An analysis of retinal receptor orientation II. Predictions for psychophysical tests. Jay M. Enoch and Alan M. Laties

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "An analysis of retinal receptor orientation II. Predictions for psychophysical tests. Jay M. Enoch and Alan M. Laties"

Transcription

1 An analysis of retinal receptor orientation II. Predictions for psychophysical tests Jay M. Enoch and Alan M. Laties With the use of histologic techniques, it was shown that retinal receptors in several species are directed toward a point near the front of the eye lens. Because of inherent limitations in histologic methods, evidence concerning photoreceptor orientation obtained by a different technique would be of value. The psychophysically determined Stiles-Crawford effect (directional sensitivity of the retina) provides a valuable test of the anatomical findings. In this paper the theoretical basis for such a test is developed and working hypotheses have been proposed. One model calls for retinal receptors to be directed toward the center of the exit pupil of the eye (anterior pointing hypothesis), and a second requires the photoreceptors to point toward the center of the retinal sphere (center pointing hypothesis). The direction of the maximum of the photopic Stiles-Crawford function would be different for the two working hypotheses at extrafoveal points. Similarly, the integrated effective visual stimulus magnitude would be different for each of the two alternative proposals. The questions raised in this paper are significant because they can help clarify the "why" of retinal receptor optical properties. Key words: retinal receptor orientation, Stiles-Crawford effect, optical properties of retinal receptors, retinal illuminance, effective visual stimulus. In prior communications the orientation of photoreceptors with reference to the optical landmarks of the eye 1 ' 2 and to each other 3 was discussed. In several species the From the Department of Ophthalmology, Washington University School of Medicine, and the Oscar Johnson Institute, St. Louis, Mo., and the Department of Ophthalmology, Hospital of the University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pa. Supported in part by Research Grant No. 5 RO1 EY and Career Development Award No. 5 K3 EY (to Jay M. Enoch) and in part by a Research to Prevent Blindness Research Professorship and by Research Grant No. 9 RO1 EY (to Alan M. Laties) of the National Eye Institute, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Md. Manuscript submitted March 10, 1971; revised manuscript accepted Oct. 5, photoreceptors were found to be coaxially aligned with a point D near the front of the eye lens along the line of sight (Fig. 1) and thus to have a graded differential orientation. Because of the inherent limitations of the anatomical techniques used, evidence about photoreceptor orientation gathered by independent means would be of value. The Stiles-Crawford effect 4 in psychophysics is an obvious alternative to anatomical methods, and the present communication addresses itself to the means by which critical psychophysical studies can be performed. The expected displacement of the maxima of sets of Stiles-Crawford functions are derived for points away from the principal line of sight for two different hypothetical conditions. In the first, photoreceptors are assumed to be normal to the globe and to point toward its cen-

2 960 Enoch and Laties Investigative Ophthalmology December 1971 Cornea Retina AE = mm ED = " DO =9.312 " AO = = OF' =11.4 mm 1 +- AF' = mm* DF" = mm AI = 3.6 mm 9 L -" 9 3 (1.2144) Gullstrand Schematic Eye Made Emmetropic Fig. 1. Constants and angles used in the analysis are shown on this schematic drawing. A ray is incident in the center of the entrance pupil of the emmetropized Gullstrand schematic eye ( ). After refraction, it continues on as if it had passed through the center of the exit pupil (D) to its retinal intercept P n. O is located at the center of the retinal sphere. ter; while in the second, photoreceptors are assumed to be everywhere aligned coaxial to a point along the line of sight passing through the center of the exit pupil of the eye. One of the two hypotheses presented predicts a decentration of the photopic directional sensitivity function in the entrance pupil of the eye for nonfoveal test points. Such a decentration would result in a reduction of the effective visual stimulus. An analogue device provided determinations of stimulus magnitude changes predicted for different Stiles-Crawford peak decentrations and different pupil sizes. This paper is a logical extension of histologic studies of photoreceptor orientation and of the analyses of relationships of photoreceptors to the eye 2 and to each other. 3 In the quantitative analysis which follows, it is assumed that one "law" or organizational format pertains to the entire retina. The possibility exists, however, that different regions of the retina exhibit different orientational properties. Such a multilaw concept was suggested by Stiles 5 as part of an analysis of a paper by Aguilar and Plaza. 0 Only limited Stiles-Crawford data exist for nonfoveal points and local variations in orientation have been discribed. This paper provides a theoretical framework for studies just completed. Working hypotheses Center pointing hypothesis (CPH). All receptors point toward the center of the retinal sphere. That is, they are radially distributed about the center of that sphere, point 0 in Fig. 1. Anterior pointing hypothesis (APH). All receptors are directed toward an anterior point in the eye; ideally they point toward the center of the iris aperture stop (the source of light) as imaged by the eye lens. That point is the center of the exit pupil of the eye, point D on Fig. 1. In this and in previous papers, 2 ' 3 descriptions of the anterior pointing hypothesis treat D as a uni-

3 Volume 10 Number 12 Retinal receptor orientation tary locus. When one considers peripheral lens characteristics and the limitations inherent in histologic observations, it may well be as valid to consider D a distribution of points, the characteristics of which have not yet been fully defined. Interreceptor differences in orientation for either hypothesis are very small. 3 In fact, the angular and spatial differences predicted are so small that they are beyond the resolution capabilities of present electron microscopic techniques for tissue sections. Alternative hypotheses describing orientational characteristics may be formulated. For example, all receptors may be regarded as being parallel to each other. As a general theory of photoreceptor orientation, this proposal must be rejected. If all photoreceptors were parallel to the foveal elements, cells at the equator of the eye would be flat against the external limiting membrane, and those situated beyond that point would have to be bent back upon themselves. All anatomic evidence indicates that such a model is not valid. As a more limited model, applying to a small region at the posterior pole of the eye, the hypothesis deserves consideration. Recently just such a proposal was advanced by Heath and Walraven 7 - s on the basis of Stiles- Crawford measurements of the central four degrees. Since the degree of receptor tilt within 2 degrees of the fovea is of so low an order as to be within the measurement error of present histologic techniques, discrimination between parallel orientation and the other possibilities presented is not possible by anatomical means. The necessary equations which allow consideration of this third working hypothesis are included in this manuscript. The authors hope Dr. Heath will extend his studies to more peripheral points in which a critical comparison between differing hypotheses can be more easily accomplished. Analysis In Fig. 1, a chief ray is directed toward the center of the entrance of the pupil of the the eye (E). When refracted by an optical system having the properties of the Gullstrand schematic eye, this ray passes through the center of the iris (I) and leaves the rear lens surface as if it had origin at a point D in the center of the exit pupil of the eye. The ray, whose angle of incidence at E is designated by Qi, leaves point D with an angle of G,?. In Fig. 1 if one assumes (1) that the iris is coincident with the anterior surface of the unaccommodated lens of the eye, (2) that the iris is centered on (and the fovea lies on) the optic axis of the eye, and (3) that the rays are paraxial, then small angle assumptions hold and the simple relationship shown in Equation 1, may be defined by performing a chief ray trace. e, = e 3 (1.2144) (1) The refracted ray DP n is incident at retinal point? n. Q s is the angle the refracted chief ray makes with the optical axis of the eye. Point O lies at the center of the retinal sphere. Modifying the Gullstrand schematic eye in order to make it emmetropic, F' serves as the center of the fovea and as the secondary focal point of the optical system of the eye. Angle P n OF', 0, is the angular displacement of the chief ray intercept P n from the fovea as measured at the center of the retinal sphere. The radius of the retinal sphere (OP n or OF' designated by r) is estimated from available reports of ocular dimensions. 9 The values of the several dimensions used in this analysis are shown on Fig. 1. The distance AF' and the derived value DF' are based on the characteristics of the emmetropized Gullstrand eye shown in Equation 2. d = DF' - r (2) In triangle DOP n, d and r are constants. Interestingly, point O lies near, but not necessarily precisely at, the center of rotation of the eye. That is, the center of the retinal sphere, O, must also lie near the center of the scleral sphere. The second assumption listed above will have to be considered further. A correction factor will have to be introduced for nonalignment of the fovea, the pupil center, and the optic axis. A number of different formulas may be used to describe the relationships between the factors considered in the model. In the initial analyses Equations 3, 4, and 5 have been found most useful. 2 ' 3 0 = e 3 + (3) sine O 3 sine sine 0 (4) r cot = esc 0 + cot 0 (5) d As the angle of incidence of the chief ray

4 962 Enoch and Laties Investigative Ophthalmology December 1971 a k "= a k (0.9094) Entrance Pupil Exit Pupil Fig. 2. In the center pointing hypothesis, the receptors are coaxial with the radii of the retinal sphere. With increase in angle 0, the forward projection of these photoreceptor axes to the pupillary plane results in marked displacements of that projection in the exit (and hence entrance) pupil. If the peak of Stiles-Crawford effect reflects the central tendency of receptor orientations, in the CPH, one would expect displacements of the S-C maximum in the entrance pupil which would increase as i or 0 becomes larger. (Oj) increases, the resultant angle of refraction (0,9) increases, and the length of the chief ray projection to the retina (x) becomes shorter. As Q 3 increases, 0 and 6., increase and 0 2 decreases. Coincidentally with an increase in 0 i5 the effective aperture of the iris becomes foreshortened in the plane of 0* as a function of cos 0*. This is the familiar horizontal narrowing of the iris on lateral view. To some extent the resultant reduction in visual stimulus magnitude is overcome by the shortening of the distance x as 0,? becomes larger. As x becomes shorter, the angle subtended by the bounds of the exit pupil becomes larger at point P n. The image quality in the peripheral retina is poor because the retinal surface does not lie in the image plane and because the aberrations are marked. Hence, the straightforward integration of energy (having origin at some infinitely distant point) irradiating all parts of the iris aperture and directed toward Point P n may not be too valid for large angles 0. Further, because of uncertainties regarding the validity of the schematic eye models for far peripheral points, predictions for these retinal areas are not without hazard. Fortunately, in order to make judgments regarding the two hypotheses presented in this discussion, the status of the far periphery need not be evaluated. As indicated, the authors assume that a single-orientation "law" holds for the entire retina. The Stiles-Crawford effect is a measure of retinal directional sensitivity for angles of incidence at the retina limited by the aperture of the pupil. Considering only photopic stimuli, one may argue that if the retina is directionally sensitive, the peak of the Stiles-Crawford function reflects the orientational tendency of the directionally sensitive elements located in the retinal sampling area. By finding the direction of maximum sensitivity at different points from the fovea outward, it should be possible to gain an independent measure of retinal receptor orientation, a measure which avoids many of the pitfalls common to histologic techniques. If receptors (inner and outer segments taken as a unit) at point P n are coaxial with the center of the exit pupil of the eye (APH), the maximum of the Stiles-Crawford function (for different angles of incidence Qi) will remain centered in the entrance pupil of the eye. On the other hand, if the receptors point toward the center of the retinal sphere (CPH), the maximum of sensitivity would displace rapidly in the eye pupil when angle i is increased. This point becomes obvious when looking at Fig. 2. Even for modest values of 0,-, the extension of the line P,,0 to the exit pupil results in substantial displacement of that line intercept (a k ") from the line F'D. The magnitude of the displacement, a k ", is given by the simple relationship shown in Equation 6. a k " = d tan 0 (6)

5 Volume 10 Number 12 Retinal receptor orientation. II 963 Translating a point in the exit pupil to a comparable point (a lc ) in the entrance pupil results in Equation 7. a k = a k "/ (7) This latter relationship is derived from paraxial ray trace with the use of the constants of the unaccommodated Gullstrand schematic eye. From these relationships, one may compute the predicted displacement (from the center of the entrance pupil) of the maximum of the photopic Stiles-Crawford effect. Starting with either Equation 1 or a modified version of it, Q 3 may be computed from given values of Q t. Given r, d, and Q 3, and using the law of sines (Equation 4), 9, may be determined. Knowing Q 3 and Q 1} one may solve for 0 (Equation 3). Then a k may be computed from Equations 6 and 7. If all receptors were parallel to the optic axis, the displacement would be a k " = r sin 0, or a k " = x sin 0 3. In this instance, the projection of the axis of the receptor to the pupillary plane falls on the opposite side of the pupil from that considered in Fig. 2. Equation 1 has also been computed for tan 0, = tan ( ). Frankly, doubt arises as to the proper form one must use when the limits of 0 = sin 0 = tan 0 breakdown (at about eight degrees) and when models derived from paraxial optics are no longer valid. The argument has been developed in a manner such that induced errors are small. Fig. 3 is a plot of a k for the two working hypotheses. The prediction for the center pointing hypothesis, the computation of which was just described, is given by the curve marked O. The anticipated result for the anterior pointing hypothesis (with receptors pointing at the exit pupil of the eye) is defined by the line designated D. Note how rapidly values on the O line increase. The entrance pupil of the eye rarely exceeds 8 mm. or a value of a fc (radial displacement from the center of the entrance pupil) of 4 mm. This means that, for values 0 7 of 15 degrees or more, the Stiles-Crawford maximum for receptors 5 4 >H e c 1-2 L Fig. 3. These are the predictor curves for the direction of maximum sensitivity for the APH (line D) and the CPH (curve O). Stated simply, the anterior pointing hypothesis predicts the nondisplacement of the S-C peak for nonfoveal points while the center pointing hypothesis predicts a rapid displacement of a k as j increases. (See comments below on the meaning of minus values of a*.) pointing toward the center of the retinal sphere (CPH) will fall outside the 8 mm. diameter pupillary boundary. Thus, one may determine the general character of receptor orientation by studying the Stiles- Crawford function in the fovea, parafovea, and near periphery of the retina. That is, properties determined in the first 20 degrees of eccentricity from the fovea should provide the necessary information. Measurement problems When angle 0i is not small, the designation and control of a k becomes complex. In Fig. 4, the problem becomes apparent. Here E lies at the center of the entrance pupil. The ray XE is a chief ray incident in the entrance at an angle 0 from the normal (optic axis of the eye). WV is a ray which is parallel to XE but displaced by

6 964 Enoch and Laties Investigative Ophthalmology December 1971 Entrance Pupil Fig. 4. This drawing illustrates one problem which arises when Oi becomes large. If = 0 degrees, the image of the exit pupil of the Stiles-Crawford instrument lies in the entrance pupil of the eye. With oblique incidence that image rotates about point E. This complicates the optics, and results in a displacement of the stimulus in the entrance pupil. distance A/ from XE. The intercept of this ray in the entrance pupil plane is U, which is located a distance of A k from E. If one assumes that the aperture stop of the Stiles- Crawford apparatus is imaged with its center at E with increasing 9.;., its rotated projection at point V lies increasingly out of the plane of the entrance pupil of the eye, and A k becomes increasingly larger than A fc '. Angle UEV = 9i and triangle UVE is a right triangle (Equation 8). cos Q[ = and a k = a : / sec 0, (8) a k This complex problem can be readily dealt with experimentally if limited focal adjustment of the aperture plane is possible in the Stiles-Crawford apparatus and if the entrance pupil plane of the eye may be visualized (or duplicated on a ground glass or other screen which may be viewed by the experimenter during the experiment). That is, a k may be measured directly (instead of measuring a k ') and blur of a projected aperture in the entrance pupil plane may be corrected. A special added problem, the refraction of light at the vitreous-retinal interface, has been brought to our attention by L. Lipetz. While the index of refraction of the vitreous is readily determined in fact, is well known the inner retinal index (or concentration of solids) has not been defined. Pending determination of inner retinal index, consideration of this matter will be deferred. Given the small distances involved, it is probable that the magnitude of the inner retinal deflection of ray DP n (Fig. 1) is small. The foveal incline presents a special problem from this point of view a point made many years ago by Walls. 10 A major problem not considered in this paper is local variation in the receptor orientation, described by O'Brien and Miller 11 and verified by Enoch. 12 One must distinguish local orientation of a small group of receptors from the general tendency of the retinal area. Histologically, differences in orientation between retinal groups of a few degrees are difficult to measure. 1 By careful application of the Stiles-Crawford technique, that is, using a bite bar, field stop adjustment, and monitoring eye position and focus, some estimate of local variations may be obtained. The O'Brien-Miller technique allows rapid evaluation only of local differences in orientation as reflected through the visual neural network. The existence of local variation of photoreceptor orientation requires that great care be employed when making Stiles- Crawford measurements for the purposes considered here. Only limited weight can be placed on the data of one observer or on one or two points determined in one meridian. Further, the use of small test field areas can lead, for these reasons, to erroneous conclusions. Although local variation in photoreceptor orientation makes it imperative that precise technique and careful evaluation be employed, it does not make the problem insoluble. Since one is interested primarily in the direction of the maximum, a technique described by Enoch 13 might be adapted. That is, instead of holding one aperture constant in the entrance pupil and translating a second aperture as is usually done, one can move both apertures simultaneously across the entrance pupil (e.g., separating Vz mm.

7 Volume 10 Number 12 Retinal receptor orientation. II c ^ 2 o / i / X * / / \ \ As / V y / \\ // / \ a OD. Nasal L. G L OD. TemporalL. 6^ 0.5. Nasal L. 9i O.S. Temporal L. & "or further in the indicated direction" * Identical points \ \ \ \ \ s \ V -3 L Fig. 5. The data of Aguilar and Plaza G have been fit to the hypothetical functions present in Fig. 3. Obviously, these data do not allow a choice between the two hypotheses. Other available data, when plotted in this manner, are equally ambiguous. The arrows tagged on certain points indicate that the direction of maximum sensitivity fell at the edge of the observer's pupil or beyond. The one set of data is dashed because of partial overlap with another set. The two points identified with the asterisk on the ordinate are the same data point (see text). aperture images about 2 mm.). The observer may then compare the brightnesses of the projections of these two apertures on the retina (bipartite field type of display two halves separated a bit). The directional sensitivity maximum should bisect the locus of the two points in the aperture stop when the two retinal images appear equally bright. The technique described assumes Stiles-Crawford function symmetry, normal photoreceptor orientation, and no vignetting. Further, angle 0i should not be so large that the factors considered in Equation 8 are of consequence. One must specify 0 i5 the position of the Stiles-Crawford directional sensitivity maximum in the entrance pupil, and the direction of translation of the maximum in relation to the angle of incidence 0*. This point becomes clearer on examination of Fig. 2. If the center pointing hypothesis is valid, the maximum (designated by a k ) would be translated to the same side of the entrance pupil as the direction of origin of the ray incident at E. Thus if the ray defining 0 originated as an angular displacement in the nasal field, the maximum of the Stiles- Crawford effect for the CPH would be displaced nasally in the entrance pupil. Information available from prior reports is too scant to allow full consideration of our hypotheses. Although Aguilar and Plaza G considered the problem of Stiles- Crawford effects away from the fovea in some detail, their description of experimental controls was limited and their data were obtained from experiments on the two eyes of one observer. In Fig. 5, Aguilar and Plaza's complex data are plotted on the predictor curves developed in Fig. 3. On the basis of their data, plots of their Stiles-Crawford function maxima could be

8 966 Enoch and Laties Investigative Ophthalmology December i _ J Equivalent Diameter of Entrance Pupil 2 mm 4mm 6mm 8mm SCE (Crawford) Non-integrated, set to match the 2mm diam. peak D e c e n t r a t i o n i n t h e E n t r a n c e P u p i l ( m m ) Fig. 6. Mean data obtained using the analogue instrument. There are integrations of the stimulus imaged in the integrating sphere. Four different equivalent diameter entrance pupils were employed. These were displaced from the center of the rotating disc in equivalent millimeter steps. For comparison, the Crawford J1 function has been plotted with data obtained using an equivalent 2 mm. entrance pupil. The arrow on the ordinate points toward the four data points plotted on Fig. 7 (in which these same points appear as a cross and three black points). made only to the nearest millimeter. If the peak of the Stiles-Crawford function were not centered in the entrance pupil for foveal fixation (e.g., when testing in the horizontal meridian), the displacement (a k ) would be plotted differently for nasal field values of 0s than for the temporal field values. Plus values of ak indicate that the peak was displaced in the same sense as 0i; minus values indicate the reverse displacement. Plus values (and minus values) indicate common (or opposed) angular orientation of these two factors. Thus on Fig. 5 (asterisks), we see the apparent anomaly of the same point plotted at two different points on the ordinate. Although several authors have presented data on the directionality of the peripheral retina, none is in a form suitable for the analysis of the problems raised in this report. 14 ' 17 Integrating the Stiles-Crawford (S-C) function It is important to estimate the effect of a displacement of the peak of the S-C

9 Volume 10 Number 12 Retinal receptor orientation. II 967 function on the magnitude of the visual stimulus. One may also use such information to distinguish between working hypotheses. Further, this material is added in order to provide a more complete analysis, in the same sense as that presented by Stiles and Crawford' 1 in their original paper. If the peak of the Stiles-Crawford function is not centered in the pupil, the effect is to reduce the effective visual stimulus. In order to quantify the decrement in stimulus, one must integrate the Stiles-Crawford function (of the first kind) across the entrance pupil of the eye and adjust that value for the effect of pupil decentration, making due correction for entrance pupil size. Method. A simple analogue measuring device was developed for this purpose. A disc with a representative S-C function drawn on it was rotated at high speed. It was illuminated by two direct current-supplied tungsten microscope illuminators. This rotating disc was imaged by a lens on the limiting aperture of an integrating sphere. The measured output of a calibrated photomultiplier provided the necessary integrations. Black flocked paper was used in areas not painted white. All measurements were in the form of the ratio, target measurement: measurement of a uniform background of black flocked paper. The S-C function chosen was the one proposed by Crawford (Equation 9). Four "eny = e-0.105r 2 (9) trance pupil" masks of different diameter were placed sequentially in front of the S-C distribution generated by this technique. The masks were centered or decentered in equivalent millimeter steps. Computed ratios for the different size masks which were located at given positions relative to the center of the rotating pattern are presented in Fig. 6. By setting the equivalent decentration in millimeters equal to au, values of Ot and 0 could be derived (Fig. 6, top). The validity of the analogue device as an integrator of the defined function was tested in two ways. A 2 mm. pupil is slightly larger than the sampling entrance pupil aperture used in Stiles-Crawford measurements. That is, the diameter of the aperture traversed across the pupil when making such determinations generally is of the order of 0.3 to 1.0 mm. While somewhat larger, a 2 mm. pupil should not differ markedly in effect from the reference function (Equation / - Arbitrarily Set (Ordinate only) * - Entrance Pupil Centered, Remaining Points Log Equivalent Entrance Pupil Area (mm 2 ) Fig. 7. This figure shows relative stimulus magnitude for different size entrance pupils of the eye ;ls for two conditions. Perfect additivity would be predicted if there was no directional sensitivity. The integrated (representative) SCE function would be followed if the observer's directional sensitivity function were centered in the entrance pupil (and the eye integrated energy falling at the retina on the basis of this factor alone). 1S These two curves are presented here to test the validity of the analogue model. Obviously, the four plotted points taken from Fig. 6 fit the integrated SCE function well. 9). The dashed distribution of Fig. 6 represents Equation 9 with its maximum value set equal to the zero-displacement measurement for the 2 mm. equivalent entrance pupil. The two distributions are closely matched. As a second test, values obtained for the entrance pupils centered condition (points lying on the ordinate indicated by the arroio) were plotted on Fig. 7. This is simply the effect of integrating the photopic Stiles-Crawford function for different size enhance pupils. The two curves plotted on Fig. 7 were taken from Enoch. 1S Perfect additivity represents the stimulus values one would predict for different size pupils if no S-C effect existed (as is assumed when computing photopic trolands and not correcting for the S-C effect). When correction is made for the directional sensitivity of the retina, the Integrated SCE curve is obtained. In psychophysical trials, added problems arise. 1S The four measured values on the ordinate of Fig. 6 were plotted on Fig. 7. The equivalent 2 mm. diameter entrance pupil was plotted at the cross mark. The remaining three points were then plotted. When this was done, they all fell on the predictor line. Thus, in this second test of this analogue integrator, a good result was obtained.

10 968 Enoch and Laties Investigative Ophthalmology December 1971 A small shadow, which was present near the edge of the "entrance pupil" mask, proved to be a limited source of error in these determinations. The shadow resulted from the necessary small separation of the rotating disc and the aperture. This factor was slightly greater for larger decentrations of the apertures and was proportionally greater the smaller the aperture diameter. Receptors not pointing at the center of the entrance pupil receive less stimulus. The predicted reduction in stimulus magnitude is given by each curve (Fig. 6). If the receptors point toward the center of the retinal sphere (CPH), sensitivity would be expected to fall off as O increases. Curves are presented for the different size entrance pupil apertures. This is an "aperture" effect and hence i? independent of test procedure employed. That is, regardless of whether or not one measures absolute threshold, increment threshold (static perimetry), flicker, visual resolution, etc., all determinations would be subject to this form of alteration in stimulus magnitude as f ( i). If center pointing is present, this characteristic would be present and should be detectable. If the receptors are directed toward the center of the exit pupil (APH), no fall-off in sensitivity as a function of 0 ( woidd occur due to aperture characteristics (for values of < limited to values shown in Fig. 3). In disturbances of receptor orientation which result in displacement of the primary S-C function, 12 ' la > 19 one would predict a loss in sensitivity comparable to that shown in Fig. 6. For each millimeter the peak of the S-C function is displaced in the entrance pupil, mean receptor orientation would be altered by 2.5 degrees. The relationship between a t and Q> for the center pointing hypothesis, and a k = O for 6 f = 0 should not be confused. It is evident from these analogue data that disturbances of several degrees in receptor orientation are probably required before meaningful decrements in visual resolution occur. 20 Discussion The psychophysical consequences of two hypothetical models of photoreceptor orientation have been calculated. The expected loci of maxima of sets of photopic Stiles-Crawford functions for parafoveal points are different in the two models. This permits the formulation of a number of clear tests between the working hypotheses. No allowance is made in the calculations for local variance in receptor orientation. Correction will have to be made for the displacement of the fovea from the optical axis of the eye. That correction will vary as a function of meridian tested. It is not possible to extend the predictions to the retinal periphery because of the limitations inherent in calculations based on the schematic eye and paraxial optics. Despite these limitations, the models described are of substantial value in understanding human receptor orientation. Simply stated, we believe differential receptor orientation is necessary for effective receptor function. The orientation characteristics common to photoreceptors in several species serves as witness to this generalization. 1 ' 3 The light-receiving structures of photoreceptors are cylinders or cones in form. Photosensitive pigment is oriented coplanar to membranous discs within the outer segments (for an example, refer to Liebman 21 ). The photoreceptors are transparent and are separated near the external limiting membrane by processes from the Miiller cells and by fibrils from the pigment epitheilum at their distal ends. The interstitial space formed by these cellular processes has a lower index of refraction, i.e., it has lower concentrations of proteins and lipids than those of the receptor outer segment (and part of the inner segment). The combination of high-index transparent cells separated by a low-index surrounding medium 22 " 24 results in the creation of fiber optics elements which, when taken together, form a bundle. In addition, the fibrils from the pigment epithelium (projecting between the receptors) contains dark melanin granules in many species. As a consequence of their form and surroundings, both rods and cones are directionally transmitting and directionally sensitive. The directional sensitivity of cones is widely accepted. Although controversial, the evidence for directional sensitivity of rods is steadily accumulating. 15 ' is, 22, when considering this problem, it must be remembered that the entrance pupil aperture allows evaluation only of directional sensitivity (Stiles-Crawford ef-

11 Volume 10 Number 12 Retinal receptor orientation. II 969 feet) over a few degrees of incidence at the retina in the human being, approximately ± 10 degrees. Here we refer to the angle subtended at the retina by the iris aperture rather than the retinal locus. That angle is translated to the entrance pupil plane by ray trace. The arguments presented above are not limited to apertures of ± 10 degrees. 7 Oriented photolabile pigment acts much like a polarizer. Probability of absorption of nonpolarized light follows a V2 (1 + cos 2 a) function, e.g., at an angle of incidence of 45 degrees only a 75.0 per cent probability of absorption exists. Further, a fiber optic element, like a microscope objective, has a limiting aperture, its numerical aperture (for an example refer to Kapany- S ). This aperture defines the limiting angle within which the fiber or receptor can contain (couple-in) energy. Dark melanin pigment sheathing in many species reinforces this effect. Since these properties are all present in rods, they, too, must be directionally sensitive. It is interesting that in the histologically all rod retina of Gecko gecko, the same anterior pointing properties were observed as were found in mixed rod-cone retinas of primates. 3 In order to be visually effective, photoreceptors in the mid to peripheral retina would ideally have an anterior pointing orientation. Assume a receptor is located at the equator of the eye and is directed toward the center of the retinal sphere (point O). If d were set equal to r (Fig. 1) radiant energy reaching this receptor would be incident at an angle of 45 degrees. The source of the visual stimulus (as seen by the receptor), the exit pupil aperture, would be somewhat foreshortened. Since a cell pointing at O most probably has its maximum sensitivity in the direction toward which it points, it would be receptive to and integrate stray light incident over a broad solid angle. Of course, the stray light absorbed would vary as a function of its angle of incidence at the cell. The summed amplitude of the stray light might well exceed the direct signal from the pupil. Melanin sheathing and pigment orientation would tend to further limit the direct signal absorbed in such a case. One is led to a number of conclusions, if this is a valid argument. Namely, the photolabile pigment in the receptor outer segment is oriented in order to more effectively absorb the light guided to it by the receptor acting as a fiber optics element. The receptor is a fiber optics element and is often sheathed by pigment to protect it from stray light. Thus, for effective function, the fiber optics element must be directed toward the source of light (ideally the center of the exit pupil of the eye) in order to receive the signal (visual stimulus) with high efficiency. One key aspect of the problem of photoreceptor orientation has not been considered here. That is, it is important to define the mechanism(s) which determine orientation at birth and maintain and correct it during life. A discussion of some phases of this problem was recently presented by Laties and Enoch. 3 A recent paper by Bourdy 20 touches on some aspects of this problem. Enoch and Hope 30 will shortly present an initial experimental analysis of theoretical arguments discussed in this paper. REFERENCES 1. Laties, A.: Histological techniques for the study of photoreceptor orientation, Tissue Celll: 63, Laties, A., Liebman, P., and Campbell, C. E. M.: Photoreceptor orientation in the primate eye, Nature 218: 172, Laties, A., and Enoch, J. M.: An analysis of retinal receptor orientation. I. Angular relationships of neighboring photoreceptors, INVEST. OPHTHALMOL. 10: 69, Stiles, W. S., and Crawford, B. H.: The luminous efficiency of rays entering the pupil at different point?, Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Series B. 112: 428, Stiles, W. S.: The directional sensitivity of the retina, Ann. R. Coll. Surg. Engl. 30: 73, Aguilar, M., and Plaza, A.: EfTecto Stiles- Crawford en vision extra-foveal, An. Soc. Real. Esp. Fisica y Quim. A. 50: 119, 1954.

12 970 Enoch and Laties Investigative Ophthalmology December Heath, C, and Walraven, P.: Receptor orientations in the central retina, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 60: 733, Heath, G.: Directional sensitivity of retinal receptors, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 60: 736, Duke-Elder, S., editor: System of ophthalmology, vol. II. The anatomy of the visual system, ed 2, London, 1961, Henry Kimpton. 10. Walls, G.: The vertebrate eye, Bloomfield Hills, Mich., 1942, Cranbrook Inst. of Science. 11. O'Brien, B., and Miller, N. : A study of the mechanism of visual acuity in the retina. W.A.D.C. Technical Report No , Wright Patterson Air Force Base, Ohio, 1953, Wright Air Development Center. 12. Enoch, J. M.: The current status of receptor amblyopia, Doc. Ophthalmol. 23: 130, (The O'Brien-Miller 11 experiment appears in a rather inaccessible document. The technique has been described in some detail in this paper.) 13. Enoch, J. M.: Receptor amblyopia, Am. J. Ophthalmol. 48: (Part II) , Crawford, B. H.: The luminous efficiency of light entering the eye pupil at different points and its relation to brightness threshold measurement. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Series B 124: 81, Flamant, F., and Stiles, W. S.: The directional and spectral sensitivities of the retinal rods to adapting fields of different wavelengths, J. Physiol. (Lond.) 107: 187, Flamant, F.: Contribution a l'etude du phenomene Stiles-Crawford, Rev. Opt. 28: 44, Westheimer, G.: Dependence of the magnitude of the Stiles-Crawford effect on retinal location, J. Physiol. 192: 309, Enoch, J. M.: Summated response of the retina to light entering different parts of the pupil, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 48: 392, Enoch, J. M.: The retina as a fiber optics bundle. Appendix B, in Kapany, N.: Fiber optics, principles and applications, New York, 1967, Academic Press, Inc., pp Ohzu, H., Enoch, J., and O'Hair, J.: Optical image modulation by the fresh isolated retina and retinal receptors, Vision Res. In press. 21. Liebman, P.: Retinal cell microspectrophotometry, Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 157: 250, Briicke, E. ( ): Cited (and elaborated on) in Von Helmholtz, H.: Handbook of physiological optics, vol. 1, ed. 3, (English translation by Southall, J. (1924), New York, 1962, Dover Publications, Inc., p. 229.) 23. Barer, R.: Refractometry and interferometry of living cells, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 47: 545, Sidman, R.: The structure and concentration of solids in photoreceptor cells studied by refractometry and interference microscopy, J. Biophys. Biochem. Cytol. 3: 15, Enoch, J. M.: Nature of the transmission of energy in the retinal receptors, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 51: 1122, Enoch, J. M.: Optical properties of the retinal receptors, J. Opt. Soc. Am. 53: 71, Enoch, J., and Glismann, L.: Physical and optical changes in excised retinal tissue: Resolution of retinal receptors as a fiber optics bundle, INVEST. OPHTHALMOL. 5: 208, Kapany, N.: Appendix N, in Strong, J.: Concepts of classical optics, San Francisco, 1948, J. H. Freeman Co., pp Bourdy, C: Eft'et Stiles-Crawford, et fixation, Vision Res. 10: 859, Enoch, J. M., and Hope, G. M.: An analysis of retinal receptor orientation III. Results of initial psychophysical tests, INVEST. OPHTHAL- MOL., to be submitted.

An analysis of retinal receptor orientation

An analysis of retinal receptor orientation An analysis of retinal receptor orientation IV. Center of the entrance pupil and the center of convergence of orientation and directional sensitivity Jay M. Enoch and G. M. Hope In the previous study,

More information

Original Articles. Directionality and waveguide properties of optically isolated rat rods. Frank L. Tobey, Jr., and Jay M. Enoch

Original Articles. Directionality and waveguide properties of optically isolated rat rods. Frank L. Tobey, Jr., and Jay M. Enoch Original Articles Directionality and waveguide properties of optically isolated rat rods Frank L. Tobey, Jr., and Jay M. Enoch We have succeeded in isolating radiation from single rat rod receptors at

More information

fringes were produced on the retina directly. Threshold contrasts optical aberrations in the eye. (Received 12 January 1967)

fringes were produced on the retina directly. Threshold contrasts optical aberrations in the eye. (Received 12 January 1967) J. Phy8iol. (1967), 19, pp. 583-593 583 With 5 text-figure8 Printed in Great Britain VISUAL RESOLUTION WHEN LIGHT ENTERS THE EYE THROUGH DIFFERENT PARTS OF THE PUPIL BY DANIEL G. GREEN From the Department

More information

Lecture 8. Lecture 8. r 1

Lecture 8. Lecture 8. r 1 Lecture 8 Achromat Design Design starts with desired Next choose your glass materials, i.e. Find P D P D, then get f D P D K K Choose radii (still some freedom left in choice of radii for minimization

More information

Image formation in fundus cameras

Image formation in fundus cameras Image formation in fundus cameras Oleg Pomerantzeff, Robert H. Webb, and Francois C. Delori Imaging in a fundus camera depends more on design of the system stop than on correction of the first fundus image

More information

Tangents. The f-stops here. Shedding some light on the f-number. by Marcus R. Hatch and David E. Stoltzmann

Tangents. The f-stops here. Shedding some light on the f-number. by Marcus R. Hatch and David E. Stoltzmann Tangents Shedding some light on the f-number The f-stops here by Marcus R. Hatch and David E. Stoltzmann The f-number has peen around for nearly a century now, and it is certainly one of the fundamental

More information

Introduction. Chapter Aim of the Thesis

Introduction. Chapter Aim of the Thesis Chapter 1 Introduction 1.1 Aim of the Thesis The main aim of this investigation was to develop a new instrument for measurement of light reflected from the retina in a living human eye. At the start of

More information

OPTICAL SYSTEMS OBJECTIVES

OPTICAL SYSTEMS OBJECTIVES 101 L7 OPTICAL SYSTEMS OBJECTIVES Aims Your aim here should be to acquire a working knowledge of the basic components of optical systems and understand their purpose, function and limitations in terms

More information

Visual Perception of Images

Visual Perception of Images Visual Perception of Images A processed image is usually intended to be viewed by a human observer. An understanding of how humans perceive visual stimuli the human visual system (HVS) is crucial to the

More information

ECEN 4606, UNDERGRADUATE OPTICS LAB

ECEN 4606, UNDERGRADUATE OPTICS LAB ECEN 4606, UNDERGRADUATE OPTICS LAB Lab 2: Imaging 1 the Telescope Original Version: Prof. McLeod SUMMARY: In this lab you will become familiar with the use of one or more lenses to create images of distant

More information

Visual Optics. Visual Optics - Introduction

Visual Optics. Visual Optics - Introduction Visual Optics Jim Schwiegerling, PhD Ophthalmology & Optical Sciences University of Arizona Visual Optics - Introduction In this course, the optical principals behind the workings of the eye and visual

More information

Visual System I Eye and Retina

Visual System I Eye and Retina Visual System I Eye and Retina Reading: BCP Chapter 9 www.webvision.edu The Visual System The visual system is the part of the NS which enables organisms to process visual details, as well as to perform

More information

Chapters 1 & 2. Definitions and applications Conceptual basis of photogrammetric processing

Chapters 1 & 2. Definitions and applications Conceptual basis of photogrammetric processing Chapters 1 & 2 Chapter 1: Photogrammetry Definitions and applications Conceptual basis of photogrammetric processing Transition from two-dimensional imagery to three-dimensional information Automation

More information

The best retinal location"

The best retinal location How many photons are required to produce a visual sensation? Measurement of the Absolute Threshold" In a classic experiment, Hecht, Shlaer & Pirenne (1942) created the optimum conditions: -Used the best

More information

Vision. The eye. Image formation. Eye defects & corrective lenses. Visual acuity. Colour vision. Lecture 3.5

Vision. The eye. Image formation. Eye defects & corrective lenses. Visual acuity. Colour vision. Lecture 3.5 Lecture 3.5 Vision The eye Image formation Eye defects & corrective lenses Visual acuity Colour vision Vision http://www.wired.com/wiredscience/2009/04/schizoillusion/ Perception of light--- eye-brain

More information

OPTICAL DEMONSTRATIONS ENTOPTIC PHENOMENA, VISION AND EYE ANATOMY

OPTICAL DEMONSTRATIONS ENTOPTIC PHENOMENA, VISION AND EYE ANATOMY OPTICAL DEMONSTRATIONS ENTOPTIC PHENOMENA, VISION AND EYE ANATOMY The pupil as a first line of defence against excessive light. DEMONSTRATION 1. PUPIL SHAPE; SIZE CHANGE Make a triangular shape with the

More information

Retina. Convergence. Early visual processing: retina & LGN. Visual Photoreptors: rods and cones. Visual Photoreptors: rods and cones.

Retina. Convergence. Early visual processing: retina & LGN. Visual Photoreptors: rods and cones. Visual Photoreptors: rods and cones. Announcements 1 st exam (next Thursday): Multiple choice (about 22), short answer and short essay don t list everything you know for the essay questions Book vs. lectures know bold terms for things that

More information

QUANTITATIVE STUDY OF VISUAL AFTER-IMAGES*

QUANTITATIVE STUDY OF VISUAL AFTER-IMAGES* Brit. J. Ophthal. (1953) 37, 165. QUANTITATIVE STUDY OF VISUAL AFTER-IMAGES* BY Northampton Polytechnic, London MUCH has been written on the persistence of visual sensation after the light stimulus has

More information

Performance Factors. Technical Assistance. Fundamental Optics

Performance Factors.   Technical Assistance. Fundamental Optics Performance Factors After paraxial formulas have been used to select values for component focal length(s) and diameter(s), the final step is to select actual lenses. As in any engineering problem, this

More information

Optical, receptoral, and retinal constraints on foveal and peripheral vision in the human neonate

Optical, receptoral, and retinal constraints on foveal and peripheral vision in the human neonate Vision Research 38 (1998) 3857 3870 Optical, receptoral, and retinal constraints on foveal and peripheral vision in the human neonate T. Rowan Candy a, *, James A. Crowell b, Martin S. Banks a a School

More information

The Photoreceptor Mosaic

The Photoreceptor Mosaic The Photoreceptor Mosaic Aristophanis Pallikaris IVO, University of Crete Institute of Vision and Optics 10th Aegean Summer School Overview Brief Anatomy Photoreceptors Categorization Visual Function Photoreceptor

More information

PHGY Physiology. SENSORY PHYSIOLOGY Vision. Martin Paré

PHGY Physiology. SENSORY PHYSIOLOGY Vision. Martin Paré PHGY 212 - Physiology SENSORY PHYSIOLOGY Vision Martin Paré Assistant Professor of Physiology & Psychology pare@biomed.queensu.ca http://brain.phgy.queensu.ca/pare The Process of Vision Vision is the process

More information

11/23/11. A few words about light nm The electromagnetic spectrum. BÓDIS Emőke 22 November Schematic structure of the eye

11/23/11. A few words about light nm The electromagnetic spectrum. BÓDIS Emőke 22 November Schematic structure of the eye 11/23/11 A few words about light 300-850nm 400-800 nm BÓDIS Emőke 22 November 2011 The electromagnetic spectrum see only 1/70 of the electromagnetic spectrum The External Structure: The Immediate Structure:

More information

Human Visual System. Prof. George Wolberg Dept. of Computer Science City College of New York

Human Visual System. Prof. George Wolberg Dept. of Computer Science City College of New York Human Visual System Prof. George Wolberg Dept. of Computer Science City College of New York Objectives In this lecture we discuss: - Structure of human eye - Mechanics of human visual system (HVS) - Brightness

More information

Visibility, Performance and Perception. Cooper Lighting

Visibility, Performance and Perception. Cooper Lighting Visibility, Performance and Perception Kenneth Siderius BSc, MIES, LC, LG Cooper Lighting 1 Vision It has been found that the ability to recognize detail varies with respect to four physical factors: 1.Contrast

More information

Non-linear projection of the retinal

Non-linear projection of the retinal Brit. J. Ophthal. (I974) 58, 709 Communications Non-linear projection of the retinal image in a wide-angle schematic eye N. DRASDO AND C. W. FOWLER From the Department of Ophthalmic Optics, University

More information

2 The First Steps in Vision

2 The First Steps in Vision 2 The First Steps in Vision 2 The First Steps in Vision A Little Light Physics Eyes That See light Retinal Information Processing Whistling in the Dark: Dark and Light Adaptation The Man Who Could Not

More information

The Human Visual System. Lecture 1. The Human Visual System. The Human Eye. The Human Retina. cones. rods. horizontal. bipolar. amacrine.

The Human Visual System. Lecture 1. The Human Visual System. The Human Eye. The Human Retina. cones. rods. horizontal. bipolar. amacrine. Lecture The Human Visual System The Human Visual System Retina Optic Nerve Optic Chiasm Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN) Visual Cortex The Human Eye The Human Retina Lens rods cones Cornea Fovea Optic

More information

Yokohama City University lecture INTRODUCTION TO HUMAN VISION Presentation notes 7/10/14

Yokohama City University lecture INTRODUCTION TO HUMAN VISION Presentation notes 7/10/14 Yokohama City University lecture INTRODUCTION TO HUMAN VISION Presentation notes 7/10/14 1. INTRODUCTION TO HUMAN VISION Self introduction Dr. Salmon Northeastern State University, Oklahoma. USA Teach

More information

Retinal contrast losses and visual resolution with obliquely incident light

Retinal contrast losses and visual resolution with obliquely incident light 69 J. Opt. Soc. Am. A/ Vol. 18, No. 11/ November 001 M. J. McMahon and D. I. A. MacLeod Retinal contrast losses and visual resolution with obliquely incident light Matthew J. McMahon* and Donald I. A.

More information

Laboratory 7: Properties of Lenses and Mirrors

Laboratory 7: Properties of Lenses and Mirrors Laboratory 7: Properties of Lenses and Mirrors Converging and Diverging Lens Focal Lengths: A converging lens is thicker at the center than at the periphery and light from an object at infinity passes

More information

Sensory receptors External internal stimulus change detectable energy transduce action potential different strengths different frequencies

Sensory receptors External internal stimulus change detectable energy transduce action potential different strengths different frequencies General aspects Sensory receptors ; respond to changes in the environment. External or internal environment. A stimulus is a change in the environmental condition which is detectable by a sensory receptor

More information

CHAPTER 33 ABERRATION CURVES IN LENS DESIGN

CHAPTER 33 ABERRATION CURVES IN LENS DESIGN CHAPTER 33 ABERRATION CURVES IN LENS DESIGN Donald C. O Shea Georgia Institute of Technology Center for Optical Science and Engineering and School of Physics Atlanta, Georgia Michael E. Harrigan Eastman

More information

J. Physiol. (I954) I23,

J. Physiol. (I954) I23, 357 J. Physiol. (I954) I23, 357-366 THE MINIMUM QUANTITY OF LIGHT REQUIRED TO ELICIT THE ACCOMMODATION REFLEX IN MAN BY F. W. CAMPBELL* From the Nuffield Laboratory of Ophthalmology, University of Oxford

More information

Optical Perspective of Polycarbonate Material

Optical Perspective of Polycarbonate Material Optical Perspective of Polycarbonate Material JP Wei, Ph. D. November 2011 Introduction Among the materials developed for eyeglasses, polycarbonate is one that has a number of very unique properties and

More information

10/8/ dpt. n 21 = n n' r D = The electromagnetic spectrum. A few words about light. BÓDIS Emőke 02 October Optical Imaging in the Eye

10/8/ dpt. n 21 = n n' r D = The electromagnetic spectrum. A few words about light. BÓDIS Emőke 02 October Optical Imaging in the Eye A few words about light BÓDIS Emőke 02 October 2012 Optical Imaging in the Eye Healthy eye: 25 cm, v1 v2 Let s determine the change in the refractive power between the two extremes during accommodation!

More information

DEFECTS OF VISION THROUGH APHAKIC SPECTACLE LENSES*t

DEFECTS OF VISION THROUGH APHAKIC SPECTACLE LENSES*t Brit. J. Ophthal. (1967) 51, 306 DEFECTS OF VISION THROUGH APHAKIC SPECTACLE LENSES*t BY ROBERT C. WELSH Miami, Florida BY the use of a series of scale diagrams an attempt is made to explain the following:

More information

OPTI-201/202 Geometrical and Instrumental Optics Copyright 2018 John E. Greivenkamp. Section 16. The Eye

OPTI-201/202 Geometrical and Instrumental Optics Copyright 2018 John E. Greivenkamp. Section 16. The Eye 16-1 Section 16 The Eye The Eye Ciliary Muscle Iris Pupil Optical Axis Visual Axis 16-2 Cornea Right Eye Horizontal Section Zonules Crystalline Lens Vitreous Sclera Retina Macula And Fovea Optic Nerve

More information

BIOPHYSICS OF VISION GEOMETRIC OPTICS OF HUMAN EYE. Refraction media of the human eye. D eye = 63 diopter, D cornea =40, D lens = 15+

BIOPHYSICS OF VISION GEOMETRIC OPTICS OF HUMAN EYE. Refraction media of the human eye. D eye = 63 diopter, D cornea =40, D lens = 15+ BIOPHYSICS OF VISION THEORY OF COLOR VISION ELECTRORETINOGRAM Two problems: All cows are black in dark! Playing tennis in dark with illuminated lines, rackets, net, and ball! Refraction media of the human

More information

PHGY Physiology. The Process of Vision. SENSORY PHYSIOLOGY Vision. Martin Paré. Visible Light. Ocular Anatomy. Ocular Anatomy.

PHGY Physiology. The Process of Vision. SENSORY PHYSIOLOGY Vision. Martin Paré. Visible Light. Ocular Anatomy. Ocular Anatomy. PHGY 212 - Physiology SENSORY PHYSIOLOGY Vision Martin Paré Assistant Professor of Physiology & Psychology pare@biomed.queensu.ca http://brain.phgy.queensu.ca/pare The Process of Vision Vision is the process

More information

COMMUNICATIONS THE ACCOMMODATION REFLEX AND ITS STIMULUS* powerful stimulus to this innervation is to be found in the disparity

COMMUNICATIONS THE ACCOMMODATION REFLEX AND ITS STIMULUS* powerful stimulus to this innervation is to be found in the disparity Brit. J. Ophthal., 35, 381. COMMUNICATIONS THE ACCOMMODATION REFLEX AND ITS STIMULUS* BY E. F. FINCHAM Ophthalmic Optics Department, Institute of Ophthalmology, London IT is well known in the practice

More information

2.71 Optics Fall 05 QUIZ 1 Wednesday, Oct. 12, 2005

2.71 Optics Fall 05 QUIZ 1 Wednesday, Oct. 12, 2005 2.71 Quiz 1 MASSACHUSETTS INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY 2.71 Optics Fall 05 QUIZ 1 Wednesday, Oct. 12, 2005 1. (60%) The optical instrument shown below is a telephoto lens. It consists of a combination of two

More information

PERIMETRY A STANDARD TEST IN OPHTHALMOLOGY

PERIMETRY A STANDARD TEST IN OPHTHALMOLOGY 7 CHAPTER 2 WHAT IS PERIMETRY? INTRODUCTION PERIMETRY A STANDARD TEST IN OPHTHALMOLOGY Perimetry is a standard method used in ophthalmol- It provides a measure of the patient s visual function - performed

More information

We have already discussed retinal structure and organization, as well as the photochemical and electrophysiological basis for vision.

We have already discussed retinal structure and organization, as well as the photochemical and electrophysiological basis for vision. LECTURE 4 SENSORY ASPECTS OF VISION We have already discussed retinal structure and organization, as well as the photochemical and electrophysiological basis for vision. At the beginning of the course,

More information

The Special Senses: Vision

The Special Senses: Vision OLLI Lecture 5 The Special Senses: Vision Vision The eyes are the sensory organs for vision. They collect light waves through their photoreceptors (located in the retina) and transmit them as nerve impulses

More information

assumptions-usually of uniformity of the retinal areas concernedabout

assumptions-usually of uniformity of the retinal areas concernedabout 364 J. Physiol. (1962), 16, pp. 364-373 With 5 text-figures Printed in Great Britain THE RELATIONSHIP OF VISUAL THRESHOLD TO RETINAL POSITION AND AREA By P. E. HALLETT,*, F. H. C. MARRIOTT AND F. C. RODGER

More information

Chapter 36. Image Formation

Chapter 36. Image Formation Chapter 36 Image Formation Image of Formation Images can result when light rays encounter flat or curved surfaces between two media. Images can be formed either by reflection or refraction due to these

More information

Section 22. The Eye The Eye. Ciliary Muscle. Sclera. Zonules. Macula And Fovea. Iris. Retina. Pupil. Optical Axis.

Section 22. The Eye The Eye. Ciliary Muscle. Sclera. Zonules. Macula And Fovea. Iris. Retina. Pupil. Optical Axis. Section 22 The Eye 22-1 The Eye Optical Axis Visual Axis Pupil Iris Cornea Right Eye Horizontal Section Ciliary Muscle Zonules Crystalline Lens Vitreous Sclera Retina Macula And Fovea Optic Nerve 22-2

More information

This question addresses OPTICAL factors in image formation, not issues involving retinal or other brain structures.

This question addresses OPTICAL factors in image formation, not issues involving retinal or other brain structures. Bonds 1. Cite three practical challenges in forming a clear image on the retina and describe briefly how each is met by the biological structure of the eye. Note that by challenges I do not refer to optical

More information

Instructional Resources/Materials: Light vocabulary cards printed (class set) Enough for each student (See card sort below)

Instructional Resources/Materials: Light vocabulary cards printed (class set) Enough for each student (See card sort below) Grade Level/Course: Grade 7 Life Science Lesson/Unit Plan Name: Light Card Sort Rationale/Lesson Abstract: Light vocabulary building, students identify and share vocabulary meaning. Timeframe: 10 to 20

More information

GROUPING BASED ON PHENOMENAL PROXIMITY

GROUPING BASED ON PHENOMENAL PROXIMITY Journal of Experimental Psychology 1964, Vol. 67, No. 6, 531-538 GROUPING BASED ON PHENOMENAL PROXIMITY IRVIN ROCK AND LEONARD BROSGOLE l Yeshiva University The question was raised whether the Gestalt

More information

Application Note (A11)

Application Note (A11) Application Note (A11) Slit and Aperture Selection in Spectroradiometry REVISION: C August 2013 Gooch & Housego 4632 36 th Street, Orlando, FL 32811 Tel: 1 407 422 3171 Fax: 1 407 648 5412 Email: sales@goochandhousego.com

More information

Chapter 36. Image Formation

Chapter 36. Image Formation Chapter 36 Image Formation Notation for Mirrors and Lenses The object distance is the distance from the object to the mirror or lens Denoted by p The image distance is the distance from the image to the

More information

Digital Image Processing

Digital Image Processing Digital Image Processing Lecture # 3 Digital Image Fundamentals ALI JAVED Lecturer SOFTWARE ENGINEERING DEPARTMENT U.E.T TAXILA Email:: ali.javed@uettaxila.edu.pk Office Room #:: 7 Presentation Outline

More information

The eye* The eye is a slightly asymmetrical globe, about an inch in diameter. The front part of the eye (the part you see in the mirror) includes:

The eye* The eye is a slightly asymmetrical globe, about an inch in diameter. The front part of the eye (the part you see in the mirror) includes: The eye* The eye is a slightly asymmetrical globe, about an inch in diameter. The front part of the eye (the part you see in the mirror) includes: The iris (the pigmented part) The cornea (a clear dome

More information

Opti 415/515. Introduction to Optical Systems. Copyright 2009, William P. Kuhn

Opti 415/515. Introduction to Optical Systems. Copyright 2009, William P. Kuhn Opti 415/515 Introduction to Optical Systems 1 Optical Systems Manipulate light to form an image on a detector. Point source microscope Hubble telescope (NASA) 2 Fundamental System Requirements Application

More information

STUDY NOTES UNIT I IMAGE PERCEPTION AND SAMPLING. Elements of Digital Image Processing Systems. Elements of Visual Perception structure of human eye

STUDY NOTES UNIT I IMAGE PERCEPTION AND SAMPLING. Elements of Digital Image Processing Systems. Elements of Visual Perception structure of human eye DIGITAL IMAGE PROCESSING STUDY NOTES UNIT I IMAGE PERCEPTION AND SAMPLING Elements of Digital Image Processing Systems Elements of Visual Perception structure of human eye light, luminance, brightness

More information

Lenses- Worksheet. (Use a ray box to answer questions 3 to 7)

Lenses- Worksheet. (Use a ray box to answer questions 3 to 7) Lenses- Worksheet 1. Look at the lenses in front of you and try to distinguish the different types of lenses? Describe each type and record its characteristics. 2. Using the lenses in front of you, look

More information

Early Visual Processing: Receptive Fields & Retinal Processing (Chapter 2, part 2)

Early Visual Processing: Receptive Fields & Retinal Processing (Chapter 2, part 2) Early Visual Processing: Receptive Fields & Retinal Processing (Chapter 2, part 2) Lecture 5 Jonathan Pillow Sensation & Perception (PSY 345 / NEU 325) Princeton University, Spring 2015 1 Summary of last

More information

PHYSICS FOR THE IB DIPLOMA CAMBRIDGE UNIVERSITY PRESS

PHYSICS FOR THE IB DIPLOMA CAMBRIDGE UNIVERSITY PRESS Option C Imaging C Introduction to imaging Learning objectives In this section we discuss the formation of images by lenses and mirrors. We will learn how to construct images graphically as well as algebraically.

More information

2 Optics of the Human Eye

2 Optics of the Human Eye Optometry Chap. c02 2013/11/11 page 15 le-tex 15 2 Optics of the Human Eye In all ophthalmic and optometric devices to be presented, the eye is an essential part of the entire optical system. Because of

More information

Objectives. 3. Visual acuity. Layers of the. eye ball. 1. Conjunctiva : is. three quarters. posteriorly and

Objectives. 3. Visual acuity. Layers of the. eye ball. 1. Conjunctiva : is. three quarters. posteriorly and OCULAR PHYSIOLOGY (I) Dr.Ahmed Al Shaibani Lab.2 Oct.2013 Objectives 1. Review of ocular anatomy (Ex. after image) 2. Visual pathway & field (Ex. Crossed & uncrossed diplopia, mechanical stimulation of

More information

J. Physiol. (I952) i 6,

J. Physiol. (I952) i 6, 350 J. Physiol. (I952) i 6, 350-356 THE REFLEXION OF LIGHT FROM THE MACULAR AND PERIPHERAL FUNDUS OCULI IN MAN BY G. S. BRINDLEY AND E. N. WILLMER From the Physiological Laboratory, University of Cambridge

More information

Simple method of determining the axial length of the eye

Simple method of determining the axial length of the eye Brit. Y. Ophthal. (1976) 6o, 266 Simple method of determining the axial length of the eye E. S. PERKINS, B. HAMMOND, AND A. B. MILLIKEN From the Department of Experimental Ophthalmology, Institute of Ophthalmology,

More information

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I Monocular Sensory Processes of Vision: Color Vision Ronald S. Harwerth, OD, PhD Office: Room 2160 Office hours: By appointment Telephone: 713-743-1940 email: rharwerth@uh.edu

More information

The Human Brain and Senses: Memory

The Human Brain and Senses: Memory The Human Brain and Senses: Memory Methods of Learning Learning - There are several types of memory, and each is processed in a different part of the brain. Remembering Mirror Writing Today we will be.

More information

Projector for interference figures and for direct measurement of 2V.

Projector for interference figures and for direct measurement of 2V. 666 Projector for interference figures and for direct measurement of 2V. By H. C. G. VINCENT, M.A., A.R.I.C., F.G.S. Department of Geology, University of Cape Town. [Taken as read March 24, 1955.] T HE

More information

Vision Science I Exam 1 23 September ) The plot to the right shows the spectrum of a light source. Which of the following sources is this

Vision Science I Exam 1 23 September ) The plot to the right shows the spectrum of a light source. Which of the following sources is this Vision Science I Exam 1 23 September 2016 1) The plot to the right shows the spectrum of a light source. Which of the following sources is this spectrum most likely to be taken from? A) The direct sunlight

More information

Lens Design I. Lecture 3: Properties of optical systems II Herbert Gross. Summer term

Lens Design I. Lecture 3: Properties of optical systems II Herbert Gross. Summer term Lens Design I Lecture 3: Properties of optical systems II 205-04-8 Herbert Gross Summer term 206 www.iap.uni-jena.de 2 Preliminary Schedule 04.04. Basics 2.04. Properties of optical systrems I 3 8.04.

More information

Eye. Eye Major structural layer of the wall of the eye is a thick layer of dense C.T.; that layer has two parts:

Eye. Eye Major structural layer of the wall of the eye is a thick layer of dense C.T.; that layer has two parts: General aspects Sensory receptors ; External or internal environment. A stimulus is a change in the environmental condition which is detectable by a sensory receptor 1 Major structural layer of the wall

More information

VISUAL PHYSICS ONLINE DEPTH STUDY: ELECTRON MICROSCOPES

VISUAL PHYSICS ONLINE DEPTH STUDY: ELECTRON MICROSCOPES VISUAL PHYSICS ONLINE DEPTH STUDY: ELECTRON MICROSCOPES Shortly after the experimental confirmation of the wave properties of the electron, it was suggested that the electron could be used to examine objects

More information

HARD TORIC CONTACT LENSES ASTIGMATISM DEFINITION AND OPTIC BASIS

HARD TORIC CONTACT LENSES ASTIGMATISM DEFINITION AND OPTIC BASIS Mario Giovanzana Milano 20.06.01 HARD TORIC CONTACT LENSES ASTIGMATISM DEFINITION AND OPTIC BASIS An astigmatism, according to Whevell (1817) has been defined as astigmatism or astigmatic ametropia; the

More information

Slide 1. Slide 2. Slide 3. Light and Colour. Sir Isaac Newton The Founder of Colour Science

Slide 1. Slide 2. Slide 3. Light and Colour. Sir Isaac Newton The Founder of Colour Science Slide 1 the Rays to speak properly are not coloured. In them there is nothing else than a certain Power and Disposition to stir up a Sensation of this or that Colour Sir Isaac Newton (1730) Slide 2 Light

More information

Basic Optics System OS-8515C

Basic Optics System OS-8515C 40 50 30 60 20 70 10 80 0 90 80 10 20 70 T 30 60 40 50 50 40 60 30 70 20 80 90 90 80 BASIC OPTICS RAY TABLE 10 0 10 70 20 60 50 40 30 Instruction Manual with Experiment Guide and Teachers Notes 012-09900B

More information

Reading. 1. Visual perception. Outline. Forming an image. Optional: Glassner, Principles of Digital Image Synthesis, sections

Reading. 1. Visual perception. Outline. Forming an image. Optional: Glassner, Principles of Digital Image Synthesis, sections Reading Optional: Glassner, Principles of Digital mage Synthesis, sections 1.1-1.6. 1. Visual perception Brian Wandell. Foundations of Vision. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, MA, 1995. Research papers:

More information

APPLICATIONS FOR TELECENTRIC LIGHTING

APPLICATIONS FOR TELECENTRIC LIGHTING APPLICATIONS FOR TELECENTRIC LIGHTING Telecentric lenses used in combination with telecentric lighting provide the most accurate results for measurement of object shapes and geometries. They make attributes

More information

Section 2 concludes that a glare meter based on a digital camera is probably too expensive to develop and produce, and may not be simple in use.

Section 2 concludes that a glare meter based on a digital camera is probably too expensive to develop and produce, and may not be simple in use. Possible development of a simple glare meter Kai Sørensen, 17 September 2012 Introduction, summary and conclusion Disability glare is sometimes a problem in road traffic situations such as: - at road works

More information

III: Vision. Objectives:

III: Vision. Objectives: III: Vision Objectives: Describe the characteristics of visible light, and explain the process by which the eye transforms light energy into neural. Describe how the eye and the brain process visual information.

More information

INTRODUCTION THIN LENSES. Introduction. given by the paraxial refraction equation derived last lecture: Thin lenses (19.1) = 1. Double-lens systems

INTRODUCTION THIN LENSES. Introduction. given by the paraxial refraction equation derived last lecture: Thin lenses (19.1) = 1. Double-lens systems Chapter 9 OPTICAL INSTRUMENTS Introduction Thin lenses Double-lens systems Aberrations Camera Human eye Compound microscope Summary INTRODUCTION Knowledge of geometrical optics, diffraction and interference,

More information

Chapter Six Chapter Six

Chapter Six Chapter Six Chapter Six Chapter Six Vision Sight begins with Light The advantages of electromagnetic radiation (Light) as a stimulus are Electromagnetic energy is abundant, travels VERY quickly and in fairly straight

More information

Chapter 2: Digital Image Fundamentals. Digital image processing is based on. Mathematical and probabilistic models Human intuition and analysis

Chapter 2: Digital Image Fundamentals. Digital image processing is based on. Mathematical and probabilistic models Human intuition and analysis Chapter 2: Digital Image Fundamentals Digital image processing is based on Mathematical and probabilistic models Human intuition and analysis 2.1 Visual Perception How images are formed in the eye? Eye

More information

Psych 333, Winter 2008, Instructor Boynton, Exam 1

Psych 333, Winter 2008, Instructor Boynton, Exam 1 Name: Class: Date: Psych 333, Winter 2008, Instructor Boynton, Exam 1 Multiple Choice There are 35 multiple choice questions worth one point each. Identify the letter of the choice that best completes

More information

AP PSYCH Unit 4.2 Vision 1. How does the eye transform light energy into neural messages? 2. How does the brain process visual information? 3.

AP PSYCH Unit 4.2 Vision 1. How does the eye transform light energy into neural messages? 2. How does the brain process visual information? 3. AP PSYCH Unit 4.2 Vision 1. How does the eye transform light energy into neural messages? 2. How does the brain process visual information? 3. What theories help us understand color vision? 4. Is your

More information

THE RECEPTIVE FIELDS OF OPTIC NERVE FIBERS

THE RECEPTIVE FIELDS OF OPTIC NERVE FIBERS THE RECEPTIVE FIELDS OF OPTIC NERVE FIBERS H. K. HARTLINE From the Eldridge Reeves Johnson Research Foundation, Philadelphia University of Pennsylvania, Received for publication May 18, 1940 Appreciation

More information

Test procedures Page: 1 of 5

Test procedures Page: 1 of 5 Test procedures Page: 1 of 5 1 Scope This part of document establishes uniform requirements for measuring the numerical aperture of optical fibre, thereby assisting in the inspection of fibres and cables

More information

EYE ANATOMY. Multimedia Health Education. Disclaimer

EYE ANATOMY. Multimedia Health Education. Disclaimer Disclaimer This movie is an educational resource only and should not be used to manage your health. The information in this presentation has been intended to help consumers understand the structure and

More information

DIGITAL IMAGE PROCESSING LECTURE # 4 DIGITAL IMAGE FUNDAMENTALS-I

DIGITAL IMAGE PROCESSING LECTURE # 4 DIGITAL IMAGE FUNDAMENTALS-I DIGITAL IMAGE PROCESSING LECTURE # 4 DIGITAL IMAGE FUNDAMENTALS-I 4 Topics to Cover Light and EM Spectrum Visual Perception Structure Of Human Eyes Image Formation on the Eye Brightness Adaptation and

More information

Simple reaction time as a function of luminance for various wavelengths*

Simple reaction time as a function of luminance for various wavelengths* Perception & Psychophysics, 1971, Vol. 10 (6) (p. 397, column 1) Copyright 1971, Psychonomic Society, Inc., Austin, Texas SIU-C Web Editorial Note: This paper originally was published in three-column text

More information

This experiment is under development and thus we appreciate any and all comments as we design an interesting and achievable set of goals.

This experiment is under development and thus we appreciate any and all comments as we design an interesting and achievable set of goals. Experiment 7 Geometrical Optics You will be introduced to ray optics and image formation in this experiment. We will use the optical rail, lenses, and the camera body to quantify image formation and magnification;

More information

Section 3. Imaging With A Thin Lens

Section 3. Imaging With A Thin Lens 3-1 Section 3 Imaging With A Thin Lens Object at Infinity An object at infinity produces a set of collimated set of rays entering the optical system. Consider the rays from a finite object located on the

More information

LIGHT ABSORPTION AND SCATTER IN THE HUMAN LENS

LIGHT ABSORPTION AND SCATTER IN THE HUMAN LENS Vision Research Vol. 11, pp129-141. Pergamon Press 1971 LIGHT ABSORPTION AND SCATTER IN THE HUMAN LENS J. MELLERIO Department of Physiological Optics, Institute of Ophthalmology, Judd Street, London WC1H

More information

Vision. By. Leanora Thompson, Karen Vega, and Abby Brainerd

Vision. By. Leanora Thompson, Karen Vega, and Abby Brainerd Vision By. Leanora Thompson, Karen Vega, and Abby Brainerd Anatomy Outermost part of the eye is the Sclera. Cornea transparent part of outer layer Two cavities by the lens. Anterior cavity = Aqueous humor

More information

EYE STRUCTURE AND FUNCTION

EYE STRUCTURE AND FUNCTION Name: Class: Date: EYE STRUCTURE AND FUNCTION The eye is the body s organ of sight. It gathers light from the environment and forms an image on specialized nerve cells on the retina. Vision occurs when

More information

SMALL VOLUNTARY MOVEMENTS OF THE EYE*

SMALL VOLUNTARY MOVEMENTS OF THE EYE* Brit. J. Ophthal. (1953) 37, 746. SMALL VOLUNTARY MOVEMENTS OF THE EYE* BY B. L. GINSBORG Physics Department, University of Reading IT is well known that the transfer of the gaze from one point to another,

More information

Vision. By: Karen, Jaqui, and Jen

Vision. By: Karen, Jaqui, and Jen Vision By: Karen, Jaqui, and Jen Activity: Directions: Stare at the black dot in the center of the picture don't look at anything else but the black dot. When we switch the picture you can look around

More information

New foveated wide angle lens with high resolving power and without brightness loss in the periphery

New foveated wide angle lens with high resolving power and without brightness loss in the periphery New foveated wide angle lens with high resolving power and without brightness loss in the periphery K. Wakamiya *a, T. Senga a, K. Isagi a, N. Yamamura a, Y. Ushio a and N. Kita b a Nikon Corp., 6-3,Nishi-ohi

More information

EXAMINATION OF THE CENTRAL VISUAL FIELD AT

EXAMINATION OF THE CENTRAL VISUAL FIELD AT Brit. J. Ophthal. (1968) 52, 408 EXAMINATION OF THE CENTRAL VISUAL FIELD AT A READING DISTANCE*t BY V. N. HIGHMAN Moorfields Eye Hospital, City Road, London THIS investigation was started in an attempt

More information

X rays X-ray properties Denser material = more absorption = looks lighter on the x-ray photo X-rays CT Scans circle cross-sectional images Tumours

X rays X-ray properties Denser material = more absorption = looks lighter on the x-ray photo X-rays CT Scans circle cross-sectional images Tumours X rays X-ray properties X-rays are part of the electromagnetic spectrum. X-rays have a wavelength of the same order of magnitude as the diameter of an atom. X-rays are ionising. Different materials absorb

More information

Chapter 18 Optical Elements

Chapter 18 Optical Elements Chapter 18 Optical Elements GOALS When you have mastered the content of this chapter, you will be able to achieve the following goals: Definitions Define each of the following terms and use it in an operational

More information

Geometric optics & aberrations

Geometric optics & aberrations Geometric optics & aberrations Department of Astrophysical Sciences University AST 542 http://www.northerneye.co.uk/ Outline Introduction: Optics in astronomy Basics of geometric optics Paraxial approximation

More information