The Quality of Appearance

Similar documents
ABSTRACT. Keywords: Color image differences, image appearance, image quality, vision modeling 1. INTRODUCTION

On Contrast Sensitivity in an Image Difference Model

Meet icam: A Next-Generation Color Appearance Model

On Contrast Sensitivity in an Image Difference Model

The Effect of Opponent Noise on Image Quality

Multiscale model of Adaptation, Spatial Vision and Color Appearance

icam06, HDR, and Image Appearance

COLOR APPEARANCE IN IMAGE DISPLAYS

ABSTRACT. Keywords: color appearance, image appearance, image quality, vision modeling, image rendering

MEASURING IMAGES: DIFFERENCES, QUALITY AND APPEARANCE

Color appearance in image displays

icam06: A refined image appearance model for HDR image rendering

Review of graininess measurements

Digital Radiography using High Dynamic Range Technique

Using Color Appearance Models in Device-Independent Color Imaging. R. I. T Munsell Color Science Laboratory

The Quantitative Aspects of Color Rendering for Memory Colors

Quantifying mixed adaptation in cross-media color reproduction

A new algorithm for calculating perceived colour difference of images

Color Reproduction Algorithms and Intent

Color Appearance Models

Evaluation of image quality of the compression schemes JPEG & JPEG 2000 using a Modular Colour Image Difference Model.

The Performance of CIECAM02

Munsell Color Science Laboratory Rochester Institute of Technology

Effective Color: Materials. Color in Information Display. What does RGB Mean? The Craft of Digital Color. RGB from Cameras.

Viewing Environments for Cross-Media Image Comparisons

Brightness Calculation in Digital Image Processing

Visibility of Ink Dots as Related to Dot Size and Visual Density

COLOR IMAGE QUALITY EVALUATION USING GRAYSCALE METRICS IN CIELAB COLOR SPACE

Subjective Rules on the Perception and Modeling of Image Contrast

ISSN Vol.03,Issue.29 October-2014, Pages:

Mark D. Fairchild and Garrett M. Johnson Munsell Color Science Laboratory, Center for Imaging Science Rochester Institute of Technology, Rochester NY

Influence of Background and Surround on Image Color Matching

Appearance Match between Soft Copy and Hard Copy under Mixed Chromatic Adaptation

Edge-Raggedness Evaluation Using Slanted-Edge Analysis

Visibility of Uncorrelated Image Noise

Visual Perception. Overview. The Eye. Information Processing by Human Observer

Keywords Perceptual gamut, display color gamut, digital projector. h ab

Edge-Aware Color Appearance

Optimizing color reproduction of natural images

Color Appearance, Color Order, & Other Color Systems

Using modern colour difference formulae in the graphic arts

A New Metric for Color Halftone Visibility

General-Purpose Gamut-Mapping Algorithms: Evaluation of Contrast-Preserving Rescaling Functions for Color Gamut Mapping

High-Dynamic-Range Scene Compression in Humans

Visual sensitivity to color errors in images of natural scenes

Reprint. Journal. of the SID

Modified Jointly Blue Noise Mask Approach Using S-CIELAB Color Difference

Reference Free Image Quality Evaluation

Addressing the colorimetric redundancy in 11-ink color separation

Using HDR display technology and color appearance modeling to create display color gamuts that exceed the spectrum locus

Perception of sparkle in anti-glare display screens

VU Rendering SS Unit 8: Tone Reproduction

WestminsterResearch

CSE512 :: 6 Feb Color. Jeffrey Heer University of Washington

Out of the Box vs. Professional Calibration and the Comparison of DeltaE 2000 & Delta ICtCp

HDR, displays & low-level vision

HOW CLOSE IS CLOSE ENOUGH? SPECIFYING COLOUR TOLERANCES FOR HDR AND WCG DISPLAYS

Update on the INCITS W1.1 Standard for Evaluating the Color Rendition of Printing Systems

Image Distortion Maps 1

Color. Maneesh Agrawala Jessica Hullman. CS : Visualization Fall Assignment 3: Visualization Software

EVALUATION OF THE CHROMATIC INDUCTION INTENSITY ON MUNKER-WHITE SAMPLES

Construction Features of Color Output Device Profiles

Adding Local Contrast to Global Gamut Mapping Algorithms

Image Quality Metrics: Applications and Requirements

The Principles of Chromatics

Simulation of film media in motion picture production using a digital still camera

Investigations of the display white point on the perceived image quality

Issues in Color Correcting Digital Images of Unknown Origin

Color Correction for Tone Reproduction

Chapter 3 Part 2 Color image processing

The Influence of Luminance on Local Tone Mapping

Color Gamut Mapping Using Spatial Comparisons

The human visual system

Appearance at the low-radiance end of HDR vision: Achromatic & Chromatic

Color Science. What light is. Measuring light. CS 4620 Lecture 15. Salient property is the spectral power distribution (SPD)


Contrast, Luminance and Colour

Visual assessment of object color chroma and colorfulness

How to Make Life More Colorful: From Image Quality to Atmosphere Experience

MODIFICATION OF ADAPTIVE LOGARITHMIC METHOD FOR DISPLAYING HIGH CONTRAST SCENES BY AUTOMATING THE BIAS VALUE PARAMETER

Downloaded From: on 06/25/2015 Terms of Use:

Perception to visualization I

A BRIGHTNESS MEASURE FOR HIGH DYNAMIC RANGE TELEVISION

Report #17-UR-049. Color Camera. Jason E. Meyer Ronald B. Gibbons Caroline A. Connell. Submitted: February 28, 2017

Illuminant Multiplexed Imaging: Basics and Demonstration

DIGITAL IMAGING. Handbook of. Wiley VOL 1: IMAGE CAPTURE AND STORAGE. Editor-in- Chief

Image Processing by Bilateral Filtering Method

CSE 332/564: Visualization. Fundamentals of Color. Perception of Light Intensity. Computer Science Department Stony Brook University

Quantitative Analysis of Tone Value Reproduction Limits

ADAPTIVE ENHANCEMENT OF LUMINANCE AND DETAILS IN IMAGES UNDER AMBIENT LIGHT

Spatio-Temporal Retinex-like Envelope with Total Variation

EFFECT OF FLUORESCENT LIGHT SOURCES ON HUMAN CONTRAST SENSITIVITY Krisztián SAMU 1, Balázs Vince NAGY 1,2, Zsuzsanna LUDAS 1, György ÁBRAHÁM 1

A model of consistent colour appearance

The HDR Photographic Survey

Does CIELUV Measure Image Color Quality?

Quantitative Analysis of ICC Profile Quality for Scanners

A simulation tool for evaluating digital camera image quality

Objective Image Quality Assessment of Color Prints

Perceptual image attribute scales derived from overall image quality assessments

Enhancement of Perceived Sharpness by Chroma Contrast

Transcription:

ABSTRACT The Quality of Appearance Garrett M. Johnson Munsell Color Science Laboratory, Chester F. Carlson Center for Imaging Science Rochester Institute of Technology 14623-Rochester, NY (USA) Corresponding author: garrett@cis.rit.edu With CIECAM02, a new level of accuracy was added to color appearance models capability predicting the appearance of simple color stimuli. Recently a new type of image appearance model, called icam, has been introduced to model the perception of complex image stimuli. This image appearance model is capable of predicting many spatial phenomena such as simultaneous contrast, crispening, and spreading. The models image appearance framework can be extended to predict image differences and image quality, through the addition of spatial filtering. The application of icam as an image difference metric, as well as an image quality metric is discussed. This paper also addresses the application of linear filters in a variety of color spaces and their impact on image quality prediction. 1. INTRODUCTION Color appearance models, in essence, are tools that translate numbers into words. The inputs to these models are numerical measurements such as luminance and the tristimulus values of a stimulus and viewing conditions. The output are words describing what the stimulus looks like. These words are the color appearance attributes, such as brightness, lightness, colorfulness, chroma, and saturation. We often use numerical modifiers with these words to describe the amount of each attribute but their main purpose is to describe how a stimulus actually appears. CIECAM02 represents the state-of-the-art in color appearance models and has been shown to accurately predict the appearance of simple stimuli across a variety of lighting and viewing conditions. 1 CIECAM02 is capable of predicting many well-known appearance phenomena, such as the Hunt and Stevens effects in which viewing conditions such as the background and surround greatly influence the appearance of a stimulus. These viewing conditions are well specified and understood when dealing with patches on a uniform background, but are less understood for dealing with spatially complex stimuli such as images. A different type of color appearance model, termed an image appearance model, has been suggested to handle spatially complex stimuli. These types of models can be considered a hybrid between models of temporal and spatial human vision and traditional color appearance. An image appearance model extends color appearance to include additional attributes relating to images. These attributes, described by Engeldrum 2 as the nesses, include such perceptions as contrast, graininess, and sharpness. To understand how an image looks, it is necessary to quantify these perceptions. A framework for describing image appearance, called icam, has been introduced. 3,4 By accurately describing the appearance of an image it is possible to extend the use of this type of model to describe image quality. This is typically achieved by predicting the quality difference between two images through the addition of spatial filtering as a pre-processing step. Image appearance attributes and the use of an image appearance model for predicting image quality are discussed in more detail below. 2. IMAGE APPEARANCE ATTRIBUTES Color appearance research has identified and specifically defined several attributes that adequately describe the perceptual response to a stimulus. These are, lightness, chroma and hue for relative appearance matching, and brightness and colorfulness for absolute appearance matching. A sixth attribute of saturation has also been defined. With these terms it is possible to describe what a

color looks like. Traditional image quality research has also focused on describing several attributes that affect the appearance and thus the quality of a given image. These image quality approaches have traditionally been split into two distinct groups: an imaging systems approach, and a human visual system approach. 2,5,6 The imaging systems approach typically measures physical parameters such as resolution, MTF, grain, and gamut volume. THen through a weighted metric, it forms a model of how these parameters influence image quality. The human visual system approach typically uses metrics that approximate the behavior of the human visual system, such as the contrast sensitivity function that describes the threshold sensitivity to spatial frequencies. One potential issue with either of these approaches when dealing with image quality and appearance that is routinely, and perhaps dangerously, ignored is that of image dependence. What may be detrimental to quality for one image may actually be good for another. This can be especially problematic if image quality/appearance attributes are measured completely independent of an image. An example of this may be the appearance attribute of graininess or its system correlate of grain, a low-frequency noise pattern. This attribute is typically measured using a CSF weighted metric measured off a uniform patch. This may not always correlate with the actual perception of graininess. in an image. An example of this is shown in Figure 1, where the same grain pattern is added to two images and is perceptible in one, but not the other. This is an example of visual masking, and some models of the human visual system take this into account. 7,8 Figure 1. Graininess perception in two images. Original images (top) have the same grain pattern added to them (center) though it is not perceptible in the tiger image (bottom left). Care must be taken when measuring other image appearance attributes, just as when quantifying color appearance attributes. An example of this is contrast, which is often described as the relationship between the darkest and brightest element in an image (also called Michelson contrast). The Stevens effect and Bartleson-Breneman equations suggests that the perception of contrast changes as a function of both overall luminance and the luminance of the surround. Simultaneous contrast also indicates that the color of the background also influences the perception of contrast. This is illustrated in Figure 2. What this suggests is that contrast cannot be described as simply the relationship between the darkest and brightest pixels in an image. Traditional color appearance models are able to account for changes in surround and overall luminance, but they fall short in accounting for local and simultaneous contrast. This is partly because the background is ill-defined for complex stimuli, is it the average of all pixels, 20% gray, or a localized average?. Another interesting image perception is that of sharpness. This is often taken to be the integrated weight of MTF of an imaging system, or other similar metrics. Sharpness, like contrast and graininess, should not be measured independent of an image, as it is an attribute of the image itself.

This is evident in Figure 3, which shows that sharpness is both a function of image content and viewing conditions (such as viewing distance.) Figure 2. Contrast as a function of local background. The integrated background for all three sections is identical as well as the relationship between the minimum and maximum pixel values, though the perceived contrast of each scale appears different. Figure 3. Image appearance attribute of sharpness as a function of viewing distance and image content. The top row images have been filtered identically. At a far viewing distance all the images look sharper but when viewed close up the portrait images are generally considered unnatural while the mountain still looks sharper.

Another interesting concept when dealing with image appearance is the idea of adaptation. Chromatic adaptation has been well studied in the color appearance world, and chromatic adaptation transforms are the fundamental basis for color appearance models. 9 Chromatic adaptation plays an important role in image appearance, though it is less well understood than for simple color stimuli. Afterall, what is considered the adapting stimulus when examining a complex image? Other forms of adaptation are important for quantifying image appearance. These includes luminance and local contrast adaptation, as well as spatial frequency adaptation. Spatial frequency adaptation is where the spatial frequency sensitivity of the human visual system is altered as a function of frequencies present in the scene. In essence, this suggests that the appearance of an image can be affected by the content of the image itself as we have seen in Figure 3. More details about spatial frequency, light, and contrast adaptation and their effects on image quality and appearance can be found in Webster 10 and Johnson. 11 3. IMAGE APPEARANCE MODELING AND IMAGE QUALITY A framework for an image appearance model called icam has been formulated and can be used to predict image quality. 4 Specifically this framework is designed to predict quality differences against a reference or ideal image. The general flowchart for the icam model as used to predict image quality is shown in Figure 4. Figure 4. Flowchart for using icam as an Image Quality Metric. To begin, two images are passed into the icam model. These images are first spatiallyfiltered using a two-dimensional contrast sensitivity function in an opponent (luminance, red-green, yellow-blue) color space. This approach is based on the S-CIELAB spatial pre-processing model. 12 The spatial filters serve to remove information where it is not visible and normalize all spatial frequencies so that the perceived color differences are equal. In essence, this means that it will take more of a color difference at higher spatial frequencies to match a color difference at a lower frequency. The general behavior of the spatial filters is low-pass for the chromatic channels and bandpass for the luminance channels. The choice of opponent color space is crucial for this step as it is necessary that the luminance and chromatic channels be mathematically orthogonal. Problems can arise when this is not the case, as shown in Figure 5. If the chromatic channels contain luminance information there can be chromatic or achromatic leakage when different spatial filters are applied. This is shown by the color fringing evident in Figure 5. The icam framework performs spatial filtering in a orthogonal color space called Y C 1 C 2. More details on this space can be found in Johnson. 13 The contrast sensitivity functions are also subjected to a spatial frequency adaptation based upon the frequency content of the image and local background. 11 This allows the framework to predict image dependent sharpness differences as described above and tends to shift the peak sensitivity of the luminance CSF to higher frequencies. The next stage in the icam framework is a local von Kries type chromatic adaptation. This is similar in nature to that used in CIECAM02, though it is performed using a low-passed version of the

image itself. This facilitates the prediction of overall color balance shifts, as well as local color changes. A local contrast predictor, again based upon a low-passed version of the image itself, follows the chromatic adaptation stage. The local contrast predictor allows the model to accommodate simultaneous contrast changes and can give an estimate of the overall contrast changes between the two images. The final step is a transform into a uniform color space for pixel-by-pixel color appearance calculations. The color appearance of any given pixel is now governed by the individual image content, as well as the overall viewing conditions. A pixel-by-pixel color difference can be taken to get an appearance change map between the two images. Figure 5. Dangers of spatial filtering in a non-orthogonal color space. The figure on the left has been filtered in an orthogonal space (luminance separate from chromatic) while the figure on the right was filtered in a nonorthogonal space. Chromatic fringing is evident in the right figure, because the luminance and chromatic filters are different sizes. Image appearance attributes and their relationship to image quality can be evaluated at every stage of calculation. The overall difference between the two images may provide an indication of overall quality changes, but often will not provide enough information to determine whether those changes result in an increase or decrease in quality or what the overall quality differences are caused by. If a single dimension is being examined, for instance examining the relationship between dynamic range and quality, then it is quite possible that the overall image difference is adequate for determining quality. However, if multiple attributes are changing, then it may be necessary to examine the appearance at each stage in the difference calculations. An analogous situation is with traditional CIE color difference calculations. A color difference between two stimuli is calculated using one of the suggested formulas, such as ΔE 94 or CIEDE2000, which provides an estimate for overall perceived differences between the stimuli. This does not provide an estimate as to what direction that change is. To see the direction of change it is necessary to examine the individual lightness, chroma and hue changes (ΔL, ΔC, Δh). The image appearance map provides insight into the overall magnitude of changes, but it does not show the directions causing the change. The individual image attributes must be examined to understand the root cause of the differences. 4. CONCLUSIONS Image appearance extends the world of color appearance to account for complex spatial stimuli. Image quality can be thought of in terms of overall image appearance, whereas image quality is a function of several attributes such as color, sharpness, contrast, and graininess. As such, being able to accurately predict the appearance of an image should be useful as a method for also predicting overall image quality. Several potential pitfalls in the prediction of image quality have been discussed. These are especially important to consider when using image independent metrics of quality, either based upon image systems parameters or models of the human visual system. The visual system constantly adapts to its environment and this includes the stimulus itself. It is crucial to consider this constant change in

adaptation if we are ever to achieve the goal of an image quality metric that behaves as a human would. Example image appearance attributes such as graininess, contrast and sharpness, and how these attributes are influenced by the image content itself were given. These examples illustrate why it is important to consider image content when evaluating quality. An image appearance framework, icam, and its use as a image quality metric was also discussed. Provisions for image dependent luminance, chromatic, and spatial frequency adaptation are included in this framework. The framework described was designed as an introduction to how an appearance model could be used to predict quality differences between two images. This method is also referred to as image quality modelling with a reference. Using image attributes such as perceived color appearance, contrast and sharpness as guidance, it may also be possible to use this type of model as a reference free quality metric. This would involve specific quantifications of all individual attributes, as well as an overall weighting of those attributes on image quality. Acknowledgments The author wishes to thank those who have generously supported research on image appearance in the Munsell Color Science Laboratory. This includes Xerox, Eastman Kodak, IBM, Fuji, OCE, NYSTAR, and the DCI Postdoctoral Fellowship. References 1 CIE, CIE TC8-01 Technical Report, A Colour Appearance Model for Color Management Systems: CIECAM02, CIE Pub. 159 (2004). 2 P.G. Engeldrum, A framework for image quality models, J. Imag. Sci. Tech, 39, 312-318 (1995). 3 M.D. Fairchild and G.M. Johnson, Meet icam: A next-generation color appearance model, IS&T/SID 10 th Color Imaging Conference, Scottsdale, 33-38 (2002). 4 M.D. Fairchild and G.M. Johnson, The icam framework for image appearance, image differences, and image quality, J. of Electronic Imaging, 13, 126-138 (2004). 5 G.M. Johnson and M.D. Fairchild, Measuring images: Differences, quality, and appearance, SPIE/IS&T Electronic Imaging Conference, 5007, 51-60 (2003). 6 B.W. Keelan, Handbook of Image Quality: Characterization and Prediction, Marcel Dekker, New York, NY (2002). 7 S. Daly, The Visible Differences Predictor: An algorithm for the assessment of image fidelity, in Digital Images and Human Vision, A. Watson, Ed., MIT, Cambridge MA, 179-206 (1993). 8 J.A. Ferwerda, S. Pattanaik, P. Shirley, and D.P. Greenberg, A model of visual masking for computer graphics, Proc. of ACM SIGGRAPH, 143-152 (1997). 9 M.D. Fairchild, Color Appearance Models, 2 nd Ed., John Wiley & Sons, West Sussex, England (2005). 10 M.A. Webster, Light adaptation, contrast adaptation, and human color vision, in Color Perception: Mind and the physical world, R. Mausfeld and D. Heyer, Ed., Oxford University Press, Oxford UK, (2003). 11 G.M. Johnson and M.D. Fairchild, On contrast sensitivity in an image difference model, IS&T PICS 2002, Portland, 18-23 (2002). 12 X. Zhang and B.A. Wandell, A spatial extension of CIELAB for digital color image reproduction, SID 96 Digest, (1996). 13 G.M. Johnson and M.D. Fairchild, The Effect of Opponent Noise on Image Quality, SPIE/IS&T Electronic Imaging Conference, San Jose, (2005).