Perception of Whether an Object Can Be Carried Through an Aperture Depends on Anticipated Speed

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Perception of Whether an Object Can Be Carried Through an Aperture Depends on Anticipated Speed"

Transcription

1 J.B. Wagman & E.A. ExperimentalP Malek: Perception sychology 2007 Hogrefe and 2007; Anticipated & Vol. Huber 54(1):54 61 Publishers Speed Perception of Whether an Object Can Be Carried Through an Aperture Depends on Anticipated Speed Jeffrey B. Wagman and Eric A. Malek Department of Psychology, Illinois State University, Normal, IL, USA Abstract. We investigated whether anticipated speed of locomotion through an aperture influences perception of whether an object can be carried through that aperture. Participants reported whether they would be able to carry objects through an aperture (a) if they were to attempt to walk through the aperture and (b) if they were to attempt to run through the aperture. Furthermore, they did so when the object was held but not seen and when the object was seen but not held. In general, perception was influenced by object width and by anticipated speed but not by perceptual modality. Perceptual boundaries occurred at smaller object widths when participants anticipated running through the aperture than when they anticipated walking through the aperture. The results build on work showing that perception of affordances is influenced by kinetic potential as well as geometric properties and that perception may be supported by the detection of modality-neutral stimulation patterns. Keywords: haptic perception, visual perception, affordances, handheld objects In general, successful behavior requires successful perception. For a person to achieve a behavioral goal, the perceptual systems must provide that person with an awareness of which behaviors are possible and which are not. Possibilities for behavior for a particular person have been termed affordances (Gibson, 1979). Affordances are determined by the fit between a person s action capabilities and a particular property of the environment (Turvey, 1992). Therefore, perceiving affordances means perceiving the environment in terms of one s action capabilities. For example, perceiving whether an aperture can be passed through (without excessive shoulder rotation) means perceiving the aperture width in relation to the widest horizontal body dimension. Perception of this affordance is constrained by the shoulder width of the perceiver, and the perceptual boundary between apertures that afford passing through and those that do not occurs at a wider aperture width for perceivers with wide shoulders than for perceivers with narrow shoulders (Gordon & Rosenblum, 2004; Warren & Whang, 1987). Attachments to the Body and Action Capabilities Experimental Psychology 2007; Vol. 54(1):54 61 DOI / The challenge of perceiving environmental properties in terms of one s action capabilities is amplified by objects attached to the body such as handheld tools, equipment worn by fire fighters, and even luggage or backpacks. Attaching an object to the body creates an integrated personplus-object system (Wagman & Taylor, 2004), the action capabilities of which may be entirely different than those of the person-without-object. For example, objects attached to the body (e.g., a wheelchair) or objects carried by the person (e.g., a large box) may extend the widest horizontal body dimension and therefore change action capabilities for passing through apertures (see Higuchi, Takada, Matsuura, & Imanaka, 2004). Therefore, perceiving affordances for the person-plus-object system means perceiving properties of the environment in relation to the action capabilities of the integrated person-plus-object system (Bongers, Smitsman, & Michaels, 2003; Smitsman, 1997; Wagman & Taylor, 2005). Perception of whether an object can be carried through an aperture is constrained by the width of the person-plus-object system, and the perceptual boundary between objects that can be carried through an aperture and those that cannot occurs when the width of the person-plus-object system approximates the width of the aperture (Wagman & Taylor, 2005). Objects attached to the body or carried by the person change action capabilities for passing through apertures because they change the person s geometric properties. Importantly, changes in action capabilities can also result from changes in a person s kinetic potential (Adolph & Avolio, 2000; Bhalla & Proffitt, 1999; Oudejans, Michaels, Bakker, & Dolné, 1996; Proffitt, Stefanucci, Banton, & Epstein, 2003). For example, a gap in the support surface that might not be crossable when approached by walking might be crossable when approached by running (see Turvey, 2004). However, whereas increased speed 2007 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers

2 J.B. Wagman & E.A. Malek: Perception and Anticipated Speed 55 may enhance action capabilities for crossing gaps in a support surface, it hinders action capabilities for passing through apertures. Participants rotate their shoulders to a greater degree when walking through apertures at a fast pace than when walking through apertures at a normal pace (Warren & Whang, 1987, Experiment 1). Accordingly, increased speed toward an aperture may also hinder action capabilities for carrying an object through an aperture, particularly when the width of the carried object is approximately equal to the width of the aperture. It is not known, however, whether anticipated speed of locomotion will influence perception of whether an object can be carried through an aperture. Perception of Affordances by Different Perceptual Modalities The two perceptual modalities primarily involved in perceiving whether an object can be carried through an aperture are the haptic system and the visual system. In a situation where the object is already in the hand(s) of the perceiver, the haptic system is primarily involved in perceiving the properties of the carried object and the visual system is primarily involved in perceiving the properties of the aperture. In a situation where the object is not yet in the hand(s) of the perceiver, the visual system is primarily responsible for perceiving properties of both the object and the aperture. From the ecological perspective on perception-action (Gibson, 1979; Turvey, 1992, 2004), there is reason to expect that perceivers will be just as successful in perceiving whether the object can be carried through the aperture regardless of which perceptual modalities are involved. From this perspective, the stimulation patterns that support perception of affordances can be found in structured ambient energy arrays (Gibson, 1979). However, structured ambient energy arrays not only support perception of affordances within a given energy array, they also seem to support perception of the same affordance across different energy arrays. For example, participants are capable of perceiving whether an aperture affords passing through regardless of whether they are viewing that aperture (Warren & Whang, 1987) or whether they are listening to sounds projected through that aperture (Gordon & Rosenblum, 2004). In addition, participants are capable of perceiving whether a surface affords upright posture regardless of whether they are viewing that surface or whether they are probing that surface with a handheld rod (while blindfolded) (Fitzpatrick, Carello, Schmidt, & Corey, 1994). Findings such as these suggest that the particulars of a given energy array may be irrelevant so long as structure in that array is sufficient to support perception of the affordance. That is, it is possible that the information for perception of affordances may be modality-neutral (Rosenblum & Gordon, 2001; see Carello, Wagman, & Turvey, 2005). With respect to perception of whether an object can be carried through an aperture, results of previous experiments have shown that participants are capable of perceiving this affordance regardless of whether the objects are perceived visually or whether the objects are perceived haptically (Wagman & Taylor, 2005). It is not known, however, whether anticipated speed of locomotion will influence perception of this affordance differently when the objects are perceived visually and when the objects are perceived haptically. Experiments 1A and 1B Experiments 1A and 1B investigated perception of whether an object can be carried through an aperture (a) when the perceiver anticipates running through the aperture and (b) when the perceiver anticipates walking through the aperture. Experiment 1A investigated perception of this affordance under these conditions when the object is held but not seen, and Experiment 1B investigated perception of this affordance under these conditions when the object is seen but not held. In order to test hypotheses about the ability to perceive this affordance when the objects are perceived haptically and when the objects are perceived visually, data from Experiments 1A and 1B were combined prior to data analysis. In this analysis, anticipated speed is treated as a within participants variable and perceptual modality is treated as a between participants variable. We expect that perception of this affordance will be influenced by the width of the object and by the anticipated speed of locomotion through the aperture. However, we do not expect that perception of this affordance will be influenced by the perceptual modality by which the objects are perceived. Specifically, we expect that in both perceptual modalities, perceivers will be more conservative in reporting that they would be able to carry objects through the aperture when they anticipate running through the aperture than when they anticipate walking through the aperture, particularly when the width of the object is approximately equal to the width of the aperture. Furthermore, we expect that perceivers will exhibit perceptual boundaries at smaller object widths when they anticipate running through the aperture than when they anticipate walking through the aperture, but we do not expect that perceptual boundaries will differ across perceptual modalities. We also expect that regardless of anticipated speed of locomotion, perceivers will be least confident in their perceptual judgments when the object width is approximately equal to the perceptual boundary. Finally, given the findings of previous research (Wagman & Taylor, 2005, see Fitzpatrick et al., 1994), we expect perceivers to be less confident in their judgments when the objects are perceived haptically than when the objects are perceived visually Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Experimental Psychology 2007; Vol. 54(1):54 61

3 56 J.B. Wagman & E.A. Malek: Perception and Anticipated Speed Figure 1. (a) The experimental paradigm for Experiment 1A. Participants held occluded objects of different widths and reported whether they would be able to carry the object through the aperture when they anticipated running through the aperture and when they anticipated walking through the aperture. (b) Perceivers were more conservative and exhibited perceptual boundaries at smaller object widths when they anticipated running through the aperture than when they anticipated walking through the aperture. Bars indicate standard error. (c) Perceivers were least confident in their perceptual reports when the width of the object was approximately equal to the perceptual boundary. Method Participants Twenty-five undergraduate students (22 women and 3 men) from Illinois State University participated in Experiment 1A. Nineteen undergraduate students (12 women and 7 men) from Illinois State University participated in Experiment 1B. All participants received extra credit in their psychology courses in exchange for their participation. One participant in Experiment 1A was excluded from data analysis due to failure to follow instructions. Materials and Apparatus Experimental Psychology 2007; Vol. 54(1):54 61 The stimuli used in Experiments 1A and 1B consisted of ten T-shaped objects constructed from (hollow) PVC pipe (1 cm inner radius, 1.35 cm outer radius). Each object consisted of a stem (15 cm in length and 50 g in mass) and two branches of equal length. The three pieces were connected by means of a standard three-way connector for PVC of this diameter (37 g) to create a T. The ten objects ranged in width (from branch tip to branch tip including three-way connector) from 50 cm to 140 cm in 10 cm increments (cf. Wagman & Taylor, 2005). For such objects, the frontal dimension that constrains whether the aperture can be passed through is the width of the carried object (see Figure 1a and Figure 2a). The aperture used in Experiments 1A and 1B was also constructed from PVC pipe (1.25 cm inner radius, 1.65 cm outer radius). The aperture was 90 cm wide and 218 cm tall. Black curtain was hung behind the aperture so that no other information about size was available to the participant. In Experiment 1A, participants wore modified plastic safety goggles with a horizontal cardboard occlusion screen (75 cm wide by 25 cm deep) attached to the bottom 2007 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers

4 J.B. Wagman & E.A. Malek: Perception and Anticipated Speed 57 Figure 2. (a) The experimental paradigm for Experiment 1B. Participants viewed objects of different widths and reported whether they would be able to carry the object through the aperture when they anticipated running through the aperture than when they anticipated walking through the aperture. (b) Perceivers were more conservative and exhibited perceptual boundaries at smaller object widths when they anticipated running through the aperture than when they anticipated walking through the aperture. Bars indicate standard error. (c) Perceivers were least confident in their perceptual reports when the width of the object was approximately equal to the perceptual boundary. Figure 1a and Figure 2a adapted from Wagman, J.B., & Taylor, K.R. (2005). Perceiving affordances for aperture crossing for the person-plus-object system. Ecological Psychology, 17, rims so that they were unable to see the T-shaped objects but were still able to see the aperture (cf. Wagman & Taylor, 2005) (see Figure 1a). In Experiment 1B, a box (95 cm tall 45 cm wide 45 cm deep) was used to support the objects (see Figure 2a). A box of this height was chosen because it approximated the waist height of an average participant. The box was placed 20 cm from the participant and was centered with the aperture. Procedure for Experiment 1A The participant stood 350 cm from the aperture in a designated area (90 cm wide 40 cm deep). The outer edges of this area were parallel with the aperture. The participant was handed one of objects such that their right hand was flush with the bottom of the stem and the object was roughly horizontal (i.e., so that the branches extended to each side). The participant grasped the object firmly and held it at the center of his or her waist (see Figure 1a). The task of the participant was to determine whether they would be able to carry each T-shaped object through the aperture if they were to hold the object horizontally at their waist and approach the aperture without turning their shoulders (cf. Wagman & Taylor, 2005; Warren & Whang, 1987). Participants provided two reports. The first was a yes or no judgment as to whether they would be able to carry the object through the aperture under these conditions, and the second was their confidence in that judgment on a scale of 1 to 7 (1 being very uncertain/not at all confident and 7 being absolutely certain/very confident). When the partic Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Experimental Psychology 2007; Vol. 54(1):54 61

5 58 J.B. Wagman & E.A. Malek: Perception and Anticipated Speed ipant provided both reports, the experimenter handed the participant the next object. Participants were allowed to wield the object for as long as necessary to provide both reports. Wielding was restricted to the wrist positioned at the center of the waist, and participants were asked not to wield the object in any way such that it would come into contact with anything (such as the floor or the goggles). At no point did the participant see the object. Participants performed this task under two conditions. In the walk condition, participants reported whether they would be able to carry the object through the aperture if they were to attempt to walk through the aperture. In the run condition, participants reported whether they would be able to carry the object through the aperture if they were to attempt to run through the aperture. All participants completed both conditions in blocked fashion, and the order of conditions was counterbalanced across participants. Within a given condition, objects were presented three times each, and order of presentation of objects was randomized. Procedure for Experiment 1B The participant stood in the designated area described above and viewed an object that had been placed on the box such that the bottom of the object was flush with the edge closest to the participant and the branches extended to each side of the box (see Figure 2a). As in Experiment 1A, participants provided two reports. The first was a yes or no judgment as to whether they would be able to carry the object through the aperture if they were to pick the object up, hold it at the center of their waist so that the branches extended to each side, and carry it toward the aperture without turning their shoulders. The second was their confidence in that judgment on a scale of 1 to 7 (see above). When the participant provided both reports, they closed their eyes while the experimenter replaced the object. Participants were allowed to view the object for as long as necessary to provide both reports. At no point did the participant touch or pick up the object. As in Experiment 1A, participants performed this task under two conditions. In the walk condition, participants reported whether they would be able to carry the object through the aperture if they were to attempt to walk through the aperture. In the run condition, participants reported whether they would be able to carry the object through the aperture if they were to attempt to run through the aperture. All participants completed both conditions in blocked fashion, and the order of conditions was counterbalanced across participants. Within a given condition, objects were presented three times each, and order of presentation of objects was randomized. Results and Discussion As described above, the data from Experiments 1A and 1B were combined prior to data analysis. In this analysis, anticipated speed is treated as a within participants variable and perceptual modality is treated as a between participants variable. First, we compared the mean percentage of yes responses for each object in each condition. A 2 (anticipated speed: walk vs. run) 2 (perceptual modality: haptics vs. vision) 10 (object width) analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed that the percentage of yes responses (i.e., responses of crossable ) decreased with increasing object width, F(9, 378) = , MSE = , p <.01, and that the percentage of yes responses was greater in the walk condition (59.8%) than in the run condition (53.1%), F(1, 42) = 25.75, MSE = , p <.01. A significant interaction of anticipated speed and object width indicated that in each perceptual modality the difference in percentage of yes responses between the run condition and the walk condition was most pronounced for objects between 90 cm and 110 cm, F(9, 378) = 6.74, MSE = , p <.01 (see Figures 1b and 2b). An interaction of object width and perceptual modality suggested that percentage of yes responses was greater in the vision condition than in the haptics condition for objects that were larger than 100 cm. There were no other significant effects (all F values < 1). 1 As expected, perception of whether the object could be carried through the aperture was influenced by the width of the object and the anticipated speed of locomotion through the aperture. Regardless of whether the objects were perceived haptically or visually, perceivers were more conservative in reporting that they would be able to carry objects through the aperture when they anticipated running through the aperture than when they anticipated walking through the aperture, particularly when the width of the object was approximately equal to the width of the aperture. Participants were more conservative in reporting that they would be able to carry objects through the aperture when objects were perceived visually than when they were perceived haptically but only for objects that were wider than the aperture (see Wagman & Taylor, 2005). Second, in order to determine if the differences in percentage of yes responses would lead to differences in perceptual boundaries, we compared perceptual boundaries derived in each of the conditions both at the level of the aggregate data and at the level of the individual participants. At the level of the aggregate data, probit analysis (Finney, 1971) was used to determine the object width that would result in a response of yes fifty percent of the time in each condition. This width is the boundary between those objects that are perceived to afford carrying through the aperture and those objects that are not (see Burton, 1992; Fitzpatrick et al., 1994). Regardless of perceptual modality, the perceptual boundary occurred at a 1 The results of this ANOVA were verified with nonparametric analyses. There were no differences between the results of the ANOVA and the results of the nonparametric analyses. Experimental Psychology 2007; Vol. 54(1): Hogrefe & Huber Publishers

6 J.B. Wagman & E.A. Malek: Perception and Anticipated Speed 59 smaller object width in the run condition than in the walk condition (p values <.05). In the haptic condition, the perceptual boundary occurred at an object width of 99.1 cm in the run condition (with lower and upper fiducial limits of 94.3 cm and cm, respectively) and at an object width of cm in the walk condition (with lower and upper fiducial limits of cm and cm, respectively) (see Figure 1b). In the vision condition, the perceptual boundary occurred at an object width of 98.1 cm in the run condition (with lower and upper fiducial limits of 96.1 cm and cm, respectively) and at an object width of cm in the walk condition (with lower and upper fiducial limits of cm and cm, respectively) (see Figure 2b). The fiducial limits overlap only in the two cases when anticipated speed is the same and perceptual modality differs, suggesting that the perceptual boundaries differ as a function of anticipated speed (p <.05) but not as a function of perceptual modality (Finney, 1971; Payton, Greenstone, & Schenker, 2003). At the level of the individual participants, the largest object for which at least two of the three responses were yes was taken to be the perceptual boundary for that participant in that condition (see Burton, 1992). A 2 (anticipated speed) 2 (perceptual modality) ANOVA revealed that the perceptual boundary occurred at a smaller object width in the run condition (96.4 cm) than in the walk condition (103.4 cm), F(1, 42) = 11.34, MSE = 96.53, p <.01. There were no other significant effects (F values < 1). Thus, as expected, both at the level of the aggregate data and at the level of the individual participant data, the perceptual boundary occurred at a smaller object width when participants anticipated running through the aperture than when they anticipated walking through the aperture (see Figure 1b and Figure 2b). Also as expected, there was no difference in perceptual boundaries when objects were perceived visually and when objects were perceived haptically. Third, we compared the mean confidence judgments on each object in each condition. A 2 (anticipated speed: walk vs. run) 2 (perceptual modality: haptics vs. vision) 10 (object width) ANOVA revealed that participants were more confident in their judgments when objects were perceived visually (5.94) than when objects were perceived haptically (5.31), F(1, 42) = 12.73, MSE = 6.88, p <.01. Furthermore, confidence varied as a function of object width, F(9, 378) = 51.30, MSE = 1.13, p <.01. Inspection of Figure 1c and Figure 2c suggests that participants were least confident when object width was approximately 100 cm. The main effect of anticipated speed was nonsignificant, F < 1, suggesting that perceivers were as confident in their perceptual judgments when they anticipated running through the aperture as when they anticipated walking through the aperture. However, an interaction of anticipated speed and object width suggests that participants were more confident in their perceptual judgments on smaller objects (objects less than 100 cm wide) in the walk condition but were more confident in their perceptual judgments on larger objects (objects greater than 100 cm wide) in the run condition, F(9, 378) = 3.73, MSE = 0.567, p <.01 (see Figure 1c and Figure 2c). Given the perceptual boundaries reported above, this interaction reveals that participants were more confident in reporting that they would be able to carry smaller objects through the aperture in the walk condition but were more confident in reporting that they would not be able to carry larger objects through the aperture in the run condition. The interaction of width and perceptual modality was marginally significant, F(9, 378) = 1.78, MSE = 1.13, p =.07. There were no other significant effects, all F values < 1. Thus, as expected, perceivers were more confident in their judgments when the objects were perceived visually than when the objects were perceived haptically, and perceivers were least confident in their judgments when the width of the object was approximately equal to the perceptual boundary (see Fitzpatrick et al., 1994; Wagman & Taylor, 2005). General Discussion Previous research has shown that perception of whether an object can be carried through an aperture is influenced by a geometric property the width of the person-plus-objectsystem (Wagman & Taylor, 2005). The results of the current experiments suggest that perception of this affordance is also influenced by (anticipated) kinetic potential the anticipated speed of locomotion through the aperture. Consistent with our hypotheses: (1) perceivers were more conservative in reporting that they would be able to carry objects through the aperture when they anticipated running through the aperture than when they anticipated walking through the aperture, particularly when the width of the object was approximately equal to the width of the aperture, (2) perceivers exhibited perceptual boundaries at smaller object widths when they anticipated running through the aperture than when they anticipated walking through the aperture, and (3) perceptual boundaries were not different when objects were perceived haptically and when objects were perceived visually. Also consistent with our hypotheses, (4) perceivers were least confident in their perceptual judgments when object width was approximately equal to the perceptual boundary, and (5) perceivers were less confident in their perceptual judgments when the objects were perceived haptically than when the objects were perceived visually. Interestingly, perceivers were equally confident in their perceptual judgments when they anticipated running through the aperture and when they anticipated walking through the aperture. A number of studies have shown that perception of affordances is influenced by changes in the perceiver s geometric properties. For example, perceivers are more conservative in reporting that they can maneuver a wheelchair through an aperture than in reporting that they can walk through an aperture (Higuchi et al., 2004). Additionally, a number of studies have shown that perception of affordances is influenced by changes in the perceiver s kinetic 2007 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Experimental Psychology 2007; Vol. 54(1):54 61

7 60 J.B. Wagman & E.A. Malek: Perception and Anticipated Speed Experimental Psychology 2007; Vol. 54(1):54 61 potential. For example, participants rotate their shoulders more when walking through apertures at a fast pace than when walking through apertures at a normal pace (Warren & Whang, 1987; see also Adolph & Avolio, 2000; Bhalla & Proffitt, 1999; Proffitt et al., 2003). Our results extend such findings by showing that perception of affordances is influenced by anticipated changes in the perceiver s kinetic potential. Perceivers were more conservative in reporting that they would be able to carry an object through an aperture when they anticipated running through the aperture than when they anticipated walking through the aperture (i.e., without actually approaching the aperture and without actually experiencing the changes in kinetic potential associated with either walking or running). The fact that participants made perceptual judgments before actually approaching the aperture was essential to show that perception of affordances is influenced by anticipated changes in kinetic potential. However, such a manipulation also provides an important limitation. Participants in both of the current experiments tended to overestimate their ability to carry objects through the aperture (although this tendency was reduced when perceivers anticipated running through the aperture). Overestimation of this ability is consistent with previous research, particularly when participants have little or no exposure to how an object attached to the body changes their action capabilities (Higuchi et al., 2004; Wagman & Taylor, 2005; see Mark, 1987). Research has shown that it may take some repeated exposure to a particular attachment to the body for a perceiver to become accustomed to how the geometric changes influence the action capabilities of the person-plus-object system (see Hirose & Nishio, 2001; Mark, 1987). In the same way, it may take some exposure to kinetic changes for the perceiver to become accustomed to how those changes influence their action capabilities (Bhalla & Proffitt, 1999; Oudejans, et al., 1996; Proffitt et al., 2003; but see Warren & Whang, 1987, Experiment 2). The fact that perceivers in the current experiments were not given the opportunity to experience how the kinetic changes associated with walking or running influence their action capabilities likely contributed to their overestimation of their abilities. Future research may investigate if the tendency to overestimate the ability to carry an object through an aperture will be further reduced if participants are exposed to their kinetic potential (by actually walking or running toward the aperture). Alternatively, participants may have overestimated their ability to carry objects through the aperture because of the instructions they were given to report whether they would be able to pass through the aperture without turning their shoulders. Ordinarily, people are not restricted in this way when walking through apertures. Shoulder turning is a natural adjustment when passing through apertures, particularly when the aperture is approximately equal to the shoulder width (Warren & Whang, 1987). It is a behavior that allows perceivers to fine tune their perceptually-guided behaviors as they unfold over time. Participants in the current experiment may have made their judgments assuming that they would be able to fine-tune the behavior by turning their shoulders despite the instructions provided by the experimenter. This, in turn, may have led to overestimations of whether the object could be carried through the aperture (for a similar account of overestimation in perception of whether an object is within reach, see Rochat & Wraga, 1997). The finding that the perceptual boundaries on carrying an object through an aperture did not differ when the objects were perceived haptically and when the objects were perceived visually is consistent with previous results (Wagman & Taylor, 2005). The current results build on this work by showing that anticipated speed of locomotion influences perceptual boundaries in the same way regardless of the perceptual modality by which the objects are perceived. Together, the findings add to a growing body of research suggesting that structured ambient energy arrays support perception of the same affordance across different energy arrays (see Carello et al., 2005). Overall, the results suggest that perception of whether an object can be carried through an aperture is influenced by the width of the carried object and the anticipated speed of locomotion through the aperture but not by the modality by which the objects are perceived. Such results highlight (a) that perception of affordances is constrained by the action capabilities of the perceiver (Gibson, 1979; Turvey, 1992), (b) that action capabilities of the perceiver are a function of both geometric properties and kinetic potential (Konczak, Meeuwsen, & Cress, 1992; Warren & Whang, 1987), and (c) information for perception of affordances may be modality-neutral (see Gordon & Rosenblum, 2001). Furthermore, the results may have implications for the design of objects attached to the body or for the training of people who use such objects. Acknowledgments We thank Dawn McBride for help with statistical analysis and for helpful discussion. We also thank two anonymous reviewers for their comments on a previous draft of this manuscript. References Adolph, K.E., & Avolio, A.M. (2000). Walking infants adapt to changing body dimensions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, Bhalla, M., & Proffitt, D.R. (1999). Visual-motor recalibration in geographical slant perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, Bongers, R.M., Smitsman, A.W., & Michaels, C.F. (2003). Geometrics and dynamics of a rod determine how it is used for reaching. Journal of Motor Behavior, 35, Hogrefe & Huber Publishers

8 J.B. Wagman & E.A. Malek: Perception and Anticipated Speed 61 Burton, G. (1992). Nonvisual judgment of the crossability of path gaps. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, Finney, D.J. (1971). Probit analysis. London: Cambridge University Press. Carello, C., Wagman, J.B., & Turvey, M.T. (2005). Acoustic specification of object properties. In J. Anderson & B. Anderson (Eds.), Moving image theory: Ecological considerations (pp ). Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press. Fitzpatrick, P., Carello, C., Schmidt, R.C., & Corey, D. (1994). Haptic and visual perception of an affordance for upright posture. Ecological Psychology, 6, Gibson, J.J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Gordon, M.S., & Rosenblum, L.D. (2004). Perception of soundobstructing surfaces using body-scaled judgments. Ecological Psychology, 16, Higuchi, T., Takada, H., Matsuura, Y., & Imanaka, K. (2004). Visual estimation of spatial requirements for locomotion in novice wheelchair users. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 10, Hirose, N., & Nishio, A. (2001). The process of adapting to perceiving new action capabilities. Ecological Psychology, 13, Konczak, J., Meeuwsen, H., & Cress, M.E. (1992). Changing affordances in stair climbing: The perception of maximum climbability in young and older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, Mark, L.S. (1987). Eyeheight-scaled information about affordances: A study of sitting and stair climbing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, Oudejans,R.,Michaels,C.F.,Bakker,F.C.,&Dolné,M.A. (1996). The relevance of action in perceiving affordances: Perception of catchableness of fly balls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, Payton, M.E., Greenstone, M.H., & Schenker, N. (2003). Overlapping confidence intervals or standard error intervals: What do they mean in terms of statistical significance. Journal of Insect Science, 3, 1 7. Proffitt, D.R., Stefanucci, J., Banton, T., & Epstein, W. (2003). The role of effort in perceiving distance. Psychological Science, 14, Rochat, P., & Wraga, M. (1997). An account of the systematic error in judging what is reachable. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, Rosenblum, L.D., & Gordon, M.S. (2001). The generality of specificity: Some lessons from audiovisual speech. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, Smitsman, A.W. (1997). The development of tool use: Changing boundaries between organism and environment. In C. Dent- Read & P. Zukow-Goldring (Eds.), Evolving explanations of development (pp ). Washington, DC: APA. Turvey, M.T. (1992). Affordances and prospective control: An outline of the ontology. Ecological Psychology, 4, Turvey, M.T. (2004). Space and its perception: The first and final frontier. Ecological Psychology, 16, Wagman, J.B., & Taylor, K.R. (2004). Chosen-striking location and the user-tool-environment system. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 10, Wagman, J.B., & Taylor, K.R. (2005). Perceiving affordances for aperture crossing for the person-plus-object system. Ecological Psychology, 17, Warren, W.H., & Whang, S. (1987). Visual guidance of walking through apertures: Body scaled information for affordances. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, Received January 19, 2006 Revision received February 20, 2006 Accepted February 22, 2006 Jeffrey B. Wagman Department of Psychology Campus Box 4620 Illinois State University Normal, IL USA Tel Fax jbwagma@ilstu.edu 2007 Hogrefe & Huber Publishers Experimental Psychology 2007; Vol. 54(1):54 61

Perceiving affordances in virtual reality: Influence of person and environmental properties in perception of standing on virtual grounds

Perceiving affordances in virtual reality: Influence of person and environmental properties in perception of standing on virtual grounds Perceiving affordances in virtual reality: Influence of person and environmental properties in perception of standing on virtual grounds Tony Regia-Corte, Maud Marchal, Gabriel Cirio, Anatole Lécuyer INRIA

More information

Rule for Scaling Shoulder Rotation Angles while Walking through Apertures

Rule for Scaling Shoulder Rotation Angles while Walking through Apertures Rule for Scaling Shoulder Rotation Angles while Walking through Apertures Takahiro Higuchi 1 *, Yasuhiro Seya 2, Kuniyasu Imanaka 1 1 Department of Health Promotion Science, Tokyo Metropolitan University,

More information

Correspondence Between Haptic and Visual Perception of Stand-on-ability: Do Hills Look as Steep as they Feel?

Correspondence Between Haptic and Visual Perception of Stand-on-ability: Do Hills Look as Steep as they Feel? The University of Southern Mississippi The Aquila Digital Community Master's Theses Fall 12-2016 Correspondence Between Haptic and Visual Perception of Stand-on-ability: Do Hills Look as Steep as they

More information

Spatial Judgments from Different Vantage Points: A Different Perspective

Spatial Judgments from Different Vantage Points: A Different Perspective Spatial Judgments from Different Vantage Points: A Different Perspective Erik Prytz, Mark Scerbo and Kennedy Rebecca The self-archived postprint version of this journal article is available at Linköping

More information

Distance perception from motion parallax and ground contact. Rui Ni and Myron L. Braunstein. University of California, Irvine, California

Distance perception from motion parallax and ground contact. Rui Ni and Myron L. Braunstein. University of California, Irvine, California Distance perception 1 Distance perception from motion parallax and ground contact Rui Ni and Myron L. Braunstein University of California, Irvine, California George J. Andersen University of California,

More information

Safety and the work environment

Safety and the work environment Safety and the work environment Dr. Jack Treffner www.metaffordance.com Organisational Psychology, Psyc317-12B August 2012 Perception, Affordances and Safety Anthropometry Average sizes of the limbs of

More information

The Haptic Perception of Spatial Orientations studied with an Haptic Display

The Haptic Perception of Spatial Orientations studied with an Haptic Display The Haptic Perception of Spatial Orientations studied with an Haptic Display Gabriel Baud-Bovy 1 and Edouard Gentaz 2 1 Faculty of Psychology, UHSR University, Milan, Italy gabriel@shaker.med.umn.edu 2

More information

Exploring body holistic processing investigated with composite illusion

Exploring body holistic processing investigated with composite illusion Exploring body holistic processing investigated with composite illusion Dora E. Szatmári (szatmari.dora@pte.hu) University of Pécs, Institute of Psychology Ifjúság Street 6. Pécs, 7624 Hungary Beatrix

More information

The Mona Lisa Effect: Perception of Gaze Direction in Real and Pictured Faces

The Mona Lisa Effect: Perception of Gaze Direction in Real and Pictured Faces Studies in Perception and Action VII S. Rogers & J. Effken (Eds.)! 2003 Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. The Mona Lisa Effect: Perception of Gaze Direction in Real and Pictured Faces Sheena Rogers 1,

More information

Perceiving Affordances for Fitting Through Apertures

Perceiving Affordances for Fitting Through Apertures Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2008, Vol. 34, No. 6, 1501 1514 Copyright 2008 by the American Psychological Association 0096-1523/08/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0011393 Perceiving

More information

GROUPING BASED ON PHENOMENAL PROXIMITY

GROUPING BASED ON PHENOMENAL PROXIMITY Journal of Experimental Psychology 1964, Vol. 67, No. 6, 531-538 GROUPING BASED ON PHENOMENAL PROXIMITY IRVIN ROCK AND LEONARD BROSGOLE l Yeshiva University The question was raised whether the Gestalt

More information

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance The Perceptual Experience of Slope by Foot and by Finger Alen Hajnal, Daniel T. Abdul-Malak, and Frank H. Durgin Online First Publication,

More information

Egocentric reference frame bias in the palmar haptic perception of surface orientation. Allison Coleman and Frank H. Durgin. Swarthmore College

Egocentric reference frame bias in the palmar haptic perception of surface orientation. Allison Coleman and Frank H. Durgin. Swarthmore College Running head: HAPTIC EGOCENTRIC BIAS Egocentric reference frame bias in the palmar haptic perception of surface orientation Allison Coleman and Frank H. Durgin Swarthmore College Reference: Coleman, A.,

More information

AGING AND STEERING CONTROL UNDER REDUCED VISIBILITY CONDITIONS. Wichita State University, Wichita, Kansas, USA

AGING AND STEERING CONTROL UNDER REDUCED VISIBILITY CONDITIONS. Wichita State University, Wichita, Kansas, USA AGING AND STEERING CONTROL UNDER REDUCED VISIBILITY CONDITIONS Bobby Nguyen 1, Yan Zhuo 2, & Rui Ni 1 1 Wichita State University, Wichita, Kansas, USA 2 Institute of Biophysics, Chinese Academy of Sciences,

More information

The Effect of Frequency Shifting on Audio-Tactile Conversion for Enriching Musical Experience

The Effect of Frequency Shifting on Audio-Tactile Conversion for Enriching Musical Experience The Effect of Frequency Shifting on Audio-Tactile Conversion for Enriching Musical Experience Ryuta Okazaki 1,2, Hidenori Kuribayashi 3, Hiroyuki Kajimioto 1,4 1 The University of Electro-Communications,

More information

Visual influence on haptic torque perception

Visual influence on haptic torque perception Perception, 2012, volume 41, pages 862 870 doi:10.1068/p7090 Visual influence on haptic torque perception Yangqing Xu, Shélan O Keefe, Satoru Suzuki, Steven L Franconeri Department of Psychology, Northwestern

More information

Virtual Distance Estimation in a CAVE

Virtual Distance Estimation in a CAVE Virtual Distance Estimation in a CAVE Eric Marsh, Jean-Rémy Chardonnet, Frédéric Merienne To cite this version: Eric Marsh, Jean-Rémy Chardonnet, Frédéric Merienne. Virtual Distance Estimation in a CAVE.

More information

Visual Guidance of Walking Through Apertures: Body-Scaled Information for Affordances

Visual Guidance of Walking Through Apertures: Body-Scaled Information for Affordances Journal ofexperimentai Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 1987, Vol. 13, No.3, 371-383 Copyright 1987 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0096 1523/87/$00.75 Visual Guidance of Walking

More information

SAR Evaluation Considerations for Handsets with Multiple Transmitters and Antennas

SAR Evaluation Considerations for Handsets with Multiple Transmitters and Antennas Evaluation Considerations for Handsets with Multiple Transmitters and Antennas February 2008 Laboratory Division Office of Engineering and Techlogy Federal Communications Commission Introduction This document

More information

Perceived depth is enhanced with parallax scanning

Perceived depth is enhanced with parallax scanning Perceived Depth is Enhanced with Parallax Scanning March 1, 1999 Dennis Proffitt & Tom Banton Department of Psychology University of Virginia Perceived depth is enhanced with parallax scanning Background

More information

Scene layout from ground contact, occlusion, and motion parallax

Scene layout from ground contact, occlusion, and motion parallax VISUAL COGNITION, 2007, 15 (1), 4868 Scene layout from ground contact, occlusion, and motion parallax Rui Ni and Myron L. Braunstein University of California, Irvine, CA, USA George J. Andersen University

More information

Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion

Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion Vision Research 42 (2002) 2731 2735 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion Erich W. Graf *, Wendy J. Adams, Martin Lages Department

More information

The Relationship between the Arrangement of Participants and the Comfortableness of Conversation in HyperMirror

The Relationship between the Arrangement of Participants and the Comfortableness of Conversation in HyperMirror The Relationship between the Arrangement of Participants and the Comfortableness of Conversation in HyperMirror Osamu Morikawa 1 and Takanori Maesako 2 1 Research Institute for Human Science and Biomedical

More information

Discriminating direction of motion trajectories from angular speed and background information

Discriminating direction of motion trajectories from angular speed and background information Atten Percept Psychophys (2013) 75:1570 1582 DOI 10.3758/s13414-013-0488-z Discriminating direction of motion trajectories from angular speed and background information Zheng Bian & Myron L. Braunstein

More information

TRAFFIC SIGN DETECTION AND IDENTIFICATION.

TRAFFIC SIGN DETECTION AND IDENTIFICATION. TRAFFIC SIGN DETECTION AND IDENTIFICATION Vaughan W. Inman 1 & Brian H. Philips 2 1 SAIC, McLean, Virginia, USA 2 Federal Highway Administration, McLean, Virginia, USA Email: vaughan.inman.ctr@dot.gov

More information

Eye Movements and the Selection of Optical Information for Catching

Eye Movements and the Selection of Optical Information for Catching ECOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY, 13(2), 71 85 Copyright 2001, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Eye Movements and the Selection of Optical Information for Catching Eric L. Amazeen, Polemnia G. Amazeen, and Peter

More information

IV: Visual Organization and Interpretation

IV: Visual Organization and Interpretation IV: Visual Organization and Interpretation Describe Gestalt psychologists understanding of perceptual organization, and explain how figure-ground and grouping principles contribute to our perceptions Explain

More information

AD-A lji llllllllllii l

AD-A lji llllllllllii l Perception, 1992, volume 21, pages 359-363 AD-A259 238 lji llllllllllii1111111111111l lll~ lit DEC The effect of defocussing the image on the perception of the temporal order of flashing lights Saul M

More information

Differences in Fitts Law Task Performance Based on Environment Scaling

Differences in Fitts Law Task Performance Based on Environment Scaling Differences in Fitts Law Task Performance Based on Environment Scaling Gregory S. Lee and Bhavani Thuraisingham Department of Computer Science University of Texas at Dallas 800 West Campbell Road Richardson,

More information

The influence of exploration mode, orientation, and configuration on the haptic Mu«ller-Lyer illusion

The influence of exploration mode, orientation, and configuration on the haptic Mu«ller-Lyer illusion Perception, 2005, volume 34, pages 1475 ^ 1500 DOI:10.1068/p5269 The influence of exploration mode, orientation, and configuration on the haptic Mu«ller-Lyer illusion Morton A Heller, Melissa McCarthy,

More information

The Perception of Optical Flow in Driving Simulators

The Perception of Optical Flow in Driving Simulators University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2009 Driving Assessment Conference Jun 23rd, 12:00 AM The Perception of Optical Flow in Driving Simulators Zhishuai Yin Northeastern

More information

The ground dominance effect in the perception of 3-D layout

The ground dominance effect in the perception of 3-D layout Perception & Psychophysics 2005, 67 (5), 802-815 The ground dominance effect in the perception of 3-D layout ZHENG BIAN and MYRON L. BRAUNSTEIN University of California, Irvine, California and GEORGE J.

More information

The effect of rotation on configural encoding in a face-matching task

The effect of rotation on configural encoding in a face-matching task Perception, 2007, volume 36, pages 446 ^ 460 DOI:10.1068/p5530 The effect of rotation on configural encoding in a face-matching task Andrew J Edmondsô, Michael B Lewis School of Psychology, Cardiff University,

More information

Salient features make a search easy

Salient features make a search easy Chapter General discussion This thesis examined various aspects of haptic search. It consisted of three parts. In the first part, the saliency of movability and compliance were investigated. In the second

More information

Introduction to Psychology Prof. Braj Bhushan Department of Humanities and Social Sciences Indian Institute of Technology, Kanpur

Introduction to Psychology Prof. Braj Bhushan Department of Humanities and Social Sciences Indian Institute of Technology, Kanpur Introduction to Psychology Prof. Braj Bhushan Department of Humanities and Social Sciences Indian Institute of Technology, Kanpur Lecture - 10 Perception Role of Culture in Perception Till now we have

More information

Experiments with An Improved Iris Segmentation Algorithm

Experiments with An Improved Iris Segmentation Algorithm Experiments with An Improved Iris Segmentation Algorithm Xiaomei Liu, Kevin W. Bowyer, Patrick J. Flynn Department of Computer Science and Engineering University of Notre Dame Notre Dame, IN 46556, U.S.A.

More information

Haptic perception of spatial relations

Haptic perception of spatial relations Perception, 1999, volume 28, pages 781 ^ 795 DOI:1.168/p293 Haptic perception of spatial relations Astrid M L Kappers, Jan J Koenderink HelmholtzInstituut,Princetonplein5,3584CCUtrecht,TheNetherlands;e-mail:a.m.l.kappers@phys.uu.nl

More information

A triangulation method for determining the perceptual center of the head for auditory stimuli

A triangulation method for determining the perceptual center of the head for auditory stimuli A triangulation method for determining the perceptual center of the head for auditory stimuli PACS REFERENCE: 43.66.Qp Brungart, Douglas 1 ; Neelon, Michael 2 ; Kordik, Alexander 3 ; Simpson, Brian 4 1

More information

This is a postprint of. The influence of material cues on early grasping force. Bergmann Tiest, W.M., Kappers, A.M.L.

This is a postprint of. The influence of material cues on early grasping force. Bergmann Tiest, W.M., Kappers, A.M.L. This is a postprint of The influence of material cues on early grasping force Bergmann Tiest, W.M., Kappers, A.M.L. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 8618, 393-399 Published version: http://dx.doi.org/1.17/978-3-662-44193-_49

More information

Investigating the effect of differential elevation of food on foraging behavior of the Eastern grey squirrel (Sciurus carolinensis)

Investigating the effect of differential elevation of food on foraging behavior of the Eastern grey squirrel (Sciurus carolinensis) Investigating the effect of differential elevation of food on foraging behavior of the Eastern grey squirrel (Sciurus carolinensis) Binh Nguyen, Nima Farchadi, Stephen Schiltz University of Maryland, Department

More information

Attenuating the haptic horizontal vertical curvature illusion

Attenuating the haptic horizontal vertical curvature illusion Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 2010, 72 (6), 1626-1641 doi:10.3758/app.72.6.1626 Attenuating the haptic horizontal vertical curvature illusion MORTON A. HELLER, ANNE D. MCCLURE WALK, RITA SCHNA

More information

Journal of Biomechanics

Journal of Biomechanics Journal of Biomechanics ] (]]]]) ]]] ]]] Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Biomechanics journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jbiomech www.jbiomech.com Short communication Computation

More information

Consumer Behavior when Zooming and Cropping Personal Photographs and its Implications for Digital Image Resolution

Consumer Behavior when Zooming and Cropping Personal Photographs and its Implications for Digital Image Resolution Consumer Behavior when Zooming and Cropping Personal Photographs and its Implications for Digital Image Michael E. Miller and Jerry Muszak Eastman Kodak Company Rochester, New York USA Abstract This paper

More information

Perception of scene layout from optical contact, shadows, and motion

Perception of scene layout from optical contact, shadows, and motion Perception, 2004, volume 33, pages 1305 ^ 1318 DOI:10.1068/p5288 Perception of scene layout from optical contact, shadows, and motion Rui Ni, Myron L Braunstein Department of Cognitive Sciences, University

More information

the human chapter 1 Traffic lights the human User-centred Design Light Vision part 1 (modified extract for AISD 2005) Information i/o

the human chapter 1 Traffic lights the human User-centred Design Light Vision part 1 (modified extract for AISD 2005) Information i/o Traffic lights chapter 1 the human part 1 (modified extract for AISD 2005) http://www.baddesigns.com/manylts.html User-centred Design Bad design contradicts facts pertaining to human capabilities Usability

More information

The Influence of Visual Illusion on Visually Perceived System and Visually Guided Action System

The Influence of Visual Illusion on Visually Perceived System and Visually Guided Action System The Influence of Visual Illusion on Visually Perceived System and Visually Guided Action System Yu-Hung CHIEN*, Chien-Hsiung CHEN** * Graduate School of Design, National Taiwan University of Science and

More information

Enclosure size and the use of local and global geometric cues for reorientation

Enclosure size and the use of local and global geometric cues for reorientation Psychon Bull Rev (2012) 19:270 276 DOI 10.3758/s13423-011-0195-5 BRIEF REPORT Enclosure size and the use of local and global geometric cues for reorientation Bradley R. Sturz & Martha R. Forloines & Kent

More information

Learning relative directions between landmarks in a desktop virtual environment

Learning relative directions between landmarks in a desktop virtual environment Spatial Cognition and Computation 1: 131 144, 1999. 2000 Kluwer Academic Publishers. Printed in the Netherlands. Learning relative directions between landmarks in a desktop virtual environment WILLIAM

More information

MOTION PARALLAX AND ABSOLUTE DISTANCE. Steven H. Ferris NAVAL SUBMARINE MEDICAL RESEARCH LABORATORY NAVAL SUBMARINE MEDICAL CENTER REPORT NUMBER 673

MOTION PARALLAX AND ABSOLUTE DISTANCE. Steven H. Ferris NAVAL SUBMARINE MEDICAL RESEARCH LABORATORY NAVAL SUBMARINE MEDICAL CENTER REPORT NUMBER 673 MOTION PARALLAX AND ABSOLUTE DISTANCE by Steven H. Ferris NAVAL SUBMARINE MEDICAL RESEARCH LABORATORY NAVAL SUBMARINE MEDICAL CENTER REPORT NUMBER 673 Bureau of Medicine and Surgery, Navy Department Research

More information

The Shape-Weight Illusion

The Shape-Weight Illusion The Shape-Weight Illusion Mirela Kahrimanovic, Wouter M. Bergmann Tiest, and Astrid M.L. Kappers Universiteit Utrecht, Helmholtz Institute Padualaan 8, 3584 CH Utrecht, The Netherlands {m.kahrimanovic,w.m.bergmanntiest,a.m.l.kappers}@uu.nl

More information

Tolerances of the Resonance Frequency f s AN 42

Tolerances of the Resonance Frequency f s AN 42 Tolerances of the Resonance Frequency f s AN 42 Application Note to the KLIPPEL R&D SYSTEM The fundamental resonance frequency f s is one of the most important lumped parameter of a drive unit. However,

More information

Vision V Perceiving Movement

Vision V Perceiving Movement Vision V Perceiving Movement Overview of Topics Chapter 8 in Goldstein (chp. 9 in 7th ed.) Movement is tied up with all other aspects of vision (colour, depth, shape perception...) Differentiating self-motion

More information

Perception of room size and the ability of self localization in a virtual environment. Loudspeaker experiment

Perception of room size and the ability of self localization in a virtual environment. Loudspeaker experiment Perception of room size and the ability of self localization in a virtual environment. Loudspeaker experiment Marko Horvat University of Zagreb Faculty of Electrical Engineering and Computing, Zagreb,

More information

Vision V Perceiving Movement

Vision V Perceiving Movement Vision V Perceiving Movement Overview of Topics Chapter 8 in Goldstein (chp. 9 in 7th ed.) Movement is tied up with all other aspects of vision (colour, depth, shape perception...) Differentiating self-motion

More information

Perceived Image Quality and Acceptability of Photographic Prints Originating from Different Resolution Digital Capture Devices

Perceived Image Quality and Acceptability of Photographic Prints Originating from Different Resolution Digital Capture Devices Perceived Image Quality and Acceptability of Photographic Prints Originating from Different Resolution Digital Capture Devices Michael E. Miller and Rise Segur Eastman Kodak Company Rochester, New York

More information

Modeling a Continuous Dynamic Task

Modeling a Continuous Dynamic Task Modeling a Continuous Dynamic Task Wayne D. Gray, Michael J. Schoelles, & Wai-Tat Fu Human Factors & Applied Cognition George Mason University Fairfax, VA 22030 USA +1 703 993 1357 gray@gmu.edu ABSTRACT

More information

Chapter 8: Perceiving Motion

Chapter 8: Perceiving Motion Chapter 8: Perceiving Motion Motion perception occurs (a) when a stationary observer perceives moving stimuli, such as this couple crossing the street; and (b) when a moving observer, like this basketball

More information

Müller-Lyer Illusion Effect on a Reaching Movement in Simultaneous Presentation of Visual and Haptic/Kinesthetic Cues

Müller-Lyer Illusion Effect on a Reaching Movement in Simultaneous Presentation of Visual and Haptic/Kinesthetic Cues The 2009 IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems October 11-15, 2009 St. Louis, USA Müller-Lyer Illusion Effect on a Reaching Movement in Simultaneous Presentation of Visual

More information

Energy. Amazing. Transformers. We live with a dizzying array of electronic. Coffee Can Speakers:

Energy. Amazing. Transformers. We live with a dizzying array of electronic. Coffee Can Speakers: Coffee Can Speakers: Amazing Energy Transformers Fifth-grade students learn the science behind speakers By Kevin Wise and Monica Haake We live with a dizzying array of electronic devices cell phones, mp3

More information

PERCEIVING MOTION CHAPTER 8

PERCEIVING MOTION CHAPTER 8 Motion 1 Perception (PSY 4204) Christine L. Ruva, Ph.D. PERCEIVING MOTION CHAPTER 8 Overview of Questions Why do some animals freeze in place when they sense danger? How do films create movement from still

More information

The Influence of Restricted Viewing Conditions on Egocentric Distance Perception: Implications for Real and Virtual Environments

The Influence of Restricted Viewing Conditions on Egocentric Distance Perception: Implications for Real and Virtual Environments The Influence of Restricted Viewing Conditions on Egocentric Distance Perception: Implications for Real and Virtual Environments Sarah H. Creem-Regehr 1, Peter Willemsen 2, Amy A. Gooch 2, and William

More information

Infants perception of depth from cast shadows

Infants perception of depth from cast shadows Perception & Psychophysics 2006, 68 (1), 154-160 Infants perception of depth from cast shadows ALBERT YONAS University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota and CARL E. GRANRUD University of Northern Colorado,

More information

The Lady's not for turning: Rotation of the Thatcher illusion

The Lady's not for turning: Rotation of the Thatcher illusion Perception, 2001, volume 30, pages 769 ^ 774 DOI:10.1068/p3174 The Lady's not for turning: Rotation of the Thatcher illusion Michael B Lewis School of Psychology, Cardiff University, PO Box 901, Cardiff

More information

Quantitative Comparison of Interaction with Shutter Glasses and Autostereoscopic Displays

Quantitative Comparison of Interaction with Shutter Glasses and Autostereoscopic Displays Quantitative Comparison of Interaction with Shutter Glasses and Autostereoscopic Displays Z.Y. Alpaslan, S.-C. Yeh, A.A. Rizzo, and A.A. Sawchuk University of Southern California, Integrated Media Systems

More information

The Lightning Event. White Paper

The Lightning Event. White Paper The Lightning Event White Paper The Lightning Event Surge Protection Solutions for PTC 1 The Lightning Event There are volumes of information available on what we believe lightning is and how we think

More information

Being There: Architectural Metaphors in the Design of Virtual Place

Being There: Architectural Metaphors in the Design of Virtual Place Being There: Architectural Metaphors in the Design of Virtual Place Rivka Oxman Faculty of Architecture and Town Planning, Haifa, Israel, 32000 http://www.technion.ac.il/~oxman Abstract. The paper reports

More information

Proprioception & force sensing

Proprioception & force sensing Proprioception & force sensing Roope Raisamo Tampere Unit for Computer-Human Interaction (TAUCHI) School of Information Sciences University of Tampere, Finland Based on material by Jussi Rantala, Jukka

More information

New Skills: Finding visual cues for where characters hold their weight

New Skills: Finding visual cues for where characters hold their weight LESSON Gesture Drawing New Skills: Finding visual cues for where characters hold their weight Objectives: Using the provided images, mark the line of action, points of contact, and general placement of

More information

The Necessary Resolution to Zoom and Crop Hardcopy Images

The Necessary Resolution to Zoom and Crop Hardcopy Images The Necessary Resolution to Zoom and Crop Hardcopy Images Cathleen M. Daniels, Raymond W. Ptucha, and Laurie Schaefer Eastman Kodak Company, Rochester, New York, USA Abstract The objective of this study

More information

Baby Boomers and Gaze Enabled Gaming

Baby Boomers and Gaze Enabled Gaming Baby Boomers and Gaze Enabled Gaming Soussan Djamasbi (&), Siavash Mortazavi, and Mina Shojaeizadeh User Experience and Decision Making Research Laboratory, Worcester Polytechnic Institute, 100 Institute

More information

The vertical-horizontal illusion: Assessing the contributions of anisotropy, abutting, and crossing to the misperception of simple line stimuli

The vertical-horizontal illusion: Assessing the contributions of anisotropy, abutting, and crossing to the misperception of simple line stimuli Journal of Vision (2013) 13(8):7, 1 11 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/13/8/7 1 The vertical-horizontal illusion: Assessing the contributions of anisotropy, abutting, and crossing to the misperception

More information

The effect of 3D audio and other audio techniques on virtual reality experience

The effect of 3D audio and other audio techniques on virtual reality experience The effect of 3D audio and other audio techniques on virtual reality experience Willem-Paul BRINKMAN a,1, Allart R.D. HOEKSTRA a, René van EGMOND a a Delft University of Technology, The Netherlands Abstract.

More information

Suggestion for a Visual Dynamics Analysis Model Using a Natural Movement Model

Suggestion for a Visual Dynamics Analysis Model Using a Natural Movement Model Suggestion for a Visual Dynamics Analysis Model Using a Natural Movement Model Seung-Jae Lee 1 and Kyung-Hoon Lee* 2 1 Assistant Professor, Division of Architecture, Mokwon University, Korea 2 Professor,

More information

The influence of changing haptic refresh-rate on subjective user experiences - lessons for effective touchbased applications.

The influence of changing haptic refresh-rate on subjective user experiences - lessons for effective touchbased applications. The influence of changing haptic refresh-rate on subjective user experiences - lessons for effective touchbased applications. Stuart Booth 1, Franco De Angelis 2 and Thore Schmidt-Tjarksen 3 1 University

More information

Distance Estimation in Virtual and Real Environments using Bisection

Distance Estimation in Virtual and Real Environments using Bisection Distance Estimation in Virtual and Real Environments using Bisection Bobby Bodenheimer, Jingjing Meng, Haojie Wu, Gayathri Narasimham, Bjoern Rump Timothy P. McNamara, Thomas H. Carr, John J. Rieser Vanderbilt

More information

Here I present more details about the methods of the experiments which are. described in the main text, and describe two additional examinations which

Here I present more details about the methods of the experiments which are. described in the main text, and describe two additional examinations which Supplementary Note Here I present more details about the methods of the experiments which are described in the main text, and describe two additional examinations which assessed DF s proprioceptive performance

More information

Effect of the number of loudspeakers on sense of presence in 3D audio system based on multiple vertical panning

Effect of the number of loudspeakers on sense of presence in 3D audio system based on multiple vertical panning Effect of the number of loudspeakers on sense of presence in 3D audio system based on multiple vertical panning Toshiyuki Kimura and Hiroshi Ando Universal Communication Research Institute, National Institute

More information

Elizabeth A. Schmidlin Keith S. Jones Brian Jonhson. Texas Tech University

Elizabeth A. Schmidlin Keith S. Jones Brian Jonhson. Texas Tech University Elizabeth A. Schmidlin Keith S. Jones Brian Jonhson Texas Tech University ! After 9/11, researchers used robots to assist rescue operations. (Casper, 2002; Murphy, 2004) " Marked the first civilian use

More information

Encoding and Code Wheel Proposal for TCUT1800X01

Encoding and Code Wheel Proposal for TCUT1800X01 VISHAY SEMICONDUCTORS www.vishay.com Optical Sensors By Sascha Kuhn INTRODUCTION AND BASIC OPERATION The TCUT18X1 is a 4-channel optical transmissive sensor designed for incremental and absolute encoder

More information

PROJECT BAT-EYE. Developing an Economic System that can give a Blind Person Basic Spatial Awareness and Object Identification.

PROJECT BAT-EYE. Developing an Economic System that can give a Blind Person Basic Spatial Awareness and Object Identification. PROJECT BAT-EYE Developing an Economic System that can give a Blind Person Basic Spatial Awareness and Object Identification. Debargha Ganguly royal.debargha@gmail.com ABSTRACT- Project BATEYE fundamentally

More information

Universities of Leeds, Sheffield and York

Universities of Leeds, Sheffield and York promoting access to White Rose research papers Universities of Leeds, Sheffield and York http://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/ This is an author produced version of a paper published in Journal of Experimental

More information

Wall bracing is one of the most important structural elements of

Wall bracing is one of the most important structural elements of CONTINUINGEDUCATION Wall Bracing and the Codes Introduction Wall bracing is one of the most important structural elements of any house, but it can also be one of the most confusing, because designing houses

More information

GEOMETRIC SHAPE DETECTION WITH SOUNDVIEW. Department of Computer Science 1 Department of Psychology 2 University of British Columbia Vancouver, Canada

GEOMETRIC SHAPE DETECTION WITH SOUNDVIEW. Department of Computer Science 1 Department of Psychology 2 University of British Columbia Vancouver, Canada GEOMETRIC SHAPE DETECTION WITH SOUNDVIEW K. van den Doel 1, D. Smilek 2, A. Bodnar 1, C. Chita 1, R. Corbett 1, D. Nekrasovski 1, J. McGrenere 1 Department of Computer Science 1 Department of Psychology

More information

Supporting Online Material for

Supporting Online Material for www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/333/6042/627/dc1 Supporting Online Material for Bats Use Echo Harmonic Structure to Distinguish Their Targets from Background Clutter Mary E. Bates, * James A. Simmons,

More information

Dissociating Ideomotor and Spatial Compatibility: Empirical Evidence and Connectionist Models

Dissociating Ideomotor and Spatial Compatibility: Empirical Evidence and Connectionist Models Dissociating Ideomotor and Spatial Compatibility: Empirical Evidence and Connectionist Models Ty W. Boyer (tywboyer@indiana.edu) Matthias Scheutz (mscheutz@indiana.edu) Bennett I. Bertenthal (bbertent@indiana.edu)

More information

Exploring TeachSpin s Two-Slit Interference, One Photon at a Time Workshop Manual

Exploring TeachSpin s Two-Slit Interference, One Photon at a Time Workshop Manual Introduction Exploring TeachSpin s Nobel Laureate Richard Feynman, one of the most joyous practitioners of physics, described single photon interference as a phenomenon which is impossible, absolutely

More information

Haptic perception of linear extent

Haptic perception of linear extent Perception & Psychophysics 1999, 61 (6), 1211-1226 Haptic perception of linear extent LAURA ARMSTRONG and LAWRENCE E. MARKS John B. Pierce Laboratory and Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut The perception

More information

A Human Factors Guide to Visual Display Design and Instructional System Design

A Human Factors Guide to Visual Display Design and Instructional System Design I -W J TB-iBBT»."V^...-*.-^ -fc-. ^..-\."» LI»." _"W V"*. ">,..v1 -V Ei ftq Video Games: CO CO A Human Factors Guide to Visual Display Design and Instructional System Design '.- U < äs GL Douglas J. Bobko

More information

On the intensity maximum of the Oppel-Kundt illusion

On the intensity maximum of the Oppel-Kundt illusion On the intensity maximum of the Oppel-Kundt illusion M a b c d W.A. Kreiner Faculty of Natural Sciences University of Ulm y L(perceived) / L0 1. Illusion triggered by a gradually filled space In the Oppel-Kundt

More information

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Report

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Report Research Report RETINAL FLOW IS SUFFICIENT FOR STEERING DURING OBSERVER ROTATION Brown University Abstract How do people control locomotion while their eyes are simultaneously rotating? A previous study

More information

Poles for Increasing the Sensibility of Vertical Gradient. in a Downhill Road

Poles for Increasing the Sensibility of Vertical Gradient. in a Downhill Road Poles for Increasing the Sensibility of Vertical Gradient 1 Graduate School of Science and Engineering, Yamaguchi University 2-16-1 Tokiwadai,Ube 755-8611, Japan r007vm@yamaguchiu.ac.jp in a Downhill Road

More information

NAVIGATIONAL CONTROL EFFECT ON REPRESENTING VIRTUAL ENVIRONMENTS

NAVIGATIONAL CONTROL EFFECT ON REPRESENTING VIRTUAL ENVIRONMENTS NAVIGATIONAL CONTROL EFFECT ON REPRESENTING VIRTUAL ENVIRONMENTS Xianjun Sam Zheng, George W. McConkie, and Benjamin Schaeffer Beckman Institute, University of Illinois at Urbana Champaign This present

More information

Body Proportions. from INFANT to ADULT. Using a Pencil to Measure Heads

Body Proportions. from INFANT to ADULT. Using a Pencil to Measure Heads Level: Beginner to Intermediate Flesch-Kincaid Grade Level: 8.9 Flesch-Kincaid Reading Ease: 59.5 Drawspace Curriculum 6.1.R3-8 Pages and 17 Illustrations Body Proportions from INFANT to ADULT Using a

More information

Size Illusion on an Asymmetrically Divided Circle

Size Illusion on an Asymmetrically Divided Circle Size Illusion on an Asymmetrically Divided Circle W.A. Kreiner Faculty of Natural Sciences University of Ulm 2 1. Introduction In the Poggendorff (18) illusion a line, inclined by about 45 0 to the horizontal,

More information

Orientation-sensitivity to facial features explains the Thatcher illusion

Orientation-sensitivity to facial features explains the Thatcher illusion Journal of Vision (2014) 14(12):9, 1 10 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/14/12/9 1 Orientation-sensitivity to facial features explains the Thatcher illusion Department of Psychology and York Neuroimaging

More information

Unit IV: Sensation & Perception. Module 19 Vision Organization & Interpretation

Unit IV: Sensation & Perception. Module 19 Vision Organization & Interpretation Unit IV: Sensation & Perception Module 19 Vision Organization & Interpretation Visual Organization 19-1 Perceptual Organization 19-1 How do we form meaningful perceptions from sensory information? A group

More information

Head-Movement Evaluation for First-Person Games

Head-Movement Evaluation for First-Person Games Head-Movement Evaluation for First-Person Games Paulo G. de Barros Computer Science Department Worcester Polytechnic Institute 100 Institute Road. Worcester, MA 01609 USA pgb@wpi.edu Robert W. Lindeman

More information

Role of gravitational cues in the haptic perception of orientation

Role of gravitational cues in the haptic perception of orientation Perception & Psychophysics 1996, 58 (8), 1278 1292 Role of gravitational cues in the haptic perception of orientation EDOUARD GENTAZ and YVETTE HATWELL Université Pierre Mendès France, Grenoble, France

More information

Calibration of Distance and Size Does Not Calibrate Shape Information: Comparison of Dynamic Monocular and Static and Dynamic Binocular Vision

Calibration of Distance and Size Does Not Calibrate Shape Information: Comparison of Dynamic Monocular and Static and Dynamic Binocular Vision ECOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY, 17(2), 55 74 Copyright 2005, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Calibration of Distance and Size Does Not Calibrate Shape Information: Comparison of Dynamic Monocular and Static and

More information

Learning the Proprioceptive and Acoustic Properties of Household Objects. Jivko Sinapov Willow Collaborators: Kaijen and Radu 6/24/2010

Learning the Proprioceptive and Acoustic Properties of Household Objects. Jivko Sinapov Willow Collaborators: Kaijen and Radu 6/24/2010 Learning the Proprioceptive and Acoustic Properties of Household Objects Jivko Sinapov Willow Collaborators: Kaijen and Radu 6/24/2010 What is Proprioception? It is the sense that indicates whether the

More information