Children s age influences their perceptions of a humanoid robot as being like a person or machine.

Similar documents
This is a repository copy of Designing robot personalities for human-robot symbiotic interaction in an educational context.

This is a repository copy of You made him be alive: Children s perceptions of animacy in a humanoid robot.

This is a repository copy of Congratulations, It s a Boy! Bench-Marking Children s Perceptions of the Robokind Zeno-R25.

This is a repository copy of Don t Worry, We ll Get There: Developing Robot Personalities to Maintain User Interaction After Robot Error.

Young Children s Folk Knowledge of Robots

Proceedings of th IEEE-RAS International Conference on Humanoid Robots ! # Adaptive Systems Research Group, School of Computer Science

Impact of robot responsiveness and adult involvement on children s social behaviours in human-robot interaction

This is a repository copy of The EASEL project: Towards educational human-robot symbiotic interaction.

SEARCHING FOR SIGNS OF INTELLIGENT LIFE: AN INVESTIGATION OF YOUNG CHILDREN S BELIEFS ABOUT INTELLIGENCE AND ANIMACY. Debra L.

A SURVEY OF SOCIALLY INTERACTIVE ROBOTS

Evaluating 3D Embodied Conversational Agents In Contrasting VRML Retail Applications

Essay on A Survey of Socially Interactive Robots Authors: Terrence Fong, Illah Nourbakhsh, Kerstin Dautenhahn Summarized by: Mehwish Alam

Robotics for Children

Preliminary Investigation of Moral Expansiveness for Robots*

ENHANCED HUMAN-AGENT INTERACTION: AUGMENTING INTERACTION MODELS WITH EMBODIED AGENTS BY SERAFIN BENTO. MASTER OF SCIENCE in INFORMATION SYSTEMS

Baby Boomers and Gaze Enabled Gaming

Assess how research on the construction of cognitive functions in robotic systems is undertaken in Japan, China, and Korea

Visual Art Standards Grades P-12 VISUAL ART

Implications on Humanoid Robots in Pedagogical Applications from Cross-Cultural Analysis between Japan, Korea, and the USA

Does the Appearance of a Robot Affect Users Ways of Giving Commands and Feedback?

Care-receiving Robot as a Tool of Teachers in Child Education

Designing and Evaluating for Trust: A Perspective from the New Practitioners

Perception vs. Reality: Challenge, Control And Mystery In Video Games

The Perception of Humanoid Robots for Domestic Use in Saudi Arabia

ROBOT-ERA PROJECT: PRELIMINARY RESULTS OF ROBOTIC SERVICES IN SMART ENVIRONMENTS WITH ELDERLY PEOPLE

Social Acceptance of Humanoid Robots

ISAAC /22/2014. Disclosures. Dynamic Assessment (DA) A Holistic Approach Incorporates active teaching within the assessment process

Machine Trait Scales for Evaluating Mechanistic Mental Models. of Robots and Computer-Based Machines. Sara Kiesler and Jennifer Goetz, HCII,CMU

How Many Pixels Do We Need to See Things?

Beyond Actuated Tangibles: Introducing Robots to Interactive Tabletops

Gaze Interaction and Gameplay for Generation Y and Baby Boomer Users

John Benjamins Publishing Company

Natural Interaction with Social Robots

Cultural Differences in Social Acceptance of Robots*

Tricia Berry Director, UT Austin Women in Engineering Program Director, Texas Girls Collaborative Project txgcp.org

SECOND YEAR PROJECT SUMMARY

Drumming with a Humanoid Robot: Lessons Learnt from Designing and Analysing Human-Robot Interaction Studies

Secret-Sharing: Interactions Between a Child, Robot, and Adult

Orchestration. Lighton Phiri. Supervisors: A/Prof. Hussein Suleman Prof. Dr. Christoph Meinel HPI-CS4A, University of Cape Town

in the New Zealand Curriculum

Birth of An Intelligent Humanoid Robot in Singapore

Perception. The process of organizing and interpreting information, enabling us to recognize meaningful objects and events.

Cognitive robots and emotional intelligence Cloud robotics Ethical, legal and social issues of robotic Construction robots Human activities in many

Non Verbal Communication of Emotions in Social Robots

Augmented Home. Integrating a Virtual World Game in a Physical Environment. Serge Offermans and Jun Hu

Haptic Abilities of Freshman Engineers as Measured by the Haptic Visual Discrimination Test

Close Encounters: Spatial Distances between People and a Robot of Mechanistic Appearance *

Arbitrating Multimodal Outputs: Using Ambient Displays as Interruptions

Experimentally Manipulating Positive User Experience Based on the Fulfilment of User Needs

National Coalition for Core Arts Standards. Visual Arts Model Cornerstone Assessment: Secondary Accomplished

AI Principles, Semester 2, Week 1, Lecture 2, Cognitive Science and AI Applications. The Computational and Representational Understanding of Mind

A review of Reasoning About Rational Agents by Michael Wooldridge, MIT Press Gordon Beavers and Henry Hexmoor

An Integrated Expert User with End User in Technology Acceptance Model for Actual Evaluation

Contents. Part I: Images. List of contributing authors XIII Preface 1

2. Publishable summary

CHILDREN USE APPEARANCE AND ORIGIN OF MOTION TO CATEGORIZE ROBOTS. Mark Somanader. Thesis. for the degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE.

The Effects of Playing Violent Video Games on Youth: A Three-Year Longitudinal Study

How Explainability is Driving the Future of Artificial Intelligence. A Kyndi White Paper

Objective Data Analysis for a PDA-Based Human-Robotic Interface*

Intelligent Systems. Lecture 1 - Introduction

Catholijn M. Jonker and Jan Treur Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Department of Artificial Intelligence, Amsterdam, The Netherlands

Leading the Agenda. Everyday technology: A focus group with children, young people and their carers

INFORMATION TECHNOLOGY ACCEPTANCE BY UNIVERSITY LECTURES: CASE STUDY AT APPLIED SCIENCE PRIVATE UNIVERSITY

IRAHSS Pre-symposium Report

Four principles for selecting HCI research questions

European Robotics Research: Achievements and challenges

ON THE EVOLUTION OF TRUTH. 1. Introduction

Iowa Research Online. University of Iowa. Robert E. Llaneras Virginia Tech Transportation Institute, Blacksburg. Jul 11th, 12:00 AM


Video Game Education

Adapting Data Collection Methods for Different Participants of the User Study: to Improve the Empathic Understanding between Designers and Users

Mobilisation and Mutual Learning (MML) Action Plans on Societal Challenges

Artificial Intelligence

DECISION MAKING IN THE IOWA GAMBLING TASK. To appear in F. Columbus, (Ed.). The Psychology of Decision-Making. Gordon Fernie and Richard Tunney

School of Informatics Director of Commercialisation and Industry Engagement

Robot Thought Evaluation Summary

Keywords: Immediate Response Syndrome, Artificial Intelligence (AI), robots, Social Networking Service (SNS) Introduction

Artificial Intelligence

RepliPRI: Challenges in Replicating Studies of Online Privacy

Balance and Tension 1

Levels of Description: A Role for Robots in Cognitive Science Education

The Effect of Haptic Feedback on Basic Social Interaction within Shared Virtual Environments

Touch Perception and Emotional Appraisal for a Virtual Agent

SITUATED CREATIVITY INSPIRED IN PARAMETRIC DESIGN ENVIRONMENTS

Towards Intuitive Industrial Human-Robot Collaboration

Who Should I Blame? Effects of Autonomy and Transparency on Attributions in Human-Robot Interaction

Human Robot Dialogue Interaction. Barry Lumpkin

122 Taking Shape: Activities to Develop Geometric and Spatial Thinking, Grades K 2 P

Contents. Mental Commit Robot (Mental Calming Robot) Industrial Robots. In What Way are These Robots Intelligent. Video: Mental Commit Robots

The media equation. Reeves & Nass, 1996

1 NOTE: This paper reports the results of research and analysis

Consenting Agents: Semi-Autonomous Interactions for Ubiquitous Consent

SIGVerse - A Simulation Platform for Human-Robot Interaction Jeffrey Too Chuan TAN and Tetsunari INAMURA National Institute of Informatics, Japan The

Texture recognition using force sensitive resistors

MIN-Fakultät Fachbereich Informatik. Universität Hamburg. Socially interactive robots. Christine Upadek. 29 November Christine Upadek 1

THE MECA SAPIENS ARCHITECTURE

Design and evaluation of a telepresence robot for interpersonal communication with older adults

The effect of gaze behavior on the attitude towards humanoid robots

Robot to Human Approaches: Preliminary Results on Comfortable Distances and Preferences

CS 350 COMPUTER/HUMAN INTERACTION

Transcription:

Children s age influences their perceptions of a humanoid robot as being like a person or machine. Cameron, D., Fernando, S., Millings, A., Moore. R., Sharkey, A., & Prescott, T. Sheffield Robotics, The University of Sheffield, UK {d.s.cameron, s.fernando, a.millings., r.k.moore, a.sharkey, t.j.prescott} @sheffield.ac.uk Abstract. Models of children s cognitive development indicate that as children grow, they transition from using behavioral cues to knowledge of biology to determine a target s animacy. This paper explores the impact of children s ages and a humanoid robot s expressive behavior on their perceptions of the robot, using a simple, low-demand measure. Results indicate that children s ages have influence on their perceptions in terms of the robot s status being a person, a machine, or a composite. Younger children (aged 6) tended to rate the robot as being like a person to a substantially greater extent than older children (aged 7) did. However, additional facially-expressive cues from the robot did not substantively impact on children s responses. Implications for future HRI studies are discussed. Keywords: HRI, design, 1 Introduction 1.1 Background With the increasing development of robots as toys, social companions, and tutors targeted towards children as users in human-robot interaction (HRI), understanding the means by which children perceive and evaluate robots is critical for HRI. Determining whether HRI is a special boundary-case for children or simply part of their existing interactions with non-living objects allows the use and testing of established developmental-psychology models of children s beliefs and behaviors. One such model considers children to develop naïve theories of animacy, transitioning from reliance on observing a target s behavior to knowledge of its biology [1]. Bio-inspired robotics can be used to explore such models, altering robots appearances and behaviors to determine influence on children s perceptions of their animacy. It is important to consider the influence on HRI that humanoid robots appearance and behavior has for child users, given that young children use their animistic intuition to attribute intelligence, biology, and goals to encountered objects [1]. Current research offers mixed indications of children s perceptions of robots as animate or machine. Children aged 3-5 have a piecemeal understanding of animacy in their beliefs concerning a robot dog; while they show refinements with age to their adfa, p. 1, 2011. Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2011

attribution of animacy, they still keep mixed beliefs regarding its agency and biology [2]. With age, children are more likely to classify pictures of humanoid robots as being pictures of machines rather than living [3]. However, further HRI work indicates children of that age do not evaluate a robotic dog as different from a stuffed dog toy in terms of animacy, biology, and mental states [4]. In sum, factors influencing children s perceptions of humanoid robots as persons or machines still remain to be uncovered. 1.2 Use Case The Expressive Agents for Symbiotic Education and Learning (EASEL) project explores human robot symbiotic interaction (HRSI) with a view to understand the development of symbiosis over long-term robot and child tutoring interactions. Symbiosis is the capacity for both the robot and the human user to mutually influence each other s behavior both within and across repeated encounters. To suitably explore HRSI, a robot needs to be responsive to the behavior and affective states of the human user and adapt its own behavior in ways that have meaningful and measurable influence on the person. This responsiveness in a Synthetic Tutoring Assistant (STA) may range from broad changes, such as tailoring tutoring style to meet a student s learning requirements, to focused changes, such as a robot s use of simulated affect expression when giving feedback. Early research in the EASEL project [5] indicates that the presence of life-like facial expressions from a humanoid robot (Robokind Zeno R50 [6]) during feedback from the robot regarding children s game performance has a differential impact based on demographics. One mechanism proposed for this effect is that the presence of facial expressions might encourage the user to respond to the robot as a social agent or person rather than a machine or object. Perceptions of the robot as a social agent or a machine may in turn have important influence on users attitudes and behaviors during HRI. As outlined above, age is a key factor likely to influence the target demographics perceptions of the STA as a social agent. This paper explores the influence that a user s age and the presence of life-like facial expressions by the robot impact on user perceptions of the robot as a social agent. We anticipate that younger children will rate the humanoid Zeno R25 as being significantly more like a person than older children will. This difference is anticipated to be strengthened by the presence of life-like robotic facial expressions. 2 Method 2.1 Design A repeated measures design was employed so that differences in responses to the facially-expressive and non-expressive states of the robot could be explored. Allocation to condition was counterbalanced so that any order effects of the repeated exposure to the robot could be accounted for. The study took place at a local primary

school across two days, allowing for approximately a day s break for participants between conditions. 2.2 Participants Children from UK school years two and three were recruited from a local primary school to take part in the study by invite to play a game with Zeno the robot. 44 children volunteered to take part in the study and 39 completed both conditions. Of completers, there were 20 female and 19 male participants; 15 participants were from year 2 (age M = 6.38, S.E. =.07) and 24 were from year 3 (age M = 7.40, S.E. =.05). These age groups were identified in previous work as being engaged with the interaction and capable of completing self-report measures [5]. 2.3 Measures The primary measure for assessing the children s perceptions of the robot was a single item 100 point thermometer scale on which children pick a point for the robot ranging from Zeno is more like a machine at the 0 point mark and Zeno is more like a person at the 100 point mark. This measure had been pre-tested in a pilot study on adults and children and had been identified as suitable for children to articulate the differences between the two end points. Zeno had been described as a robot in the recruitment phase of the study and so use of the word robot as an anchor point might have unduly primed children s responses. This paper details part of a broader study concerning children s feelings, thoughts, and behavior in HRI, so it was necessary to use minimal items for measures to reduce overburdening participants with questions. Additional items used to control for children s interaction and understanding of the game consisted of: children s number of correct actions in the game and recognition of the robot s expression Zeno was pleased / was disappointed / didn t mind, when I got an answer right / wrong. 2.4 Procedure This study was developed to replicate procedure from a prior field-study [5] in a new, controlled testing environment. The experiment took place in a local primary school, where participants completed the game under the supervision of the research staff and one member of school staff. Information regarding participation was sent before recruitment and informed consent was obtained from parents. During the game, children were free to position themselves within a designated play zone (so that movements could be correctly detected by the system) marked out by tape starting 1.80m from the robot and extending to 3.66m away. Interaction with Zeno took the form of the well-known game of Simon Says, which initiated once children stepped into the play zone and was autonomously delivered by the robot, including instructions for the children, commands to obey (or not),

and feedback on the children s performance reported as their score 1. Children played a maximum of ten rounds and the game was repeated to present the alternate condition for the child on the next day of testing. After each session, children completed a brief self-report questionnaire, including the critical measures described above. Participant experimenter interaction consistency was maintained across sessions for all tasks and the experimenter remained blind to condition throughout. The sole experimental manipulation coincided with Zeno s vocal feedback after each game turn by including happy or sad facial animations corresponding to the vocal feedback on the children s performance. By contrast, in the control condition, Zeno s facial expression remained static when delivering feedback. 3 Results There was a significant main effect for age F(1,37) = 7.92, p <.01. Children in the younger age group rate Zeno as being more like being a person (M = 77.27, S.E. = 8.83) to a substantially greater extent than those in the older age group (M = 41.58, S.E. = 6.98). This is a large effect observed (d =.95), which post-hoc tests indicate to be sufficiently powered (.88). There was no main effect for gender F(1,37) =.71 p =.41, as both boys and girls tended to rate Zeno as being a mix of a machine and a person (Ms = 48.61 & 58.68, S.E.s = 8.57 & 8.35 respectively). Results did not materially change when controlling for number of correct responses, nor children s perceptions of Zeno s responses to their actions. There was a strong correlation in children s responses between the two conditions r(39) =.77, p <.01. There was no main effect seen for the presence of robotic facial expressions F(1,37) =.87, p =.36 as children reported similar scores for rating Zeno as a person or machine in the facially-expressive condition (M = 59.49, S.E. = 6.07) and the nonexpressive condition (M = 55.36, S.E. 6.03). Similarly, there was no observed interaction effects between the condition and age groups of participants F(1,37) =.61, p =.44 (See Figure 1), gender F(1,37) <.01, p =.99, nor order in which conditions were presented F(1,37) <.01, p =.94. 1 Full details of the game s procedure can be found in [5], which was unchanged for this study

Mean rating of Zeno's Staus 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 Year 2 Year 3 Non Expressive Expressive Fig. 1. Mean ratings of children s perceptions of Zeno as being like a machine (lower values) or like a person (higher values) 4 Discussion In line with existing literature [3, 4], on average older children considered the Zeno robot to be significantly more like a machine than the younger children did. Critically, this large effect was demonstrated using a simple and accessible measure that could be included in future studies without overburdening young participants. Consistency in ratings between conditions was high; although the inclusion of expressions did not substantively affect children s ratings for either age group, this early work might have seen ceiling effects rating Zeno as like a person due to its apparent autonomous movement and response to the children. In particular for young children, these cues could be instrumental in their regarding a humanoid robot as being like a person [1]. While not formally recorded in the current study, think-aloud reasoning by the children on making a judgment included statements such as He talks like a person but he s got oil and gears, (when rating as more like a machine) and He s like a person because he knows when I move (when rating as a person). These suggest possible use of behavioral cues and biological/mechanical knowledge to inform their judgements and further work exploring why children are making particular ratings is recommended. The results have implications for the use of Zeno in the planned tutoring role as an STA [7] in both a social, tutoring capacity and a user-acceptance capacity. Firstly, it may impact on the potential for Zeno to act as a co-learner or tutor for scenarios concerning biology or health education. For example, user engagement in inquiry learning through comparative work on the differences between machines and people may be influenced by a child s perception of Zeno as like a machine or person. Second, a

child s perspective of whether Zeno is like a machine or person may impact on the type of behaviors the user expects form the robot. Sufficient differences between expected and observed robotic features or behaviors may give rise to unease, as predicted by models of the uncanny valley [8] and potentially impact on user engagement. Further work exploring if and how children s perceptions of a robot s status as being like a person or machine can impact on the practical parameters within which HRI can occur is recommended, particularly with regards to user engagement. Additional future work could benefit from longitudinal data collection to consider changes with age within individuals, or including older children to explore age-related trends in children s beliefs. Further adaptation of the study design could include comparison of robots with closer or further resemblance to humans or animals in appearance or behavior to explore cues children draw upon to inform their perceptions of robots and potentially their interactions. 5 Acknowledgments This work is supported by the European Union Seventh Framework Programme (FP7-ICT-2013-10) under grant agreement no. 611971. We wish to acknowledge the contribution of all project partners to the ideas investigated in this study. 6 References 1. Carey, S.. Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT press, 1985. 2. Okita, Sandra Y., and Daniel L. Schwartz.. Young children's understanding of animacy and entertainment robots. International Journal of Humanoid Robotics 3(3) (2006) 393-412 3. Saylor, Megan M., Mark Somanader, Daniel T. Levin, and Kazuhiko Kawamura. How do young children deal with hybrids of living and non living things: The case of humanoid robots. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 28(4) (2010) 835-851 4. Kahn Jr, Peter H., Batya Friedman, Deanne R. Perez-Granados, and Nathan G. Freier.. Robotic pets in the lives of preschool children. In CHI'04 extended abstracts on Human factors in computing systems. ACM (2004) 1449-1452 5. Cameron, D., Fernando, S., Collins, E., Millings, A., Moore, R.K., Sharkey, A., Evers, V., & Prescott, T.. Presence of life-like robot expressions influences children s enjoyment of human-robot interactions in the field. To appear in: AISB2015: Proceedings of the symposium on New Frontiers in Human-Robot Interaction, (2015) 6. Hanson, D., Baurmann, S., Riccio, T., Margolin, R., Dockins, T., Tavares, M., & Carpenter, K.. Zeno: A cognitive character. AI Magazine (2009) 9-11 7. Fernando, S., Collins, E.C., Duff, A., Moore, R.K., Verschure, P.F.M.J., & Prescott, T.J. Optimising Robot Personalities for Symbiotic Interaction. In Biomimetic and Biohybrid Systems, Springer International Publishing, (2014) 392-395. 8. Moore, Roger K. A Bayesian explanation of the Uncanny Valley effect and related psychological phenomena. Scientific reports 2(864) (2012).