COMPARATIVE FEEDING BEHAVIOR OF IMMATURE AND ADULT HERRING GULLS NICOLAAS A. M. VERBEEK

Similar documents
GULLS WINTERING IN FLORIDA: CHRISTMAS BIRD COUNT ANALYSIS. Elizabeth Anne Schreiber and Ralph W. Schreiber. Introduction

Identification of immature Mediterranean Gulls

Greenlaw Mountain Hawk Watch Fall 2011

Recent work on the development of feeding skills by young birds has

EEB 4260 Ornithology. Lecture Notes: Migration

INTERBREEDING OF THE GLAUCOUS-WINGED GULL AND WESTERN GULL IN THE PACIFIC NORTHWEST

Many passerines defend territories in the non-breeding season, either as permanent residents (e.g., Verbeek 1973, Morton and Shalter 1977)

Sea Birds. Copyright 2012 LessonSnips

AERIAL SURVEY OF BIRDS AT MONO LAKE ON AUGUST 24, 1973

Roberts Bank Terminal 2 Project Field Studies Information Sheet

Red-breasted Merganser Minnesota Conservation Summary

Are Horseshoe Crab Eggs a Limiting Resource for Red Knots?

OBSERVATIONS ON WATERBIRDS AT JEROME PARK RESERVOIR DURING THE COLD WINTER OF

Protecting Beach-nesting Birds in Louisiana VOLUNTEER TRAINING

4/24/08. Behavioral Ecology / Evolutionary Ecology

Greenlaw Mountain Hawk Watch Fall 2014

HERON AND EGRET MONITORING RESULTS AT WEST MARIN ISLAND: 2003 NESTING SEASON

LOUISIANA BIRD RECORDS COMMITTEE

Report to the New York State Department of Environmental Conservation Common Tern (Sterna hirundo) Research and Management Oneida Lake, New York 2015

MUD, BIRDS and POPPYCOCK*

Report on the Black Headed Gull Ringing Project

Working with wildlife A DAY IN THE LIFE

Effect of laying date on chick production in Oyster catcher s and Herring Gulls

SEASONAL CHANGES IN WOOD DUCK ROOSTING FLIGHT HABITS

Wood Stork Nesting Population Survey Results 2016 and Radio-tracking Dice

LOUISIANA BIRD RECORDS COMMITTEE

FEMALE-FEMALE PAIRING AND SEX RATIOS IN GULLS: AN HISTORICAL PERSPECTIVE

Sanderling. Appendix A: Birds. Calidris alba. New Hampshire Wildlife Action Plan Appendix A Birds-67

Differential Timing of Spring Migration between Sex and Age Classes of Yellow-rumped Warblers (Setophaga coronata) in Central Alberta,

Danish White Storks in south-west England

THE SHY ALBATROSS (THALASSARCHE CAUTA):

Second Term Extra Credit: Bald Eagle Field Study America s most prestigious bird of prey

Walking beaches, volunteers amass data on dead seabirds 8 November 2017, by Phuong Le

BIOL 426. Ornithology Spring 2011

Increased use of intertidal resources benefits breeding success in a generalist gull species

SEABIRDS. Background WATER SEDIMENTS SHORELINES USES

Seasonal changes in the response of oystercatchers Haematopus ostralegus to human disturbance

Behavioral Adaptations for Survival 1. Co-evolution of predator and prey ( evolutionary arms races )

AUTUMN HAWK MIGRATIONS AT FORT JOHNSON, CHARLESTON, S.C.

Lasiurus blossevillii (Red Bat)

Atlantic. O n t h e. One of the best parts of fall is hearing the cacophony of honking,

THE SPRING MIGRATION OF THE OVER EUROPE.

Greenlaw Mountain Hawk Watch Fall 2012

Anticipate your Subject Part # 1

CLASS FOUR: Seabird Research Tools and Methods

Phaetusa simplex (Large-billed Tern)

Protocol for Censusing Yellow-billed Magpies (Pica nuttalli) at Communal Roosts

Thanks for invitation to attend this workshop. Michael asked if I would talk about puffins in the UK particularly the studies I ve been involved in

Semipalmated Sandpiper

Oregon Dunes National Recreation Area BCS Number: 47-5

Central California. 600,000 breeding seabirds + 8 million people (SF Bay Area) Potential for disturbance is high!

Project Title: Migration patterns, habitat use, and harvest characteristics of long-tailed ducks wintering on Lake Michigan.

Acknowledgments. Special thanks to Devin Givens, Inverness Yacht Club members, and Ryan Bartling, California Department of Fish and Wildlife

State of the Estuary Report 2015

Maryland Coastal Bays Colonial Waterbird and Islands Report 2018

DIET AND FORAGING BEHAVIOR OF RING-BILLED GULLS BREEDING AT DOG LAKE, MANITOBA

NATIONAL PARK AUTHORITY REPORT ON PEAK DISTRICT BIRD OF PREY INITIATIVE

Bye Bye Birdie? Part II Featured scientist: Richard Holmes from the Hubbard Brook Experimental Forest

2012 Report on the Common Tern (Sterna hirundo) at Oneida Lake Report to the New York State Department of Environmental Conservation

APPENDIX 11.2 BRENT GEESE SURVEY REPORT

Vermont Tourism and Recreation Survey

Roberts Bank Terminal 2 Project Field Studies Information Sheet

Migration Math N79. Theme: Natural History. Author: Loris J. Chen Teacher, North Arlington Middle School. Subject Areas Science, Math

Anticipate your Subject Part # 2

Double-Crested Cormorants on Lake Champlain

FOOD PARASITISM AND COMPETITION IN TWO TERNS CARL D. HOPKINS AND R. HAVEN WILEY

Bird Hazard Management Plan. Patrick Port Botany Terminal

National Park Service U.S. Department of the Interior. Haleakala National Park Makawao, Maui, Hawai'i

Osprey Nest Abundance, Distribution, and Productivity in Casco Bay

Whimbrel. Appendix A: Birds. Numenius phaeopus [M] New Hampshire Wildlife Action Plan Appendix A Birds-225

Spring Migration: Loons are Returning to the BWCAW

COVER PAGE. Home address 5875 Brasstown Creek Road, Young Harris GA 30582

SEASONAL MOVEMENTS OF BLACKBIRDS ACROSS THE ARCHIPELAGO OF WESTERN LAKE ERIE 1

Ruddy Turnstone. Appendix A: Birds. Arenaria interpres [M,W] New Hampshire Wildlife Action Plan Appendix A Birds-50

Appendix A Little Brown Myotis Species Account

MPA Baseline Program. Annual Progress Report. Use of Estuarine, Intertidal, and Subtidal Habitats by Seabirds Within the MLPA South Coast Study Region

Level 6-7 Two Years Vacation

threatens their survival.

2015 population status of the Peregrine Falcon in the Yukon Territory

Effects of human activity on the foraging behavior of sanderlings Calidris alba

General Secretariat Delegations Problem of necrophagous birds in Spain because of shortage of natural food: a serious threat to biodiversity

Increase of the California Gull Population in the San Francisco Bay and the Impacts on Western Snowy Plovers

U. S. National Park Service

Wildlife distributions and habitat use on the mid-atlantic Outer Continental Shelf

Northampton Washlands: Frequently Asked Questions

Osprey Monitoring Guide

THE COMMON LOON. Population Status and Fall Migration in Minnesota MINNESOTA ORNITHOLOGISTS UNION OCCASIONAL PAPERS: NUMBER 3

MANAGEMNT OF CORMORANT COLONIES IN DENMARK

Herd composition and dispersion in the Whooper Swan

Humboldt Bay NWR BCS number: 86-4

MANUAL FOR BUILDING OWNERS AND CONTRACTORS ACCESSING ROOFTOPS WITH PROTECTED NESTING BIRDS

Caspian Gull. Caspian Gull at Hythe (Ian Roberts)

American White Pelican Minnesota Conservation Summary

Nesting success in Ahuriri River

OLD NESTS AS CUES FOR NEST-SITE SELECTION: AN EXPERIMENTAL TEST WITH RED-WINGED BLACKBIRDS

PHENOLOGY LESSON TEACHER GUIDE

AGE-RELATED DIFFERENCES IN RUDDY TURNSTONE FORAGING AND AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIOR SARAH GROVES

Horned Grebe vs. Eared Grebe: Head shape and occurrence timing

UNITED STATES AMLR ~:c:~=~: PROGRAM AMLR 1998/99 FIELD SEASON REPORT

Starfish finger puppet

Transcription:

COMPARATIVE FEEDING BEHAVIOR OF IMMATURE AND ADULT HERRING GULLS NICOLAAS A. M. VERBEEK Many birds do not breed until they are 2 or more years old. Such delayed breeding is generally found among large, long-lived, Lack (1954) in which reproduction non-passerine species. suggested that delayed breeding has probably evolved in species at an earlier age would not be likely to succeed or might be harmful to the parents. Ashmole (1963) and Amadon (1964) suggested that young birds might be unable to catch food as efficiently adults do. Recently, a number of field studies have shown that the ability to obtain food improves with age (see Buckley and Buckley 1974 for refer- ences). Given the inefficiency, it might be expected that the young have to compensate somehow for their lack of success, e.g. by spending more time in feeding. The object of this study was first to establish feeding efficiency in young and adult Herring Gulls (Larus argentatus), which show delayed breeding, and then to see how the young make up for any inefficiencies. The study was made on Walney Island, Cumbria, as England, in 1973 and 1974. Herring Gulls and Lesser Black-backed Gulls (L. ~USCUS) breed on the southern end of the island. sources such as the large intertidal dump on the island. The colony is very dependent on nearby food areas of Morecambe Bay and a garbage METHODS At low water during spring tides extensive mussel (Mytilus edulis) beds are exposed in Morecambe Bay. Associated with the mussels is the common starfish (Asterias rubens). Large numbers of Herring Gulls of all age classes fed upon these starfishes. Because of the distance between my observation post and the nearest beds (ca. 400 m) I could only distinguish (I used a 20-45 X zoom scope) between adults and immature birds (all those showing brown feathers). The immatures included birds at least 1 year old and older. The exact number of birds under observation is unknown as birds arrived and left continuously. At any one time approximately 50 birds were present, mainly adults. The gulls fed on starfishes (not mussels) by plunge diving for them, sometimes disappearing completely under water. Both immatures and adults fed in the same water apparently under similar conditions and in similar places. When I saw a gull diving I recorded whether it was successful (emerging with a starfish) or unsuccessful, and what the subsequent fate of the starfish was. Because the period when starfishes were available was short (about 2 hours) and infrequent (only during spring tides), I recorded as many dives as possible (different birds) rather than watching individuals over a period of time. IJnder these circumstances I could not obtain information on diving rates. Neither was it feasible to obtain meaningful information on the size of the starfishes taken. The data were collected during 25 hours between 3 and 21 April 1973. 415

416 THE WILSON BULLETIN * Vol. 89, No. 3, September 1977 On 5 and 6 April 1974, Hans Kruuk and I watched Herring and Lesser Black-backed gulls feeding at low tide on the crab, Hyus araneus. Gulls that managed to get a crab were often pursued by others in aerial chases. On both days we counted all the gulls feeding in a small bay, and the proportion of the various age classes represented. For each chase we recorded the number, the species, and the age classes participating as pursuers as well as the age class and the species of the pursued bird. In addition we noted whether the pursued bird lost its prey, and if possible, which pursuer obtained it. We distinguished between first-year-birds (brown young of the previous year), subadults (those showing a mixture of first-year and adult coloration), and adults. Subadult Lesser Black-backed Gulls were not seen in the general area of the colony until 19 April. We therefore assumed that all first-year-birds and subadults in our sample were Herring Gulls. In the analysis, chases in which one or more Lesser Black-backed Gulls pursued another of their species were not included. I did include those Lesser Black-backed Gulls that were chased by Herring Gulls and those that chased jointly with Herring Gulls. On 3 occasions at low water during spring tides an assistant and I counted all the gulls feeding in Morecambe Bay. The counts were made when the birds returned to the colony. We sat on the shore between the colony and the mussel beds and each of US counted the gulls that flew through his half of the sky. After 15 min I recorded our scores and the counting began anew. This was repeated until almost all gulls had returned to the colony. RESULTS Adult Herring Gulls were more successful in catching a starfish on the first dive (18 [64%] out of 28 dives) than immatures (3 [ 16%] out of 19 dives). These results are significantly different (x2 = 8.16, d.f. 1, P < 0.01). This has been shown too for the Brown Pelican (Pelicanus occidentalis) (Orians 1969) and the Sandwich Tern (Sterr~a sarzdvicensis) (Dunn 1972)) but not for the adult and juvenile Royal Tern (S. maxima) (Buckley and Buckley 1974). Following a successful dive many starfishes were dropped in being brought up from the bottom, or in flight during transport from the water to the land. The gulls often did not attempt to retrieve these starfishes, perhaps because they fell in deep water. Many others were stolen when a diving gull surfaced or while it was being pursued in flight. In cases where they were not pursued in flight, adult Herring Gulls (N = 28) dropped 3 out of 28 starfishes, while immatures (N = 6) dropped 3 out of 6. These results are not significantly different (P = 0.053, Fisher exact probability test). Immature Royal Terns drop significantly more fish than adults do (Buckley and Buckley 1974)) and Dunn (1972) suggests the same for Sandwich Terns. On the mussel beds a gull could lose its starfish when supplanted, when occupied defending a starfish by long-calling (Tinbergen 1959)) or when a third bird took it while the owner was busy chasing another gull. The proportion of starfishes eaten versus those not eaten (Table 1) by adult and immature birds is not significantly different (x = 1.65, d.f. 1, P > 0.05). I

Verbeek l HERRING GULL FEEDING BEHAVIOR 417 TABLE 1 DIFFERENCES BETWEEN ADULT AND IMMATURE HERRING GULLS IN THE FATE OF A STARFISI~ ONCE IT IS CAUGHT RY DIVING Fate of starfish Adult Herring Gull Immature Herring Gull Eaten 28 (36%) 1 (10%) Dropped 26 (33%) 4 (40%) Stolen 23 (30%) 4 (40%) Abandoned 1 ( 1%) 1 (10%) think that this is only because my sample for the immatures is small (Table 1). I gathered data only on those birds that were actually seen to dive. Many cases where birds were seen to be feeding on the beds without my knowing how they had obtained their starfish in the first place, went unrecorded. I gained the impression from these additional observations that young birds were more prone to have their starfish stolen than adults. Many gulls feed in Morecambe Bay at low water during spring tides. For instance, on 20 April we counted 20,473 gulls returning to the colony. In Fig. 1A this count is plotted in relation to low tide. Similar counts were made on 3 May (19,142 gulls) and 4 July (20,304 gulls). Considering the short time that the mussel beds are exposed it is doubtful that any of these gulls had enough time to make more than 1 trip. Each of the 3 curves in Fig. 1A has a major and a minor peak. The medians of any 2 curves in Fig. 1A are not significantly different. During preliminary counts prior to 20 April I had noticed that adult Lesser Black-backed Gulls and immatures tended to return to the colony later in relation to low tide than did adult Herring Gulls. To analyze this I counted the proportion of immatures in 500 gulls of both species that flew past me. Once 500 gulls had been counted I repeated the procedure until almost all gulls had returned to the colony. For each 500 gulls I recorded the period over which the count was obtained and the midpoint of the period was entered in Fig. 1B. The same procedure was used to obtain a curve (not shown) for the proportion of adult Lesser Black-backed Gulls among all gulls returning to the colony. I used this information to analyze the species and age composition of the returning birds. F or instance, the curve of 20 April (Fig. 1A) is redrawn in Fig. 1C. Clearly, the minor peaks (Fig. 1A) referred to earlier are the result of the later return of Lesser Black-backed Gulls, and immatures of both species. In an aerial chase a bird carrying a crab could be pursued by 1 to 9 other gulls. Although relatively few first-year-birds were present, they participated

418 THE WILSON BULLETIN - Vol. 89, No. 3, September 1977 5000 -.20ApnL -0 3 May - 4 July I 500 -i0aprtl I -19Aprll -1May 5000* FIG. 1. Number of gulls returning from the mussel beds to the colony plotted in relation to low tide ($1. (A) Each point on the graph is placed in the middle of a 15min period and indicates the number of all gulls returning during that time span. (B) The number of immature Herring and Lesser Black-backed gulls per 500 gulls of all ages counted for each point on the graph. (Cl Adult Herring Gulls (HG), adult Lesser Black-backed Gulls (LBG), and immature gulls (IG) of both species. See text for further explanation. in many more chases per bird than adult Herring Gulls (Table 2). Of the 82 recorded chases, 67 involved 1 or more of the 39 immatures present. Of the 111 adult Herring Gulls 1 or more participated in only 24 chases. Comparing the number of successful pursuers (i.e. those birds that managed to obtain the prey from the bird they were pursuing) with the number of birds involved as pursuers (Table 2)) all 3 age classes of Herring Gulls scored equally well (x2 = 0.982, d.f. 2, P > 0.05). Per bird, adult Herring Gulls were pursued significantly less than immature birds (x2 = 25.31, d.f. 1, P < 0.001). All 3 age classes of the Herring Gull were equally successful in keeping their catch while being pursued (x = 0.39, d.f. 1, P > 0.05). DISCUSSION Herring Gulls feed on a variety of foods in diverse places, including the intertidal, harbors, fields, and garbage dumps. The kinds of food obtained there require different types and degrees of skill. Starfishes are an important source of food as judged by the many birds that catch them and by the

Verbeek * HERRING GULL FEEDING BEHAVIOR 419 TABLE 2 AERIAL CHASES IN A KNOWN NUMBER OF HERRING GULLS AND THE FREQUENCY WITH WHICH EACH AGE CLASS IS PURSUED AND TAKES PART AS PURSUER Age class No. of birds present Pursuing birds Pursued birds No. of No. of No. of No. of times Successful times pursuits/ No. of times pursued/ pursued involved birds successful pursued birds present bird1 as pursuer present pursuws2 First year 10 15 1.5 11 31 3.1 5 Subadult 29 21.7 14 70 2.4 8 Adult 111 19.2 10 30.3 6 1 Those birds that managed to keep their prey. 2 Those birds that managed to obtain the food from the bird they were pursuing. large amounts of calcarious remains found in the colony (Shaffer 1971, pers. obs.). Young birds should thus learn to catch them and this study shows that they do. To make up for their lack of success, several alternatives are possible. For instance, the young may try harder by spending more time diving, by diving more frequently, by being more persistent in diving repeatedly for the same starfish until successful, or by feeding on other types of food as well (i.e. by being generalists). These important aspects could not be studied under the circumstances. One other way of making up for their shortfall in obtaining food is to resort to stealing, either on the ground or in the air. In this study the immatures participated in a greater number of pursuits than one would have expected from the number of immatures present (Table 2). Adult Herring Gulls do not chase others as much as do immatures (Table 2). Apparently as the young mature, chasing becomes less important as a way of obtaining food. There may be several reasons for this. Adults may be less successful as chasers than younger birds, but this was not the case. Secondly, adults may give up sooner than immatures and very short chases involving adults may thus go unrecorded. Some evidence from aerial chases over the garbage dump in Oxford, England, in winter shows that young birds were as tenacious as adults. Apparently, the propensity of immature Herring Gulls to steal is real (Drury and Smith 1968). Moyle (1966) reports similar observations for immatures of Lams ghcescens. Because of their general inefficiency, it seems reasonable to assume that the immatures require more time than the adults to satisfy their food requirements. Th is is indirectly supported by the fact that most of the immatures returned to the colony later than most of the adult Herring Gulls, the bulk of which returned 45 min earlier than the immatures (Fig. 1-C).

420 THE WILSON BULLETIN * Vol. 89, No. 3, September 1977 The data suggest that at any given age a Herring Gull employs mostly those feeding skills that produce the best results. As the birds mature they learn new skills, improve on them, and discard or de-emphasize others. For an immature Herring Gull stealing on the ground and in aerial chases is apparently important. However, the young spend some time learning the feeding skills used by adults. Older birds do not chase as much as younger ones and I assume that for them other forms of feeding are more efficient in terms of time and energy expenditure. SUMMARY Immature Herring Gulls are less efficient in capturing starfishes by diving and possibly also in transporting them in flight than are adults. In contrast to the adults, the immatures steal much of their food from other gulls on the ground and in aerial pursuits. As the birds mature, stealing gives way to independent methods of finding food. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS It is a pleasure to thank Professor N. Tinbergen and Dr. H. Kruuk for helpful discussions and companionship in the field. Linda Verbeek and Robert Fergusson helped me count gulls. I thank Holker Estates Ltd. and the Lake District and Lancashire Naturalists Trusts for permission to work in the Walney gullery. Mr. Walter Shepherd, Warden of the South Walney Nature Reserve, was helpful in many ways. The work was in part supported by a grant from the Natural Environmental Research Council to Professor N. Tinbergen. LITERATURE CITED AMADON, D. 1964. The evolution of low reproductive rates in birds. Evolution 18:105-110. ASHMOLE, N. P. 1963. The regulation of numbers of tropical oceanic birds. Ibis 103:458-473. BUCKLEY, F. G. AND P. A. BUCKLEY. 1974. Comparative feeding ecology of wintering adult and juvenile Royal Terns. Ecology X:1053-1063. DRURY, W. H., JR. AND W. J. SMITH. 1968. Defense of feeding areas by adult Herring Gulls and intrusion by young. Evolution 22:193-201. DUNN, E. K. 1972. Effect of age on the fishing ability of Sandwich Terns Sterna sundvicensis. Ibis 17:325-335. LACK, D. 1954. The natural regulation of animal numbers. Oxford Univ. Press, London. MOYLE, P. 1956. Feeding behavior of the Glaucous-winged Gull on an Alaskan salmon stream. Wilson Bull. 78:175-l%. ORIANS, G. H. 1969. Age and hunting success in the Brown Pelican (Pelicanus occident&s). Anim. Behav. 17:3X-319. SHAFFER, L. C. 1971. Specializations in the feeding behaviour of gulls and other birds. D. Phil. Thesis, Oxford Univ., Oxford, England.

Verbeek * HERRING GULL FEEDING BEHAVIOR 421 TINBERGEN, N. 1959. Comparative studies of the hehaviour of gulls (Laridae) : a progress report. Behaviour 15:1-70. DEPT. OF ZOOLOGY, ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR RESEARCH GROUP, SOUTH PARKS ROAD, OXFORD, 0x1 UPS, ENGLAND (PRESENT ADDRESS: DEPT. OF BIOLOGICAL SCIENCES, SIMON FRASER UNIV., BURNABY, B.C. CANADA V5A 1~6). AC- CEPTED 20 SEPT. 1976. REQUEST FOR ASSISTANCE WANTED: Data on the Seasonal Distribution of North American Gulls.-We are developing a procedure whereby the U.S. Air Force can predict the potential seasonal hazard to aircraft represented by gulls in parts of North America. This knowledge will be used to schedule missions around high risk areas thereby reducing the likelihood of bird-aircraft collisions. Supplemental data on local gull populations are needed from all parts of the continent. The assistance of field workers is solicited to aid us in this task. For each observation, please provide the following information: list of species present, approximate number of each species, precise locality description, dates observed, any information about causes for concentrations (e.g. sanitary landfill operation), and any details about the frequency of such concentrations in the respective areas. Information is sought from inland as well as coastal localities. Please submit reports of your gull observations to Dr. William E. Southern, Department of Biological Sciences, Northern Illinois University, DeKalb, IL 60115. Data will be gathered for a 2-year period beginning 1 September 1977.