and for the 20 position (IVF: 1091 ms, DVF: 918 ms, F(1, 7) = 1.77, p > 0.05). However, we can notice that concerning the 0 position, the standard deviation was 450 ms in the IVF condition: for two subjects the mean

4 100 R. Palluel-Germain et al. / Neuroscience Letters 401 (2006) Fig. 2. Hand path curvature. (A) Mean hand path curvature and standard deviations according to the visual feedback, the object position and the marker position. (B) Mean normalised trajectories according to the visual feedback (IVF open circles and DVF thin lines) for the wrist, the thumb and the index finger. Dotted circles figure the three different objects positions. MT was very long (2218 and 1493 ms). For the other subjects mean MT was 955 ms (MTs ranging from 766 to 1100 ms). The goal of the present experiment was to evaluate whether changing the visual context could affect the processes involved in planning grasping movements. To this aim we compared the spatial temporal characteristics of grasping movements performed either under a remote visual control condition, or in a direct visual feedback condition. Results show that the curvature of the hand path performed by the hand was significantly straighter in IVF than in DVF condition for grasping movements directed at the objects located at 20 on the left and at 0 with respect to the subject s midsagittal axis. Such contrasted performances could result from the fact that controlling movements through a videoscreen constrains subjects to produce slow movements. In this case, the increase of on-line adjustments in slower movements might cause hand paths to be less curved. However, this hypothesis seems hardly plausible since we observed comparable MTs (for most of the participants) in both IVF and DVF conditions for the object located at 0 whereas path curvatures differed, the hand path being straighter in the IVF condition. Then if one assumes that hand trajectories can give insight into how visually directed movements are planned [3,4,16,17], we rather suppose that the differences observed in path curvature provide evidence that these two situations of visual feedback involve different control strategies. Furthermore, this assumption is backed up by the fact that differences in path curvature exist even right at the beginning of the movement (i.e., 150 ms after movement onset). Therefore, observing straighter trajectories under indirect visual feedback suggests that, in this situation, movements tend to be planned in extrinsic space so that the resulting path is roughly straight. These observations confirm previous results obtained with pointing movements [18]. In addition, as we can consider that in grasping movements the hand path is widely determined by the demands of the task [5,6,10,19,22,24], the present data show that the visual feedback constraint appears to dominate biomechanical constraints and has a massive influence on the trajectory formation. An interesting consequence of the present results is that they can reconcile controversial data reported in the literature. Indeed, as pointed in the introduction a large set of data showed that when subjects carried out reaching movements, hand paths were consistently straight [9,16,26,27]. By contrast, other studies observed that the shape of the movement carried out by the hand varied as a function of the movement direction. This outcome was interpreted as implying that reaching movements are not planned to follow a straight line in the extrinsic space

5 R. Palluel-Germain et al. / Neuroscience Letters 401 (2006) [1,5,14,17]. It is worth noting that a large set of studies describing extrinsic space planning used an indirect visual control situation in order to study motor planning processes [8,9,11,13,15]. Thus, divergent results obtained in the literature could be explained by the nature of the visual feedback provided when executing hand movement. A similar conclusion may be inferred from results obtained by Messier and Kalaska [15]. They indeed show the type of visual feedback provided (either IVF or DVF conditions) has an influence on the pattern of variable errors evidenced in a pointing task. Our results extend this finding by showing that, unlike under direct visual feedback situation, movements performed under indirect visual feedback tend to be planned in extrinsic space. Movements in the IVF condition being controlled though a video-system, it might be argued that the contrasted performances observed between IVF and DVF conditions for the 20 and 0 object positions originated from the fact that the visual feedback was in two dimensions in the IVF condition. Thus, one may think that this pattern of results could result from the processing of the monocular cues dominant in the IVF condition. However, it has been shown that the shapes of movement trajectories in a reaching task are similar under binocular and monocular viewing [12]. In addition, as the cylinder was viewed from the top in the IVF condition, we may suppose that the use of depth cues specifying the height of the object and thus the size of the grasping surface was limited in the IVF condition. However, this hypothesis is unlikely because (1) no significant difference in the height of the hand trajectory was observed between IVF and DVF conditions (z-axis, results not presented), and (2) once participants completed the first movement, they had access to the vertical dimension of the target object. We rather think that a visual constraint can straighten the hand path because in a remote control situation subjects estimate the desired trajectory by using hand and target positions displayed on the video screen. The idea of a visual hand-centred perception of target position is in agreement with studies concerned with frames of reference used in adaptation to directional bias in a video-controlled reaching task [2,20,21]. Boy et al. [2], for example, showed that in a remote control situation involving a non-congruent visual scene according to the actual work space, the initial orientation of hand trajectory in the first trial corresponded to the directional bias introduced. Therefore, in a remote visual control condition, the movement is initially coded as a vector between the initial hand position and the target position both determined from the visual display. However, this conclusion has to be moderated. Indeed, when the object was located at 20 with respect to the sagittal axis, movements carried out under the two conditions of visual feedback were quite similar, as regard to the hand path and to the movement duration. This result stresses the fact that the effect of the visual context on movement planning is not isotropic: when movements are controlled through a video screen, the effect of the visual context seems to differ according to the visual hemispace considered. Some previous reports are in agreement with the fact that the features of the hand trajectory in goaldirected movements depends on the workspace region in which the movement is executed. For example, in the horizontal plane, transverse movements were found to be outwardly convex, but radial movements were relatively straight [25]. In the vertical plane up and down movements were found to be outwardly convex, whereas back and forth movements were relatively straight [1]. In the same vein, the present data show that controlling one s own movements through a video display could also induce a modification in path curvature to an extent that depends on movement direction. Acknowledgement We want to thank Matthieu Bain to help running this experiment. References [1] C.G. Atkeson, J.M. Hollerbach, Kinematic features of unrestrained vertical arm movements, J. Neurosci. 5 (1985) [2] F. Boy, R. Palluel-Germain, J.P. Orliaguet, Y. Coello, Dissociation between where and how judgements of one s own motor performance in a video-controlled reaching task, Neurosci. Lett. 386 (2005) [3] M. Desmurget, M. Jordan, C. Prablanc, M. Jeannerod, Constrained and unconstrained movements involve different control strategies, J. Neurophysiol. 77 (1997) [4] M. Desmurget, D. Pelisson, Y. Rossetti, C. Prablanc, From eye to hand: planning goal-directed movements, Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 22 (1998) [5] M. Desmurget, C. Prablanc, Postural control of three-dimensional prehension movements, J. Neurophysiol. 77 (1997) [6] M. Desmurget, C. Prablanc, Y. Rossetti, M. Arzi, Y. Paulignan, C. Urquizar, J.C. Mignot, Postural and synergic control for threedimensional movements of reaching and grasping, J. Neurophysiol. 74 (1995) [7] T. Flash, N. Hogan, The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed mathematical model, J. Neurosci. 5 (1985) [8] J. Gordon, M.F. Ghilardi, C. Ghez, Accuracy of planar reaching movements. I. Independence of direction and extent variability, Exp. Brain Res. 99 (1994) [9] J. Gordon, M.F. Ghilardi, C. Ghez, Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. I. Spatial errors, J. Neurophysiol. 73 (1995) [10] H. Grea, M. Desmurget, C. Prablanc, Postural invariance in threedimensional reaching and grasping movements, Exp. Brain Res. 134 (2000) [11] H. Imamizu, S. Shimojo, The locus of visual-motor learning at the task or manipulator level: implications from intermanual transfer, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 21 (1995) [12] D.C. Knill, D. Kersten, Visuomotor sensitivity to visual information about surface orientation, J. Neurophysiol. 91 (2004) [13] J.W. Krakauer, Z.M. Pine, M.F. Ghilardi, C. Ghez, Learning of visuomotor transformations for vectorial planning of reaching trajectories, J. Neurosci. 20 (2000) [14] F. Lacquaniti, J.F. Soechting, S.A. Terzuolo, Path constraints on pointto-point arm movements in three-dimensional space, Neuroscience 17 (1986) [15] J. Messier, J.F. Kalaska, Differential effect of task conditions on errors of direction and extent of reaching movements, Exp. Brain Res. 115 (1997) [16] P. Morasso, Spatial control of arm movements, Exp. Brain Res. 42 (1981) [17] R. Osu, Y. Uno, Y. Koike, M. Kawato, Possible explanations for trajectory curvature in multijoint arm movements, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 23 (1997)

6 102 R. Palluel-Germain et al. / Neuroscience Letters 401 (2006) [18] R. Palluel-Germain, F. Boy, J.P. Orliaguet, Y. Coello, Visual and motor constraints on trajectory planning in pointing movements, Neurosci. Lett. 372 (2004) [19] Y. Paulignan, V.G. Frak, I. Toni, M. Jeannerod, Influence of object position and size on human prehension movements, Exp. Brain Res. 114 (1997) [20] I. Pennel, Y. Coello, J.P. Orliaguet, Frame of reference and adaptation to directional bias in a video-controlled reaching task, Ergonomics 45 (2002) [21] I. Pennel, Y. Coello, J.P. Orliaguet, Visuokinesthetic realignment in a video-controlled reaching task, J. Mot. Behav. 35 (2003) [22] D.A. Rosenbaum, R.J. Meulenbroek, J. Vaughan, C. Jansen, Posturebased motion planning: applications to grasping, Psychol. Rev. 108 (2001) [23] R.L. Sainburg, J.E. Lateiner, M.L. Latash, L.B. Bagesteiro, Effects of altering initial position on movement direction and extent, J. Neurophysiol. 89 (2003) [24] J.B. Smeets, E. Brenner, A new view on grasping, Motor Control 3 (1999) [25] Y. Uno, M. Kawato, R. Suzuki, Formation and control of optimal trajectory in human multijoint arm movement. Minimum torque-change model, Biol. Cybern. 61 (1989) [26] P. Vindras, P. Viviani, Frames of reference and control parameters in visuomanual pointing, J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 24 (1998) [27] P. Vindras, P. Viviani, Altering the visuomotor gain. Evidence that motor plans deal with vector quantities, Exp. Brain Res. 147 (2002)

Illusions as a tool to study the coding of pointing movements

Illusions as a tool to study the coding of pointing movements Exp Brain Res (2004) 155: 56 62 DOI 10.1007/s00221-003-1708-x RESEARCH ARTICLE Denise D. J. de Grave. Eli Brenner. Jeroen B. J. Smeets Illusions as a tool to study the coding of pointing movements Received:

More information

Effects of arm configuration on patterns of reaching variability in 3D space. Kishor Lakshmi Narayanan

Effects of arm configuration on patterns of reaching variability in 3D space. Kishor Lakshmi Narayanan Effects of arm configuration on patterns of reaching variability in 3D space by Kishor Lakshmi Narayanan A Thesis Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Science Approved

More information

The Haptic Perception of Spatial Orientations studied with an Haptic Display

The Haptic Perception of Spatial Orientations studied with an Haptic Display The Haptic Perception of Spatial Orientations studied with an Haptic Display Gabriel Baud-Bovy 1 and Edouard Gentaz 2 1 Faculty of Psychology, UHSR University, Milan, Italy gabriel@shaker.med.umn.edu 2

More information

Here I present more details about the methods of the experiments which are. described in the main text, and describe two additional examinations which

Here I present more details about the methods of the experiments which are. described in the main text, and describe two additional examinations which Supplementary Note Here I present more details about the methods of the experiments which are described in the main text, and describe two additional examinations which assessed DF s proprioceptive performance

More information

Muscular Torque Can Explain Biases in Haptic Length Perception: A Model Study on the Radial-Tangential Illusion

Muscular Torque Can Explain Biases in Haptic Length Perception: A Model Study on the Radial-Tangential Illusion Muscular Torque Can Explain Biases in Haptic Length Perception: A Model Study on the Radial-Tangential Illusion Nienke B. Debats, Idsart Kingma, Peter J. Beek, and Jeroen B.J. Smeets Research Institute

More information

The Influence of Visual Illusion on Visually Perceived System and Visually Guided Action System

The Influence of Visual Illusion on Visually Perceived System and Visually Guided Action System The Influence of Visual Illusion on Visually Perceived System and Visually Guided Action System Yu-Hung CHIEN*, Chien-Hsiung CHEN** * Graduate School of Design, National Taiwan University of Science and

More information

Misjudging where you felt a light switch in a dark room

Misjudging where you felt a light switch in a dark room Exp Brain Res (2011) 213:223 227 DOI 10.1007/s00221-011-2680-5 RESEARCH ARTICLE Misjudging where you felt a light switch in a dark room Femke Maij Denise D. J. de Grave Eli Brenner Jeroen B. J. Smeets

More information

Shape Distortion Produced by Isolated Mismatch Between Vision and Proprioception

Shape Distortion Produced by Isolated Mismatch Between Vision and Proprioception J Neurophysiol 99: 231 243, 2008. First published October 31, 2007; doi:10.1152/jn.00507.2007. Shape Distortion Produced by Isolated Mismatch Between Vision and Proprioception Nicole Malfait, 1,3,5 Denise

More information

Influence of Shape Elements on Performance during Haptic Rotation

Influence of Shape Elements on Performance during Haptic Rotation Influence of Shape Elements on Performance during Haptic Rotation Kathrin Krieger 1, Alexandra Moringen 1 Astrid M.L. Kappers 2, and Helge Ritter 1 1 Neuroinformatics, CITEC, University Bielefeld, Germany

More information

Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion

Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion Vision Research 42 (2002) 2731 2735 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion Erich W. Graf *, Wendy J. Adams, Martin Lages Department

More information

A Three-Dimensional Evaluation of Body Representation Change of Human Upper Limb Focused on Sense of Ownership and Sense of Agency

A Three-Dimensional Evaluation of Body Representation Change of Human Upper Limb Focused on Sense of Ownership and Sense of Agency A Three-Dimensional Evaluation of Body Representation Change of Human Upper Limb Focused on Sense of Ownership and Sense of Agency Shunsuke Hamasaki, Atsushi Yamashita and Hajime Asama Department of Precision

More information

IOC, Vector sum, and squaring: three different motion effects or one?

IOC, Vector sum, and squaring: three different motion effects or one? Vision Research 41 (2001) 965 972 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres IOC, Vector sum, and squaring: three different motion effects or one? L. Bowns * School of Psychology, Uni ersity of Nottingham, Uni ersity

More information

Effects of roll visual motion on online control of arm movement: reaching within a dynamic virtual environment

Effects of roll visual motion on online control of arm movement: reaching within a dynamic virtual environment DOI 10.1007/s001-008-1598-z RESEARCH ARTICLE Effects of roll visual motion on online control of arm movement: reaching within a dynamic virtual environment Assaf Y. Dvorkin Æ Robert V. Kenyon Æ Emily A.

More information

Stereoscopic occlusion and the aperture problem for motion: a new solution 1

Stereoscopic occlusion and the aperture problem for motion: a new solution 1 Vision Research 39 (1999) 1273 1284 Stereoscopic occlusion and the aperture problem for motion: a new solution 1 Barton L. Anderson Department of Brain and Cogniti e Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of

More information

A Pilot Study: Introduction of Time-domain Segment to Intensity-based Perception Model of High-frequency Vibration

A Pilot Study: Introduction of Time-domain Segment to Intensity-based Perception Model of High-frequency Vibration A Pilot Study: Introduction of Time-domain Segment to Intensity-based Perception Model of High-frequency Vibration Nan Cao, Hikaru Nagano, Masashi Konyo, Shogo Okamoto 2 and Satoshi Tadokoro Graduate School

More information

Perceived depth is enhanced with parallax scanning

Perceived depth is enhanced with parallax scanning Perceived Depth is Enhanced with Parallax Scanning March 1, 1999 Dennis Proffitt & Tom Banton Department of Psychology University of Virginia Perceived depth is enhanced with parallax scanning Background

More information

A Behavioral Adaptation Approach to Identifying Visual Dependence of Haptic Perception

A Behavioral Adaptation Approach to Identifying Visual Dependence of Haptic Perception A Behavioral Adaptation Approach to Identifying Visual Dependence of Haptic Perception James Sulzer * Arsalan Salamat Vikram Chib * J. Edward Colgate * (*) Laboratory for Intelligent Mechanical Systems,

More information

Motor planning for reach memorization task in 3D space

Motor planning for reach memorization task in 3D space New Jersey Institute of Technology Digital Commons @ NJIT Theses Theses and Dissertations Fall 2010 Motor planning for reach memorization task in 3D space Atul Narkhede New Jersey Institute of Technology

More information

Effect of the Implementation of the Two-Third Power Law in Teleoperation

Effect of the Implementation of the Two-Third Power Law in Teleoperation Effect of the Implementation of the Two-Third Power Law in Teleoperation Yves Rybarczyk and Diogo Carvalho Abstract This research consists in studying the effect of the implementation of a biological law

More information

Discriminating direction of motion trajectories from angular speed and background information

Discriminating direction of motion trajectories from angular speed and background information Atten Percept Psychophys (2013) 75:1570 1582 DOI 10.3758/s13414-013-0488-z Discriminating direction of motion trajectories from angular speed and background information Zheng Bian & Myron L. Braunstein

More information

Spatial Judgments from Different Vantage Points: A Different Perspective

Spatial Judgments from Different Vantage Points: A Different Perspective Spatial Judgments from Different Vantage Points: A Different Perspective Erik Prytz, Mark Scerbo and Kennedy Rebecca The self-archived postprint version of this journal article is available at Linköping

More information

Development of A Finger Mounted Type Haptic Device Using A Plane Approximated to Tangent Plane

Development of A Finger Mounted Type Haptic Device Using A Plane Approximated to Tangent Plane Development of A Finger Mounted Type Haptic Device Using A Plane Approximated to Tangent Plane Makoto Yoda Department of Information System Science Graduate School of Engineering Soka University, Soka

More information

Patterns of Hand Motion during Grasping and the Influence of Sensory Guidance

Patterns of Hand Motion during Grasping and the Influence of Sensory Guidance The Journal of Neuroscience, February 15, 2002, 22(4):1426 1435 Patterns of Hand Motion during Grasping and the Influence of Sensory Guidance Marco Santello, Martha Flanders, and John F. Soechting Department

More information

Haptic Discrimination of Perturbing Fields and Object Boundaries

Haptic Discrimination of Perturbing Fields and Object Boundaries Haptic Discrimination of Perturbing Fields and Object Boundaries Vikram S. Chib Sensory Motor Performance Program, Laboratory for Intelligent Mechanical Systems, Biomedical Engineering, Northwestern Univ.

More information

Role of gravitational cues in the haptic perception of orientation

Role of gravitational cues in the haptic perception of orientation Perception & Psychophysics 1996, 58 (8), 1278 1292 Role of gravitational cues in the haptic perception of orientation EDOUARD GENTAZ and YVETTE HATWELL Université Pierre Mendès France, Grenoble, France

More information

Experiments on the locus of induced motion

Experiments on the locus of induced motion Perception & Psychophysics 1977, Vol. 21 (2). 157 161 Experiments on the locus of induced motion JOHN N. BASSILI Scarborough College, University of Toronto, West Hill, Ontario MIC la4, Canada and JAMES

More information

GROUPING BASED ON PHENOMENAL PROXIMITY

GROUPING BASED ON PHENOMENAL PROXIMITY Journal of Experimental Psychology 1964, Vol. 67, No. 6, 531-538 GROUPING BASED ON PHENOMENAL PROXIMITY IRVIN ROCK AND LEONARD BROSGOLE l Yeshiva University The question was raised whether the Gestalt

More information

Visuomotor feedback gains are modulated by gaze position

Visuomotor feedback gains are modulated by gaze position J Neurophysiol 12: 2522 2531, 218. First published September 5, 218; doi:1.1152/jn.182.218. RESEARCH ARTICLE Control of Movement Visuomotor feedback gains are modulated by gaze position X Anouk J. de Brouwer,

More information

PREDICTION OF FINGER FLEXION FROM ELECTROCORTICOGRAPHY DATA

PREDICTION OF FINGER FLEXION FROM ELECTROCORTICOGRAPHY DATA University of Tartu Institute of Computer Science Course Introduction to Computational Neuroscience Roberts Mencis PREDICTION OF FINGER FLEXION FROM ELECTROCORTICOGRAPHY DATA Abstract This project aims

More information

Müller-Lyer Illusion Effect on a Reaching Movement in Simultaneous Presentation of Visual and Haptic/Kinesthetic Cues

Müller-Lyer Illusion Effect on a Reaching Movement in Simultaneous Presentation of Visual and Haptic/Kinesthetic Cues The 2009 IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems October 11-15, 2009 St. Louis, USA Müller-Lyer Illusion Effect on a Reaching Movement in Simultaneous Presentation of Visual

More information

Low-Frequency Transient Visual Oscillations in the Fly

Low-Frequency Transient Visual Oscillations in the Fly Kate Denning Biophysics Laboratory, UCSD Spring 2004 Low-Frequency Transient Visual Oscillations in the Fly ABSTRACT Low-frequency oscillations were observed near the H1 cell in the fly. Using coherence

More information

E X P E R I M E N T 12

E X P E R I M E N T 12 E X P E R I M E N T 12 Mirrors and Lenses Produced by the Physics Staff at Collin College Copyright Collin College Physics Department. All Rights Reserved. University Physics II, Exp 12: Mirrors and Lenses

More information

10.2 Images Formed by Lenses SUMMARY. Refraction in Lenses. Section 10.1 Questions

10.2 Images Formed by Lenses SUMMARY. Refraction in Lenses. Section 10.1 Questions 10.2 SUMMARY Refraction in Lenses Converging lenses bring parallel rays together after they are refracted. Diverging lenses cause parallel rays to move apart after they are refracted. Rays are refracted

More information

Adaptive Humanoid Robot Arm Motion Generation by Evolved Neural Controllers

Adaptive Humanoid Robot Arm Motion Generation by Evolved Neural Controllers Proceedings of the 3 rd International Conference on Mechanical Engineering and Mechatronics Prague, Czech Republic, August 14-15, 2014 Paper No. 170 Adaptive Humanoid Robot Arm Motion Generation by Evolved

More information

Running an HCI Experiment in Multiple Parallel Universes

Running an HCI Experiment in Multiple Parallel Universes Author manuscript, published in "ACM CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (alt.chi) (2014)" Running an HCI Experiment in Multiple Parallel Universes Univ. Paris Sud, CNRS, Univ. Paris Sud,

More information

Effects of Visual-Vestibular Interactions on Navigation Tasks in Virtual Environments

Effects of Visual-Vestibular Interactions on Navigation Tasks in Virtual Environments Effects of Visual-Vestibular Interactions on Navigation Tasks in Virtual Environments Date of Report: September 1 st, 2016 Fellow: Heather Panic Advisors: James R. Lackner and Paul DiZio Institution: Brandeis

More information

Calibration of Distance and Size Does Not Calibrate Shape Information: Comparison of Dynamic Monocular and Static and Dynamic Binocular Vision

Calibration of Distance and Size Does Not Calibrate Shape Information: Comparison of Dynamic Monocular and Static and Dynamic Binocular Vision ECOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY, 17(2), 55 74 Copyright 2005, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Calibration of Distance and Size Does Not Calibrate Shape Information: Comparison of Dynamic Monocular and Static and

More information

Egocentric reference frame bias in the palmar haptic perception of surface orientation. Allison Coleman and Frank H. Durgin. Swarthmore College

Egocentric reference frame bias in the palmar haptic perception of surface orientation. Allison Coleman and Frank H. Durgin. Swarthmore College Running head: HAPTIC EGOCENTRIC BIAS Egocentric reference frame bias in the palmar haptic perception of surface orientation Allison Coleman and Frank H. Durgin Swarthmore College Reference: Coleman, A.,

More information

TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.6 No.7 July 2002

TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.6 No.7 July 2002 288 Opinion support this theory contains unintended classical grouping cues that are themselves likely to be responsible for any grouping percepts. These grouping cues are consistent with well-established

More information

Development of a Finger Mounted Type Haptic Device Using a Plane Approximated to Tangent Plane

Development of a Finger Mounted Type Haptic Device Using a Plane Approximated to Tangent Plane Journal of Communication and Computer 13 (2016) 329-337 doi:10.17265/1548-7709/2016.07.002 D DAVID PUBLISHING Development of a Finger Mounted Type Haptic Device Using a Plane Approximated to Tangent Plane

More information

First-order structure induces the 3-D curvature contrast effect

First-order structure induces the 3-D curvature contrast effect Vision Research 41 (2001) 3829 3835 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres First-order structure induces the 3-D curvature contrast effect Susan F. te Pas a, *, Astrid M.L. Kappers b a Psychonomics, Helmholtz

More information

Grasping Occam s Razor

Grasping Occam s Razor Grasping Occam s Razor Jeroen B.J. Smeets, Eli Brenner, and Juul Martin Abstract Nine years after proposing our new view on grasping, we re-examine the support for the approach that we proposed. This approach

More information

AGING AND STEERING CONTROL UNDER REDUCED VISIBILITY CONDITIONS. Wichita State University, Wichita, Kansas, USA

AGING AND STEERING CONTROL UNDER REDUCED VISIBILITY CONDITIONS. Wichita State University, Wichita, Kansas, USA AGING AND STEERING CONTROL UNDER REDUCED VISIBILITY CONDITIONS Bobby Nguyen 1, Yan Zhuo 2, & Rui Ni 1 1 Wichita State University, Wichita, Kansas, USA 2 Institute of Biophysics, Chinese Academy of Sciences,

More information

Directional Bias in the Perception of Cast Shadows

Directional Bias in the Perception of Cast Shadows Article Directional Bias in the Perception of Cast Shadows i-perception January-February 2017: 1 17! The Author(s) 2017 DOI: 10.1177/2041669516682267 journals.sagepub.com/home/ipe Tomomi Koizumi Graduate

More information

Haptic control in a virtual environment

Haptic control in a virtual environment Haptic control in a virtual environment Gerard de Ruig (0555781) Lourens Visscher (0554498) Lydia van Well (0566644) September 10, 2010 Introduction With modern technological advancements it is entirely

More information

Calibrating Reach Distance to Visual Targets

Calibrating Reach Distance to Visual Targets Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 7, Vol. 33, No. 3, 64 66 Copyright 7 by the American Psychological Association 96-23/7/$12. DOI:.37/96-23.33.3.64 Calibrating Reach

More information

Evaluating Effect of Sense of Ownership and Sense of Agency on Body Representation Change of Human Upper Limb

Evaluating Effect of Sense of Ownership and Sense of Agency on Body Representation Change of Human Upper Limb Evaluating Effect of Sense of Ownership and Sense of Agency on Body Representation Change of Human Upper Limb Shunsuke Hamasaki, Qi An, Wen Wen, Yusuke Tamura, Hiroshi Yamakawa, Atsushi Yamashita, Hajime

More information

A Three-Channel Model for Generating the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex in Each Eye

A Three-Channel Model for Generating the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex in Each Eye A Three-Channel Model for Generating the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex in Each Eye LAURENCE R. HARRIS, a KARL A. BEYKIRCH, b AND MICHAEL FETTER c a Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, Canada

More information

Test of pan and zoom tools in visual and non-visual audio haptic environments. Magnusson, Charlotte; Gutierrez, Teresa; Rassmus-Gröhn, Kirsten

Test of pan and zoom tools in visual and non-visual audio haptic environments. Magnusson, Charlotte; Gutierrez, Teresa; Rassmus-Gröhn, Kirsten Test of pan and zoom tools in visual and non-visual audio haptic environments Magnusson, Charlotte; Gutierrez, Teresa; Rassmus-Gröhn, Kirsten Published in: ENACTIVE 07 2007 Link to publication Citation

More information

Reaching beyond reach

Reaching beyond reach Exp Brain Res (1999) 126:19 30 Springer-Verlag 1999 RESEARCH ARTICLE Martha Flanders Linda Daghestani Alain Berthoz Reaching beyond reach Received: 1 October 1998 / Accepted: 7 January 1999 Abstract The

More information

THE POGGENDORFF ILLUSION WITH ANOMALOUS SURFACES: MANAGING PAC-MANS, PARALLELS LENGTH AND TYPE OF TRANSVERSAL.

THE POGGENDORFF ILLUSION WITH ANOMALOUS SURFACES: MANAGING PAC-MANS, PARALLELS LENGTH AND TYPE OF TRANSVERSAL. THE POGGENDORFF ILLUSION WITH ANOMALOUS SURFACES: MANAGING PAC-MANS, PARALLELS LENGTH AND TYPE OF TRANSVERSAL. Spoto, A. 1, Massidda, D. 1, Bastianelli, A. 1, Actis-Grosso, R. 2 and Vidotto, G. 1 1 Department

More information

Salient features make a search easy

Salient features make a search easy Chapter General discussion This thesis examined various aspects of haptic search. It consisted of three parts. In the first part, the saliency of movability and compliance were investigated. In the second

More information

Haptic perception of spatial relations

Haptic perception of spatial relations Perception, 1999, volume 28, pages 781 ^ 795 DOI:1.168/p293 Haptic perception of spatial relations Astrid M L Kappers, Jan J Koenderink HelmholtzInstituut,Princetonplein5,3584CCUtrecht,TheNetherlands;e-mail:a.m.l.kappers@phys.uu.nl

More information

Real-Time Scanning Goniometric Radiometer for Rapid Characterization of Laser Diodes and VCSELs

Real-Time Scanning Goniometric Radiometer for Rapid Characterization of Laser Diodes and VCSELs Real-Time Scanning Goniometric Radiometer for Rapid Characterization of Laser Diodes and VCSELs Jeffrey L. Guttman, John M. Fleischer, and Allen M. Cary Photon, Inc. 6860 Santa Teresa Blvd., San Jose,

More information

Monocular occlusion cues alter the influence of terminator motion in the barber pole phenomenon

Monocular occlusion cues alter the influence of terminator motion in the barber pole phenomenon Vision Research 38 (1998) 3883 3898 Monocular occlusion cues alter the influence of terminator motion in the barber pole phenomenon Lars Lidén *, Ennio Mingolla Department of Cogniti e and Neural Systems

More information

This is a postprint of. The influence of material cues on early grasping force. Bergmann Tiest, W.M., Kappers, A.M.L.

This is a postprint of. The influence of material cues on early grasping force. Bergmann Tiest, W.M., Kappers, A.M.L. This is a postprint of The influence of material cues on early grasping force Bergmann Tiest, W.M., Kappers, A.M.L. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 8618, 393-399 Published version: http://dx.doi.org/1.17/978-3-662-44193-_49

More information

The Contribution of Visual & Somatosensory Input to Target Localization During the Performance of a Precision Grasping & Placement Task by Naime Tugac

The Contribution of Visual & Somatosensory Input to Target Localization During the Performance of a Precision Grasping & Placement Task by Naime Tugac The Contribution of Visual & Somatosensory Input to Target Localization During the Performance of a Precision Grasping & Placement Task by Naime Tugac A thesis presented to the University of Waterloo in

More information

FATIGUE INDEPENDENT AMPLITUDE-FREQUENCY CORRELATIONS IN EMG SIGNALS

FATIGUE INDEPENDENT AMPLITUDE-FREQUENCY CORRELATIONS IN EMG SIGNALS Fatigue independent amplitude-frequency correlations in emg signals. Adam SIEMIEŃSKI 1, Alicja KEBEL 1, Piotr KLAJNER 2 1 Department of Biomechanics, University School of Physical Education in Wrocław

More information

Haptic guidance improves the visuo-manual tracking of trajectories.

Haptic guidance improves the visuo-manual tracking of trajectories. Haptic guidance improves the visuo-manual tracking of trajectories. Jérémy Bluteau, Sabine Coquillart, Yohan Payan, Edouard Gentaz To cite this version: Jérémy Bluteau, Sabine Coquillart, Yohan Payan,

More information

TapBoard: Making a Touch Screen Keyboard

TapBoard: Making a Touch Screen Keyboard TapBoard: Making a Touch Screen Keyboard Sunjun Kim, Jeongmin Son, and Geehyuk Lee @ KAIST HCI Laboratory Hwan Kim, and Woohun Lee @ KAIST Design Media Laboratory CHI 2013 @ Paris, France 1 TapBoard: Making

More information

Galvanic vestibular stimulation in humans produces online arm movement deviations when reaching towards memorized visual targets

Galvanic vestibular stimulation in humans produces online arm movement deviations when reaching towards memorized visual targets Neuroscience Letters 318 (2002) 34 38 www.elsevier.com/locate/neulet Galvanic vestibular stimulation in humans produces online arm movement deviations when reaching towards memorized visual targets J.-P.

More information

The Shape-Weight Illusion

The Shape-Weight Illusion The Shape-Weight Illusion Mirela Kahrimanovic, Wouter M. Bergmann Tiest, and Astrid M.L. Kappers Universiteit Utrecht, Helmholtz Institute Padualaan 8, 3584 CH Utrecht, The Netherlands {m.kahrimanovic,w.m.bergmanntiest,a.m.l.kappers}@uu.nl

More information

Van Orden Stars & Cheiroscopic Tracing

Van Orden Stars & Cheiroscopic Tracing Van Orden Stars & Cheiroscopic Tracing Vision is not merely a matter of passive perception, it is an intelligent process of active construction. What you see is, invariably, what your visual intelligence

More information

Experiment 12: Microwaves

Experiment 12: Microwaves MASSACHUSETTS INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY Department of Physics 8.02 Spring 2005 OBJECTIVES Experiment 12: Microwaves To observe the polarization and angular dependence of radiation from a microwave generator

More information

Optics Practice. Version #: 0. Name: Date: 07/01/2010

Optics Practice. Version #: 0. Name: Date: 07/01/2010 Optics Practice Date: 07/01/2010 Version #: 0 Name: 1. Which of the following diagrams show a real image? a) b) c) d) e) i, ii, iii, and iv i and ii i and iv ii and iv ii, iii and iv 2. A real image is

More information

Head-Movement Evaluation for First-Person Games

Head-Movement Evaluation for First-Person Games Head-Movement Evaluation for First-Person Games Paulo G. de Barros Computer Science Department Worcester Polytechnic Institute 100 Institute Road. Worcester, MA 01609 USA pgb@wpi.edu Robert W. Lindeman

More information

A Study of Perceptual Performance in Haptic Virtual Environments

A Study of Perceptual Performance in Haptic Virtual Environments Paper: Rb18-4-2617; 2006/5/22 A Study of Perceptual Performance in Haptic Virtual Marcia K. O Malley, and Gina Upperman Mechanical Engineering and Materials Science, Rice University 6100 Main Street, MEMS

More information

Representing Spatial Location and Layout From Sparse Kinesthetic Contacts

Representing Spatial Location and Layout From Sparse Kinesthetic Contacts Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2003, Vol. 29, No. 2, 310 325 Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096-1523/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0096-1523.29.2.310

More information

Science Binder and Science Notebook. Discussions

Science Binder and Science Notebook. Discussions Lane Tech H. Physics (Joseph/Machaj 2016-2017) A. Science Binder Science Binder and Science Notebook Name: Period: Unit 1: Scientific Methods - Reference Materials The binder is the storage device for

More information

Distance perception from motion parallax and ground contact. Rui Ni and Myron L. Braunstein. University of California, Irvine, California

Distance perception from motion parallax and ground contact. Rui Ni and Myron L. Braunstein. University of California, Irvine, California Distance perception 1 Distance perception from motion parallax and ground contact Rui Ni and Myron L. Braunstein University of California, Irvine, California George J. Andersen University of California,

More information

Scene layout from ground contact, occlusion, and motion parallax

Scene layout from ground contact, occlusion, and motion parallax VISUAL COGNITION, 2007, 15 (1), 4868 Scene layout from ground contact, occlusion, and motion parallax Rui Ni and Myron L. Braunstein University of California, Irvine, CA, USA George J. Andersen University

More information

Reverse Perspective Rebecca Achtman & Duje Tadin

Reverse Perspective Rebecca Achtman & Duje Tadin Reverse Perspective Rebecca Achtman & Duje Tadin Basic idea: We see the world in 3-dimensions even though the image projected onto the back of our eye is 2-dimensional. How do we do this? The short answer

More information

Proprioception & force sensing

Proprioception & force sensing Proprioception & force sensing Roope Raisamo Tampere Unit for Computer-Human Interaction (TAUCHI) School of Information Sciences University of Tampere, Finland Based on material by Jussi Rantala, Jukka

More information

Discrimination of Virtual Haptic Textures Rendered with Different Update Rates

Discrimination of Virtual Haptic Textures Rendered with Different Update Rates Discrimination of Virtual Haptic Textures Rendered with Different Update Rates Seungmoon Choi and Hong Z. Tan Haptic Interface Research Laboratory Purdue University 465 Northwestern Avenue West Lafayette,

More information

Independence of perceptual and sensorimotor predictions in the size weight illusion

Independence of perceptual and sensorimotor predictions in the size weight illusion Independence of perceptual and sensorimotor predictions in the size weight illusion J. Randall Flanagan and Michael A. Beltzner Department of Psychology, Queen s University, Kingston, Ontario, K7L 3N6,

More information

Dissociating Ideomotor and Spatial Compatibility: Empirical Evidence and Connectionist Models

Dissociating Ideomotor and Spatial Compatibility: Empirical Evidence and Connectionist Models Dissociating Ideomotor and Spatial Compatibility: Empirical Evidence and Connectionist Models Ty W. Boyer (tywboyer@indiana.edu) Matthias Scheutz (mscheutz@indiana.edu) Bennett I. Bertenthal (bbertent@indiana.edu)

More information

Acoustic noise reduction of MRI systems by means of magnetic shielding

Acoustic noise reduction of MRI systems by means of magnetic shielding Acoustic noise reduction of MRI systems by means of magnetic shielding D. Biloen, N.B. Roozen Philips Applied Technologies, P.O.Box 218/Bldg. SAQ 2121, 56MD Eindhoven, The Netherlands {david.biloen, n.b.roozen}@philips.com,

More information

Perception of Haptic Force Magnitude during Hand Movements

Perception of Haptic Force Magnitude during Hand Movements 2008 IEEE International Conference on Robotics and Automation Pasadena, CA, USA, May 19-23, 2008 Perception of Haptic Force Magnitude during Hand Movements Xing-Dong Yang, Walter F. Bischof, and Pierre

More information

Coordinate system representations of movement direction in the premotor cortex

Coordinate system representations of movement direction in the premotor cortex Exp Brain Res (2007) 176:652 657 DOI 10.1007/s00221-006-0818-7 RESEARCH NOTE Coordinate system representations of movement direction in the premotor cortex Wei Wu Nicholas G. Hatsopoulos Received: 3 July

More information

Reaching Within a Dynamic Virtual Environment

Reaching Within a Dynamic Virtual Environment Reaching Within a Dynamic Virtual Environment Assaf Y. Dvorkin, Robert V. Kenyon, and Emily A. Keshner Abstract Planning and execution of reaching movements requires a series of computational processes

More information

Perceptual-motor coordination in an endoscopic surgery simulation

Perceptual-motor coordination in an endoscopic surgery simulation Surg Endosc (1999) 13: 127 132 Springer-Verlag New York Inc. 1999 Perceptual-motor coordination in an endoscopic surgery simulation J. G. Holden, 1, * J. M. Flach, 1 Y. Donchin 2 1 Psychology Department,

More information

Experience-dependent visual cue integration based on consistencies between visual and haptic percepts

Experience-dependent visual cue integration based on consistencies between visual and haptic percepts Vision Research 41 (2001) 449 461 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Experience-dependent visual cue integration based on consistencies between visual and haptic percepts Joseph E. Atkins, József Fiser, Robert

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Report

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Report Research Report RETINAL FLOW IS SUFFICIENT FOR STEERING DURING OBSERVER ROTATION Brown University Abstract How do people control locomotion while their eyes are simultaneously rotating? A previous study

More information

The Representational Effect in Complex Systems: A Distributed Representation Approach

The Representational Effect in Complex Systems: A Distributed Representation Approach 1 The Representational Effect in Complex Systems: A Distributed Representation Approach Johnny Chuah (chuah.5@osu.edu) The Ohio State University 204 Lazenby Hall, 1827 Neil Avenue, Columbus, OH 43210,

More information

Reach-to-Grasp Actions Under Direct and Indirect Viewing Conditions

Reach-to-Grasp Actions Under Direct and Indirect Viewing Conditions Western University Scholarship@Western Undergraduate Honours Theses Psychology 2014 Reach-to-Grasp Actions Under Direct and Indirect Viewing Conditions Ashley C. Bramwell Follow this and additional works

More information

Perception of scene layout from optical contact, shadows, and motion

Perception of scene layout from optical contact, shadows, and motion Perception, 2004, volume 33, pages 1305 ^ 1318 DOI:10.1068/p5288 Perception of scene layout from optical contact, shadows, and motion Rui Ni, Myron L Braunstein Department of Cognitive Sciences, University

More information

Chapter 3. Adaptation to disparity but not to perceived depth

Chapter 3. Adaptation to disparity but not to perceived depth Chapter 3 Adaptation to disparity but not to perceived depth The purpose of the present study was to investigate whether adaptation can occur to disparity per se. The adapting stimuli were large random-dot

More information

ENGINEERING GRAPHICS ESSENTIALS

ENGINEERING GRAPHICS ESSENTIALS ENGINEERING GRAPHICS ESSENTIALS Text and Digital Learning KIRSTIE PLANTENBERG FIFTH EDITION SDC P U B L I C AT I O N S Better Textbooks. Lower Prices. www.sdcpublications.com ACCESS CODE UNIQUE CODE INSIDE

More information

Curve. Glenn Grefer Field Application Engineer

Curve. Glenn Grefer Field Application Engineer Curve Glenn Grefer Field Application Engineer What is Curve? Curve is an optional addition to Calypso used to measure non-standard geometries. In GD&T terms, Profile Of A Line 2 Types of Curves 3D Curve

More information

Do Stereo Display Deficiencies Affect 3D Pointing?

Do Stereo Display Deficiencies Affect 3D Pointing? Do Stereo Display Deficiencies Affect 3D Pointing? Mayra Donaji Barrera Machuca SIAT, Simon Fraser University Vancouver, CANADA mbarrera@sfu.ca Wolfgang Stuerzlinger SIAT, Simon Fraser University Vancouver,

More information

Haptic perception of linear extent

Haptic perception of linear extent Perception & Psychophysics 1999, 61 (6), 1211-1226 Haptic perception of linear extent LAURA ARMSTRONG and LAWRENCE E. MARKS John B. Pierce Laboratory and Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut The perception

More information

Interference in stimuli employed to assess masking by substitution. Bernt Christian Skottun. Ullevaalsalleen 4C Oslo. Norway

Interference in stimuli employed to assess masking by substitution. Bernt Christian Skottun. Ullevaalsalleen 4C Oslo. Norway Interference in stimuli employed to assess masking by substitution Bernt Christian Skottun Ullevaalsalleen 4C 0852 Oslo Norway Short heading: Interference ABSTRACT Enns and Di Lollo (1997, Psychological

More information

Reducing the motor response in haptic parallel matching eliminates the typically observed gender difference

Reducing the motor response in haptic parallel matching eliminates the typically observed gender difference Exp Brain Res (2016) 234:105 112 DOI 10.1007/s00221-015-4437-z RESEARCH ARTICLE Reducing the motor response in haptic parallel matching eliminates the typically observed gender difference Hanneke I. van

More information

Factors affecting curved versus straight path heading perception

Factors affecting curved versus straight path heading perception Perception & Psychophysics 2006, 68 (2), 184-193 Factors affecting curved versus straight path heading perception CONSTANCE S. ROYDEN, JAMES M. CAHILL, and DANIEL M. CONTI College of the Holy Cross, Worcester,

More information

Light: Reflection and Refraction Light Reflection of Light by Plane Mirror Reflection of Light by Spherical Mirror Formation of Image by Mirror Sign Convention & Mirror Formula Refraction of light Through

More information

Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand

Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand Salvatore Aglioti*, Joseph F.X. DeSouza t and Melvyn A. Goodale* *Dipartimento di Scienze Neurologiche, e Della Visione, Sezione di Fisologia Umana,

More information

Immersive Virtual Environment for Visuo-Vestibular Therapy: Preliminary Results

Immersive Virtual Environment for Visuo-Vestibular Therapy: Preliminary Results Immersive Virtual Environment for Visuo-Vestibular Therapy: Preliminary Results Jean-Dominique Gascuel, Henri Payno, Sebastien Schmerber, Olivier Martin To cite this version: Jean-Dominique Gascuel, Henri

More information

Quintic Hardware Tutorial Camera Set-Up

Quintic Hardware Tutorial Camera Set-Up Quintic Hardware Tutorial Camera Set-Up 1 All Quintic Live High-Speed cameras are specifically designed to meet a wide range of needs including coaching, performance analysis and research. Quintic LIVE

More information

Universities of Leeds, Sheffield and York

Universities of Leeds, Sheffield and York promoting access to White Rose research papers Universities of Leeds, Sheffield and York http://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/ This is an author produced version of a paper published in Journal of Experimental

More information

Takeharu Seno 1,3,4, Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2, Stephen Palmisano 5 1

Takeharu Seno 1,3,4, Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2, Stephen Palmisano 5 1 Perception, 13, volume 42, pages 11 1 doi:1.168/p711 SHORT AND SWEET Vection induced by illusory motion in a stationary image Takeharu Seno 1,3,4, Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2, Stephen Palmisano 1 Institute for

More information