Deceptive plumage signals in birds: manipulation of predators or prey?

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "Deceptive plumage signals in birds: manipulation of predators or prey?"

Transcription

1 Blackwell Publishing LtdOxford, UKBIJBiological Journal of the Linnean Society The Linnean Society of London? Original Article DECEPTIVE FALSE FACE IN BIRDS J. J. NEGRO ET AL. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2007, 90, With 3 figures Deceptive plumage signals in birds: manipulation of predators or prey? JUAN JOSÉ NEGRO 1, GARY R. BORTOLOTTI 2 * and JOSÉ HERNÁN SARASOLA 1 1 Department of Applied Biology, Estación Biológica de Doñana, Avda de María Luisa s/n, Pabellón del Perú, Sevilla, Spain 2 Department of Biology, University of Saskatchewan, 112 Science Pl, Saskatoon SK S7N 5E2, Canada Received 11 October 2005; accepted for publication 1 May 2006 Several species of raptors have two ocelli (eye-like patches) in the back of their head, giving them the appearance of a false face, although this trait has rarely been reported. According to our observations, these markings may be widespread in the family Falconidae, some Accipiter hawks, and some owls (e.g. Glaucidium, and some Athene, Aegolius, and Surnia). In this study, we outline general classes of hypotheses that may account for ocelli on the heads of raptors. The most frequently evoked general hypothesis is that ocelli offer protection against attackers. For example, a predator may abort the effort, be deflected by the ocelli, or be warned that the bearer will retaliate if attacked. We propose two alternatives. Most raptors with ocelli typically include a large proportion of passerine birds in their diets. The false faces may have evolved to aid in the hunting of small birds by actually provoking, or manipulating the nature of, a mobbing response. The benefit of doing so may either be immediate because there are numerous accounts of mobbers being killed, or the benefit may be postponed if the predator is using mobbing as a means of evaluating hunting prospects in a given area. An analysis of the pygmy-owls of the genus Glaucidium indicates that species displaying ocelli in the nape tend to have a high proportion of small birds in their diets and live in open habitats, whereas the opposite is true for species without ocelli. Pygmy-owls with ocelli are also considerably smaller and, collectively, these findings are most consistent with false faces being a conspicuous visual signal to deceive mobbing birds so they can be preyed upon The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2007, 90, ADDITIONAL KEYWORDS: deceptive signal false face hunting techniques mobbing ocelli raptors. INTRODUCTION The coloration of animals may be adaptive in at least three different ways: (1) providing camouflage; (2) for communication in intra- or interspecific interactions; and (3) serving a physiological function (e.g. protecting against ultraviolet radiation) (Burtt, 1981; Savalli, 1995; Ortolani, 1999). As an example of case 2, animals of widely different taxa, including vertebrates and invertebrates, are said to deceive predators by displaying conspicuous colour markings in parts of their bodies; but see also Zahavi & Zahavi (1997). Perhaps some of the best known examples involve ocelli, or false eyes, on the wings of moths or on the bodies of some caterpillars and fish (Cott, 1940). *Corresponding author. gary.bortolotti@usask.ca In this study, we are concerned with the false face exhibited on the back of the heads of birds of prey, both diurnal raptors and owls. These markings have passed unnoticed or at least unreported so far for most species, except for the American kestrel (Falco sparverius L.) (Clay, 1953), pygmy-owls of the genus Glaucidium (Steyn, 1979; del Hoyo, Elliot & Sargatal, 1999; Deppe et al., 2003), and, more recently, the Eurasian hobby (Falco subbuteo L.) (Negro, Grande & Sarasola, 2004). The effect of a false face is generally achieved by the presence of two rounded spots in the occipital area, which are either lighter or darker in colour than the surrounding plumage in the head. These markings have also been termed occipital spots, nuchal or nape patches, eyespots, false eyes, dorsal faces, and combinations of related terms. We use ocelli for the spots and false faces for the overall visual phenomenon, but 467

2 468 J. J. NEGRO ET AL. emphasize that this should not bias the interpretation of function. However, in birds at large, ocelli are also found in nonraptorial species such as the peacock (Pavo cristatus L.), wild turkey (Meleagris gallopavo L.), and some pheasants (del Hoyo, Elliot & Sargatal, 1994). These ocelli are rounded spots formed by circular patterns and colours and often are more eye-like in appearance in that they typically have a pupil. They tend to occur in single feathers, although an individual bird can display many of them as in the peacock s train. These single-feather ocelli appear to play a role in mate choice (Petrie, Halliday & Sanders, 1991). Due to their developmental complexity, these ocelli appear to be costly traits, and probably evolved through sexual selection (Andersson, 1994; Maynard Smith & Harper, 2003). The ocelli on the heads of raptors (here including both hawks and owls) are of a different nature than those of peacocks and pheasants. The former are a visual effect created by several small, similarly coloured contour feathers on the nape, which visually stand out against contiguous contour feathers of a different colour (for a description of ocelli in hobbies, see Negro et al., 2004). Unlike the ornamental, singlefeather ocelli that are found exclusively on males, ocelli of birds of prey do not meet the criteria for sexually selected traits (Andersson, 1994) because, within species, they often occur in both sexes and all age groups including nestlings. Although the most common explanation for ocelli is that they confer protection against predators (Cott, 1940), there is no single test of this hypothesis for birds. The possible functions of the ocelli of the American kestrel and pygmy-owls have been debated. Clay (1953) proposed that the evolution of such a complex pattern in the American kestrel by chance alone was highly improbable, and suggested plausible functions: (1) the ocelli were deflective marks, sensu Cott (1940), to misdirect the attack of predators or mobbing birds and (2) prey, in particular small birds, might be mislead as to where an attacking kestrel was actually looking (although he did not give much credence to this idea). Mueller (1971) proposed that the ocelli of the American kestrel were used in territorial contests although this appears to unlikely given what is known of this species behaviour (Balgooyen, 1975). The ocelli of kestrels may instead give the illusion of watchful eyes that would make predators or competitors think that the falcon had discovered them (Balgooyen, 1975). More recently, Deppe et al. (2003) used painted wooden models of northern pygmy-owls (Glaucidium gnoma Wagler) with and without occipital ocelli to test whether avian mobbers were influenced, or not, by those markings. The presence of ocelli appeared to redirect the attacks towards the front of the model, and thus the study by Deppe et al. (2003) provides the first empirical evidence for a link between ocelli and avian mobbers. The American kestrel and the pygmy-owls are not unique among birds of prey in having ocelli. The recognition of several new species with such plumage traits warrants a quest for general explanations. In our opinion, there are two general classes of hypotheses: (1) the illusion of false faces may confer protection to the bearers or (2) they may be an aid in capturing prey. Here, we present a description of the phenomenon in several raptors where it has been previously unreported. We also review mobbing behaviour because it provides a crucial perspective for several arguments, and propose a series of functional hypotheses for the evolution of raptorial ocelli. We outline predictions of these hypotheses and test them using the genus Glaucidium, a large group of owls characterized for having evolved the most perfect and undisputed nuchal ocelli/false face in birds (at least to human eyes). SPECIES WITH OCELLI We have only found published references to a false face in American kestrels, pygmy-owls, the northern hawk owl Surnia ulula L. (del Hoyo et al., 1999; Svensson et al., 2000), the Eurasian hobby (Negro et al., 2004), and two little owl species of the genus Athene (König, Weick & Becking, 1999). In addition, Jenkins, Wagner & Hoffman (1991) mentioned nape patches for the Taita falcon (Falco fasciinucha Reichenow and Neumann), and Steyn (1979) describes ocelli in the bat hawk (Macheiramphus alcinus Bonaparte). Our sources for determining the presence of ocelli in raptors have been, in part, colour plates and photographs in books, including field guides and handbooks (Cramp & Simmons, 1977; del Hoyo et al., 1994, 1999; Forsman, 1999; König et al., 1999; Svensson et al., 2000; Wheeler, 2003). A general problem with bird plates and photos is that individuals tend to be presented frontally. In addition, artists may sometimes misrepresent the markings, and species descriptions do not usually include the back of the head. Therefore, we also examined museum specimens, and live birds in the field, zoos, rehabilitation centres, and private collections (see Acknowledgements). The family Falconidae shows a high prevalence of species with ocelli, and it is clear that these markings have appeared in phylogenetically distinct lineages (Fig. 1). It is not our intention to present an exhaustive survey and a description of species. A more complete analysis within Falco will be presented elsewhere (G. R. Bortolotti and J. J. Negro, unpubl. data). We have chosen examples where ocelli/false faces are reasonably distinct and usually present in males and females of both young and adults (Fig. 1).

3 DECEPTIVE FALSE FACE IN BIRDS 469 Figure 1. Phylogenetic tree of the Falconidae based on the sequence of the mitochondrial cytochrome b gene, adapted from Wink & Sauer-Gürth (2004). The black highlighted taxa are species for which ocelli are present. The grey highlighted taxa have ocelli in only some plumages (e.g. depending on sex and age). The remaining taxa do not have ocelli. In some species, such as the peregrine falcon (Falco peregrinus Tunstall), the presence of ocelli is age- and subspecies-dependant. Even within the species presented, the markings of interest are not always obvious and may require a particular perspective or feather disposition of the birds under observation. The ocelli of some species are hardly visible when the feathers are flattened over the head, as in museum specimens (Clay, 1953), which may explain why these spots have been overlooked or misrepresented in most species. Ocelli as discussed here are located on the nape and in two visually distinct areas. In the Fal-

4 470 J. J. NEGRO ET AL. conidae, the occipital spots are round or elliptical and of a lighter colour than the surrounding feathers. However, in American kestrels, and lanner (Falco biarmicus Temminck) and barbary (Falco pelegrinoides Temminck) falcons, ocelli are dark patches surrounded by a lighter colour. In the latter species, the ocelli may mimic the true eyes of the bird in the front. However, in species where the ocelli are light in colour, they may cover much of the back of the head when fully displayed and convey the impression of an owl s facial disk. In this case, of which hobbies and saker falcons (Falco cherrug Gray) are good examples, it may be more appropriate to speak of a false owl face. The image of a face per se is reinforced in many species by the appearance of the triangular dark stripe pointing down that separates the two spots, and may mimic a raptor s bill (Balgooyen, 1975). The owl illusion is reinforced in falcons because they have rather short necks and blunt faces (Cade, 1982; del Hoyo et al., 1994). They are also big-headed to accommodate their large eyes, more so when they fluff out the feathers of the cheeks, nape, and crown (Cade, 1982), when the ocelli are typically most visible. Because the ocelli are located side by side, an owl-like image is particularly evident because Strigiformes are the only birds with frontally placed eyes. Falcons are thus morphologically preadapted to resemble owls. Furthermore, Clay (1953) noticed that the American kestrel s habit of head-bobbing accentuated the conspicuousness of the ocelli and enhanced the resemblance of the ocelli to the eyes of an owl. In the case of the Falconidae, the spots are distinct areas separated by entire feathers of a different colour. By contrast, many Accipitridae have a single pale area on the back of the head previously described as the cryptic occipital spot by Hafner & Hafner (1977). For most hawks and allies, this large, triangular area of basally white feathers gives a very different visual effect than the occelli we discuss here. However, both sparrowhawks (Accipiter nisus L.) and bat hawks (Steyn, 1979) show two distinct spots even though the pale coloration (at the base of black-tipped feathers) is not confined to two areas of the head (G. R. Bortolotti, J. J. Negro & J. H. Sarasola, pers. observ.). In the case of pygmy-owls, ocelli are typically distinct spots, as they are in the Falconidae, which are dark (black or brown) and resemble enlarged true eyes. Approximately three-quarters of the many species of pygmy-owls (depending on the taxonomy used) have such spots (König et al., 1999; see below). In some cases (e.g. Glaucidium tephronotum Sharpe) spots are indistinct whereas, in others, they may appear but be incorrectly identified as a nuchal collar (Finch-Davies & Kemp, 1980: 330). For those species with ocelli, the owls seem to mimic themselves with remarkable accuracy (Bent, 1961; Steyn, 1979). The effect of a false face is reinforced in some species of Glaucidium and Athene by the presence of whitish eyebrows over the dark, rounded spot (König et al., 1999). Ocelli in Athene tend to not be as distinct as in pygmy-owls and are apparently lacking in some populations (e.g. Athene noctua Scopoli) (G. R. Bortolotti & J. J. Negro, pers. observ.), nor are they immediately obvious in Surnia, where a false face is created by diagonal stripes running from the top of the head to the nape (Svensson et al., 2000; Johnsgard, 2002; G. R. Bortolotti & J. J. Negro pers. observ.). Johnsgard (2002) suggested that these diagonal stripes might be an evolutionary precursor to the ocelli of pygmy-owls. We have also observed these diagonal stripes and false faces on the saw-whet (Aegolius acadicus Gmelis) and boreal (Tengmalm s) (Aegolius funereus L.) owl. AVIAN MOBBING Mobbing is a widespread antipredator behaviour in animals (Ostreiher, 2003) that is commonly employed by passerine birds against diurnal raptors and owls (Curio, 1978a, b; Caro, 1986). A considerable amount of research has been conducted on both proximate factors that elicit mobbing (Hinde, 1954a, b; Altmann, 1956; Curio, 1975), and its potential adaptive significance (Curio, 1978a, b; McLean & Rhodes, 1991). The question of the adaptive significance of avian mobbing is far from being solved (McLean & Rhodes, 1991; Zahavi & Zahavi, 1997). The reason why so much effort has been put into studying the harassment of predators involves the great risks posed by such behaviour, and the nature of the consequences to predators. It is clear that, in many cases, mobbing birds place themselves in deadly risk by approaching their predators (Curio & Regelmann, 1985; Sordahl, 1990). During mobbing, individuals come close to their potential predators and move around them emitting alarm calls (Altmann, 1956). The literature abounds with examples where mobbers actually strike their targets, and it is not uncommon for them to be so absorbed in the attacks that they continue the behaviour long after the predator has left the area (Altmann, 1956; Sparks & Soper, 1989; G. R. Bortolotti, J. J. Negro & J. H. Sarasola, pers. observ.). Mobbers may even be at risk from secondary predators because they can be so fixated on their target (Altmann, 1956; Walker, 1983). Humans have exploited this behaviour by attracting birds for the purpose of killing them for food by using owls as lures (Sparks & Soper, 1989). There are also numerous reports of harassers being killed by the predator they are mobbing (Curio & Regelmann, 1985; Poiani & Yorke, 1989; Sordahl, 1990; J. L. Alcaide pers. comm.). Although the costs seem obvious, the specific benefits of mobbing are far from clear (McLean & Rhodes, 1991; Ostreiher, 2003). Harassment may force

5 DECEPTIVE FALSE FACE IN BIRDS 471 raptors to move on to other areas (Flasskamp, 1994), or provide a way to increase social status or prestige to the mobbers (Zahavi & Zahavi, 1997). It is clear that mobbing behaviour can negatively affect raptors in a variety of ways. The damage may vary from lost hunting opportunities (Bildstein, 1982; Flasskamp, 1994; Pavey & Smyth, 1998), to suffering distress and physical damage (Poiani & Yorke, 1989; Koehler, 1992; Flasskamp, 1994), or death (Bent, 1961), and there may even be population consequences because it has been suggested (Mikkola, 1983: 116) that the Eurasian pygmy-owl (Glaucidium passerinum L.) in the Black Forest of Germany was mobbed to local extinction. HYPOTHESES FOR THE FUNCTION OF FALSE FACES IN PREDATORS Unlike other predator prey relationships that may have given rise to defensive false faces in prey species, such as in moths hunted by birds, here we have a signalling predator with three potential types of receivers: (1) predators; (2) prey of the signaller; (3) and conspecifics. A deceptive false face may protect its bearer against surprise attacks by any of the three types of receivers. However, if the signal was an aid in hunting, it would only be directed to the behaviour of potential prey. Along with formulating the hypotheses, we briefly present predictions regarding both the signal (i.e. its detectability) and the signaller (i.e. attributes of the raptor) (Table 1). PROTECTION HYPOTHESES Preconditions for the following group of hypotheses are that the species involved, even though they are predators themselves, are in turn depredated or are at risk in intraspecific conflicts, or are harassed by other animals (i.e. mobbers). HYPOTHESIS 1: DECEPTIVE DETECTION Deceptive detection refers to the fact that a predator may be fooled into thinking that the prey (the raptor in this case) is actually aware of the predator when it is not. Descriptions of how this would work have so far focused on the false face functioning to abort a strike (Clay, 1953). However, we believe a more plausible scenario is that the ocelli would give the appearance that the raptor (as prey) was vigilant, and hence an attack by another raptor would not be initiated. Hafner & Hafner (1977) suggested that the cryptic occipital spot of the Accipitridae could function in deflection. They proposed that the bright white spot formed a facial disc that would deflect predatory attacks on sleeping birds. We believe this is implausible because the single spot does not appear face-like and, as noted by Hafner & Hafner (1977), it makes the individual more conspicuous, which surely must be disadvantageous to a roosting bird. Steyn (1979) proposed that the false face may be advantageous when a pygmy-owl was on its nest and a predator, upon inspecting the cavity, would see a hint of a face in the darkness. HYPOTHESIS 2: DEFLECTION TO LESS VITAL PARTS OF THE BODY This hypothesis assumes that the attackers, whether predators, conspecifics or mobbers, aim at the eyes of their victim, which are the most sensitive areas in the head. Contrary to the previous hypothesis, the attack actually takes place, but as the attacker is fooled, blows are directed to the back of the head. This deflection hypothesis has been well developed by Cott (1940) Table 1. Summary of predictions for five hypotheses under two general categories, protection or predatory aid, concerning the function of ocelli in raptors Protection Predatory aid Deceptive detection Deflection Startle Mobber manipulation Mobber census Detectability of the signal Permanent ocelli Y Y N Y Y Ocelli displayed at will N Y Y Y Y Open habitat Y Y Y Attributes of the raptor Conspicuous behaviour N N N Y Y Birds in diet Y Y Agile Y Y = yes for supportive; N = no for incongruent; = no directional prediction.

6 472 J. J. NEGRO ET AL. for many taxa. Bold elements of coloration, such as ocelli, are target-like and deflect enemies from more to less vital parts of the body. When watching mobbing birds, it is clear that dives are preferentially directed at the heads of predators (G. R. Bortolotti, J. J. Negro & J. H. Sarasola, pers. observ.). However, at least for owls with no ocelli (e.g. Bubo virginianus) passerines attack the back of the head, which should be less vulnerable than the front (Heinrich, 1987). That false eyes can change the directionality of attacks by mobbers has been demonstrated experimentally by Deppe et al. (2003). Models of pygmy-owls without ocelli were attacked from the rear; however, when eyespots were painted on the back of the head, passerines redirected attacks to the front. These observations suggest that the deflection hypothesis, although having intuitive appeal, is unlikely. HYPOTHESIS 3: STARTLE RESPONSE This hypothesis has not been typically evoked for birds (but see Deppe et al., 2003), but has been proposed for conspicuously patterned wings on moths (Schlenoff, 1985). The sudden display of a contrasting colour pattern in the head may have an effect on attackers by inducing a startle response. This is especially plausible for the many species with ocelli that are largely visible only after ptiloerection, which is the normal response of an individual in a heightened state of alert. Shown to a predator, the ocelli would give prey a chance to escape. Shown to mobbers, they may induce a fear response to prevent close approach or to make attacks less accurate. Against aggressive conspecifics, the sudden display of contrasting colour could give a potential victim time to escape or retaliate. HYPOTHESIS 4: APOSEMATIC SIGNAL It has recently being proposed (Newman, Buesching & Wolff, 2005) that dark facial masks of medium-sized mammalian carnivores are aposematic (i.e. warning of aggressive defensive behaviour that could be harmful to larger predators). By definition, these masks are located frontally. The malar stripes of falcons and the well-defined facial disks of owls are potential visual signals, perhaps serving the same function as the bandit masks of racoons (Procyon lotor) and other carnivores. However, we see no obvious or logical relationship between the presence of ocelli in the nape and a warning to larger predators, mainly because the ocelli would not be visible during a confrontation. When approached by a predator (including humans), a falcon or owl always faces it, bobbing the head with erected feathers and striking with their talons if the attacker gets close enough (G. R. Bortolotti, J. J. Negro & J. H. Sarasola, pers. observ.). THE FALSE FACE AS A PREDATORY AID Unlike the previous category of hypotheses, there is only one potential type of receiver of the signal (i.e. small avian prey). There is ample evidence that raptors are often mobbed by the same species that they typically hunt. The mobbing response is often viewed as being evolutionary justified by the fact that mobbers want to chase away predators that may kill their kin or themselves. The necessary preconditions for these hypotheses to be supported are: (1) the inclusion of mobbing birds in the predator s diet or (2) the predator mimics a species that is subjected to mobbing. HYPOTHESIS 5: MOBBER MANIPULATION Some raptors may exploit what appears to be an innate response of mobbing birds against owls (Hinde, 1954a, b). Using models, Hinde demonstrated that owl shapes with frontal eyes are the most powerful releasers of mobbing responses. Owls are likely feared as nocturnal predators of roosting birds but, on the other hand, may not be agile enough during the day to retaliate. The alarm reactions of small birds are said to be very different toward falcons and hawks than toward owls (i.e. by fleeing, seeking cover and keeping a distance to the former) (Sparks & Soper, 1989). By mimicking an owl, a diurnal bird of prey may attract potential prey within a closer range and facilitate the hunt. Alternative to enticing small birds closer, ocelli may put mobbers in a different position relative to the predator. The experiments by Deppe et al. (2003) described above are highly suggestive of a predatory function to ocelli. If passerines normally attack an owl from behind, but are deceived into attacking it frontally, the raptor is in a much better position to strike. Mobbers regularly pass within a few centimetres, and may actually strike raptors, and so their exact position relative to the predator influences risk dramatically. HYPOTHESIS 6: MOBBER CENSUS This is a novel hypothesis and differs from the previous one in that mobbing is elicited by the predator to gain information about the types and abundance of potential prey in the area. Because the mobbing response is more intense in the vicinity of nest sites and when there are nestlings (Shields, 1984; Heinrich, 1987; G. R. Bortolotti, J. J. Negro & J. H. Sarasola, pers. observ.), the predator may obtain information about the presence of nestlings and fledglings, which are typically easier prey than adults. It may be difficult to test such an hypothesis because the benefit to the raptor may not be immediately obvious; however, it may help explain the settlement and movement patterns of raptors.

7 DECEPTIVE FALSE FACE IN BIRDS 473 AN ANALYSIS OF GLAUCIDIUM The large number of species in this nearly worldwide genus (del Hoyo et al., 1999) allows the investigation of patterns patterns in the detectability of the signal and attributes of the birds that are consistent with the predictions of each hypothesis (Table 1). We searched the literature broadly for all available information on these species, and so our morphological and ecological data were derived from a variety of sources including major reviews (Voous, 1989; del Hoyo et al., 1999; König et al., 1999; Johnsgard, 2002) as well as books on local avifauna worldwide. Unfortunately, little is known of the biology and behaviour of many species. As a result, our quantitative analysis was restricted to body size and the use of general categories for habitat (open, open forest and forest, dense forest), and birds in the diet (high, moderate, low). Because the morphological data for most species were presented as ranges, we use the mid point in the range for wing length, and the low end for mass. The latter would be indicative of males, which are not going to vary as much females (e.g. over the breeding season). For heuristic purposes, we treat species as individual units, recognizing, however, that they are not phylogenetically independent. The taxonomy within this group is currently in flux. Of 32 species, 24 have ocelli/false faces. Species with and without ocelli are distinct in size because there is only nominal overlap (Fig. 2). On average (mean ± SE), species without ocelli weighed (115.5 ± 11.9 g, N = 8) twice as much as species with ocelli (56.3 ± 1.5 g, N = 24). Pygmy-owls can be found in a broad range of habitats and eat a diversity of foods; however, the two habits do not appear to be independent. Eight of ten (80%) species predominantly preying on birds were found in habitats described as open or open forest whereas ten of 14 (71%) species with only a low proportion of birds in the diet were found in forest or dense forest. To describe a species in general terms, we assigned each a three-point score for decreasing openness of habitat and decreasing proportion of birds in diet (see above). The sum score represents a scale from birds in open habitats to dense forest dwellers that do and do not eat birds, respectively. Birds with false faces decrease in frequency along this continuum whereas those without them increase (Fig. 3). It is interesting to note that although some species with ocelli are found in the forested extreme of the scale, the reverse was not true. It should be noted that the heights of the bars in Figure 3 are contingent on phylogeny; however, the general trend for populations of pygmy-owls with false faces to be characterized as open-country predators of birds would remain. The same conclusion can be reached by reviewing species descriptions (del Hoyo et al., 1999). DISCUSSION All hypotheses presented in this review are logical constructs, and have not been deduced from actual field observations of the ocelli in action. There are no available data for any species to demonstrate that the ocelli influence the fitness of their bearers. However, we believe this kind of review, broadening the perspective on what may be plausible, is necessary to bring to light different predictions concerning the potential function of the ocelli, and may help researchers to design experiments and test our hypotheses. The same Body mass (g) Number of species Wing chord (mm) Figure 2. Wing chord length and body mass for Glaucidium pygmy-owls. Solid circles are species without ocelli and open circles have ocelli. Diet/habitat score Figure 3. Distribution of species of Glaucidium pygmyowls along a scale of open habitat species that eat birds (2) to dense forest species that do not eat birds (6).

8 474 J. J. NEGRO ET AL. approach has been used to deal with, what were at the time, puzzling traits such as avian mobbing (Curio, 1978a, b), alarm signals (Klump & Shalter, 1984a, b), the stotting in gazelles (Caro, 1986), and the pelage patterns of carnivores (Ortolani, 1999; Newman et al., 2005). Ocelli may have different functions depending on context or species, and hence some of the hypotheses are not mutually exclusive (Table 1). Support for the antipredator or antimobber category of hypotheses is that the predatory birds displaying false faces are typically small and thus more vulnerable if struck. However, there are several lines of evidence that cast some doubt on this interpretation. The distinction between species with and without ocelli was particularly striking for Glaucidium (Fig. 2). The difference in size, although relatively large, may not be enough in absolute terms to alter risk from predators because most raptors are far larger than both types of owls. It is well known that owls that feed on pygmy-owls also prey on even larger owl species (Forsman et al., 2004). Larger size as a protective value against mobbers is also likely to be of marginal value. For example, using data on body masses of 3352 species from Dunning (1993), the mean pygmy-owl without ocelli would be heavier than 95.0% of passerines. However, even at half that mass, owls with ocelli are still heavier than 84.3% of passerines. We believe that such a large difference in size associated with ocelli is more likely to be explained by foraging ecology than a mere risk of mobbing. Smaller owls may have ocelli because they are faster and more agile, and thus more likely to be effective predators of birds (which could include mobbers). Generally, raptors specializing in killing passerines are small (see size distributions in Falco and Accipiter). The mobber manipulation hypothesis is the only one that links plumage to agility (Table 1). The small size of Glaucidium owls does not deter them from killing birds as large or larger than themselves (Steyn, 1979; Mikkola, 1983; del Hoyo et al., 1999; König et al., 1999; Holt & Petersen, 2000; Proudfoot & Johnson, 2000), and they have been observed to kill mobbers (Finch-Davies & Kemp, 1980; Fry, Keith & Urban, 1988). One difficulty for the protection hypotheses is that ocelli are often large, contrasted and symmetrical spots, which may actually increase detection (Forsman & Merilaita, 1999). Objects that are rounded in shape are more striking and easily seen and recognized (Scaife, 1976a, 1976b). At least to human eyes, they appear to increase conspicuousness (Hafner & Hafner, 1977). We agree with Zahavi & Zahavi (1997: 53) who considered the predator-avoidance hypothesis an insult to the intelligence of predators. Predators may be less likely to be fooled than passerines because there appears to be an asymmetry between raptorial and nonraptorial birds in the degree to which each responds to models. For example, passerines are well known to mob stuffed raptors as well as crude wooden or plastic models (Hinde, 1954a, b; Curio, 1975; Deppe et al., 2003). In contrast, carved decoys have been used for centuries by humans to attract and hunt waterfowl, but these decoys are not generally attacked by birds of prey. Similarly, live bait is virtually a necessity in trapping raptors (G. R. Bortolotti, J. J. Negro & J. H. Sarasola, pers. observ.). Hawks, falcons and owls can be captured with the use of a large owl as a lure when they themselves mob. However, it is well known that live lures are considerably more effective in eliciting a mobbing attack than even taxidermy mounts, and crude models are totally ineffective (Bloom, 1987; Jacobs, 1996; G. R. Bortolotti, J. J. Negro & J. H. Sarasola, pers. observ.). Therefore, we think it is implausible that raptors would be fooled by ocelli of their own, or other species. Although the conspicuousness of the ocelli works against the protection hypotheses, it supports those regarding ocelli as predatory aids. Furthermore, other components of the detectability of ocelli and attributes of the species are supported by predictions of the predatory aid hypotheses (Table 1). Across species, ocelli can be either hidden, or at least reduced, or permanently conspicuous, which is contrary to most of the protection hypotheses and consistent with the predatory aid hypotheses (Table 1). That ocelli can be hidden at will by many species may mean that individuals may decide to limit unwanted mobbing. Predators with false faces tend to eat small birds, hence the preponderance within the Falconidae (note especially among passerine eaters such as subbuteo, columbarius, and fasciinucha) and within Glaucidium (Fig. 3). To aid raptors in foraging, these signals should be visible from a distance, and may be most effective from afar given their crude resemblance to a face. Most falcons, and ocelli-bearing pygmy-owls (Fig. 3), are associated with open habitats. The behaviour of these species should be equally important in broadcasting the signal. Falcons are well known for conspicuous perching. The same is true for many pygmy-owls with false faces. Many species of Glaucidium can be active in a broad range of hours throughout the day, although species with ocelli are noted as being diurnal (Bent, 1961; Steyn, 1979; Voous, 1989; Kullberg, 1995; del Hoyo et al., 1999). There are numerous anecdotes of diurnal hunting pygmy-owls acting in a conspicuous fashion, most commonly the habit of perching in exposed locations such as the tips of trees. When hunting mammals, Eurasian pygmy-owls sit motionless, low in trees but, when they hunt birds, they follow flocks high in the canopy (Kullberg, 1995). When diurnal species are not hunting, they are said to find thick foliage (Fry et al., 1988; Voous, 1989), stay hidden (König et al., 1999), or roost in cavities (König et al., 1999) or against a tree (Fry et al., 1988), as also

9 DECEPTIVE FALSE FACE IN BIRDS 475 described for species without ocelli when roosting. Most studies reviewing the genus make special note of how dense roosting is for the purpose of hiding from mobbers (e.g. Voous, 1989; König et al., 1999), and that pygmy-owls are mobbed if discovered at or disturbed from their roost (Voous, 1989; del Hoyo et al., 1999; König et al., 1999). Although it could be argued that conspicuous, diurnal raptors may need protection more, and so have ocelli, it is not clear then why such a trait should repeatedly appear in predators rather than prey species. Although the mobber manipulation hypothesis has never been proposed for raptors, there are other presumed cases of deception of prey among animals. Micrastur forest falcons are said to vocally provoke a mobbing response in birds as a hunting technique (Smith, 1969). The use of attractants is illustrated by species of fish that lure smaller fish or arthropods into their mouths using appendages resembling flies. Some semiaquatic vipers are said to use the tips of their tails to deceive frogs into believing they are small worms (Wharton, 1960). Mobber attraction has been suggested as an explanation for the accipiter-like plumage colour and morphology of hawk cuckoos (Cuculus spp.); by being mobbed, they may census potential hosts for their parasitic eggs (del Hoyo, Elliot & Sargatal, 1997). McLean & Rhodes (1991) recognized the similarity between social parasites and predators in potentially provoking mobbing as a means of attracting or obtaining information about their victims. The species showing ocelli typically display them at will, perch conspicuously, are diurnal, of small size, agile, and have a high proportion birds in their diet. This description fits well all predictions of the predatory aid hypotheses, but infrequently so or contrary to the predictions of the protection hypotheses (Table 1). However, because the available evidence is not yet conclusive, field experiments using models of raptors with and without ocelli are needed to distinguish among the hypotheses (Deppe et al., 2003). These models should be presented in a systematic way to mobbers, potential predators, and conspecifics. Also sorely needed is more information on how raptors hunt avian prey. Although diet composition is typically well known from the analysis of pellets and prey remains, actual observations of hunting episodes are scarce or go unreported. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The first two authors contributed equally to this paper. We thank Arantxa, of Grupo para la Recuperación de la Fauna Autóctona y su Hábitat (Grefa), for lending us a live hobby, and Lauren Bortolotti for comments on the manuscript. We are also indebted to the following museum curators for allowing access to specimens: Josefina Barreiro (Museo Nacional de Ciencias Naturales, Madrid), José Cabot (Estación Biológica de Doñana, Sevilla), Andrew Kitchener (Royal Museum of Scotland, Edinburgh), Leslie Jessop (Hancock Museum, Newcastle Upon Tyne), Allan J. Baker (Royal Ontario Museum, Toronto). G.R.B. was funded by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada, J.H.S. was supported by a scholarship from Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Cient ficas y Técnicas de Argentina (CONICET), and J.J.N. s travel was assisted by F. Hiraldo. REFERENCES Altmann SA Avian mobbing and predator recognition. Condor 58: Andersson MB Sexual selection. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Balgooyen TG Another possible function of the American kestrel s deflection face. Jack-Pine Warbler 53: Bent AC Life histories of North American birds of prey. New York, NY: Dover Publications. Bildstein KL Responses of northern harriers to mobbing passerines. Journal of Field Ornithology 53: Bloom P Capturing and handling raptors. In: Pendleton BAG, Millsap BA, Cline KW, Bird DM, eds. Raptor management techniques manual. Washington DC: National Wildlife Federation Burtt EH Jr The adaptiveness of animal colors. Bioscience 31: Cade TJ The falcons of the world. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press. Caro TM The functions of stotting in Thomson s gazelles: some tests of the predictions. Animal Behaviour 34: Clay WM Protective coloration in the American sparrow hawk. Wilson Bulletin 65: Cott HB Adaptive coloration in animals. London: Oxford University Press. Cramp S, Simmons KEL The birds of western Paleartic. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Curio E The functional organisation of anti-predator behaviour in the pied flycatcher: a study of avian visual perception. Animal Behaviour 23: Curio E. 1978a. The adaptive significance of avian mobbing. I. Teleonomic hypotheses and predictions. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 47: Curio E. 1978b. The adaptive significance of avian mobbing. II. Cultural transmission of enemy recognition in blackbirds: effectiveness and some constrains. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 47: Curio E, Regelmann K The behavioural dynamics of great tits (Parus major) approaching a predator. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 69: Deppe C, Holt D, Tewksbury J, Broberg L, Petersen J, Wood K Effect of northern pygmy-owl (Glaucidium gnoma) eyespots on avian mobbing. Auk 120: Dunning JB CRC handbook of avian body masses. Bota Racon, FL: CRC Press.

10 476 J. J. NEGRO ET AL. Finch-Davies C, Kemp A The birds of prey of southern Africa. Johannesburg: Winchester Press. Flasskamp A The adaptive significance of avian mobbing. V. An experimental test of the move on hypothesis. Ethology 96: Forsman D The raptors of Europe and the Middle East. London: T & AD Poyser. Forsman ED, Anthony RG, Meslow EC, Zabel CJ Diets and foraging behaviour of northern spotted owls in Oregon. Journal of Raptor Research 38: Forsman A, Merilaita S Fearful symmetry: pattern size and asymmetry affects aposematic signal efficacy. Evolutionary Ecology 13: Fry CH, Keith S, Urban EK The birds of Africa, Vol. 3. London: Academic Press. Hafner JC, Hafner MS The cryptic occipital spot in the Accipitridae (Falconiformes). Auk 94: Heinrich B One man s owl. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Hinde RA. 1954a. Factors governing the changes in strength of a partially inborn response, as shown by the mobbing behaviour of the chaffinch (Fringilla coelebs). I. The nature of the response, and an examination of its course. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological Sciences 142: Hinde RA. 1954b. Factors governing the changes in strength of a partially inborn response, as shown by the mobbing behaviour of the chaffinch (Fringilla coelebs). II. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological Sciences 142: Holt DW, Petersen JL Northern pygmy-owl. In: Poole A, Stettenheim P, Gill F, eds. The birds of North America. Philadelphia, PA: Academy of Natural Sciences. del Hoyo J, Elliot A, Sargatal J Handbook of the birds of the world, vol. II: New World vultures to guineafowls Barcelona: Lynx editions. del Hoyo J, Elliot A, Sargatal J Handbook of the birds of the world, vol. IV: sandgrouse to cuckoos. Barcelona: Lynx editions. del Hoyo J, Elliot A, Sargatal J Handbook of the birds of the world, vol. V: barn-owls to hummingbirds. Barcelona: Lynx editions. Jacobs EA A mechanical owl as a trapping lure for raptors. Journal of Raptor Research 30: Jenkins AR, Wagner ST, Hoffman I First breeding record of the taita falcon in South Africa. Ostrich 62: 78. Johnsgard PA North American owls: biology and natural history, 2nd. edn. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution Press. Klump GM, Shalter MD. 1984a. Acoustic behaviour of birds and mammals in the predator context. I. Factors affecting the structure of alarm signals. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 66: Klump GM, Shalter MD. 1984b. Acoustic behaviour of birds and mammals in the predator context. II. The functional significance and evolution of alarm signals. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 66: Koehler A A kestrel to the sky. Journal of Raptor Research 26: König C, Weick F, Becking J Owls: a guide to the owls of the world. Tonbridge, UK: Pica Press. Kullberg C Strategy of the pygmy owl while hunting avian and mammalian prey. Ornis Fennica 72: Maynard Smith J, Harper D Animal signals. Oxford: Oxford University Press. McLean IG, Rhodes G Enemy recognition and response in birds. Current Ornithology 8: Mikkola H Owls of Europe. London: T & AD Poyser. Mueller HC Displays and vocalizations of the sparrow hawk. Wilson Bulletin 83: Negro JJ, Grande JM, Sarasola JH Do Eurasian hobbies (Falco subuteo) have false eyes on the nape? Journal of Raptor Research 38: Newman C, Buesching D, Wolff JO The function of facial masks in midguild carnivores. Oikos 108: Ortolani A Spots, stripes, tail tips and dark eyes: predicting the function of carnivore colour patterns using the comparative method. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 67: Ostreiher Royal Is mobbing altruistic or selfish behaviour? Animal Behaviour 66: Pavey CR, Smyth AK Effects of avian mobbing on roost use and diet of powerful owls, Ninox strenua. Animal Behaviour 55: Petrie M, Halliday T, Sanders C Peahens prefer peacocks with elaborate trains. Animal Behaviour 41: Poiani A, Yorke M Predator harassment: more evidence on the deadly risk. Ethology 83: Proudfoot GA, Johnson RR Ferruginous pygmy-owl. In: Poole A, Stettenheim P, Gill F, eds. The birds of North America. Philadelphia, PA: Academy of Natural Sciences. Savalli UM The evolution of bird coloration and plumage elaboration. A review of hypotheses. Current Ornithology 12: Scaife M. 1976a. The response to eye-like shapes by birds. I. The effect of context: a predator and a strange bird. Animal Behaviour 24: Scaife M. 1976b. The response to eye-like shapes by birds. II. The importance of staring, pairedness and shape. Animal Behaviour 24: Schlenoff DH The startle response of blue jays to Catocala (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae) prey models. Animal Behaviour 33: Shields WM Barn swallow mobbing: self-defence, collateral kin defence, group defence, or parental care? Animal Behaviour 32: Smith NG Provoked release of mobbing a hunting technique of Micrastur falcons. Ibis 111: Sordahl TA The risks of avian mobbing and distraction behavior: an anecdotal review. Wilson Bulletin 102: Sparks J, Soper T Owls: their natural and unnatural history. New York, NY: Facts on File. Steyn P Observations on pearl-spotted and barred owls. Bokmakierie 31: Svensson L, Grant PJ, Mullarney K, Zetterström D Collins bird guide. London: Harper Collins Publishers. Voous KH Owls of the northern hemisphere. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

11 DECEPTIVE FALSE FACE IN BIRDS 477 Walker DG Golden eagle killing mobbing carrion crows. British Birds 76: 312. Wharton CH Birth and behavior of a brood of cottonmouths, Agkistrodon piscivorus piscivorus, with notes on tail-luring. Herpetologica 16: Wheeler BK Raptors of eastern north America. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Wink M, Sauer-Gürth H Phylogenetic relationships in diurnal raptors based on nucleotide sequences of mitochondrial and nuclear marker genes. In: Chancelor RD, Meyburg BU, eds. Raptors worldwide. Berlin: World Working Group on Birds of Prey and Owls Zahavi A, Zahavi A The handicap principle. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Falco rufigularis (Bat Falcon)

Falco rufigularis (Bat Falcon) Falco rufigularis (Bat Falcon) Family: Falconidae (Falcons) Order: Falconiformes (Diurnal Birds of Prey) Class: Aves (Birds) Fig. 1. Bat falcon, Falco rufigularis. [http://www.inaturalist.org/observations/769241,

More information

GENERAL PROTOCOL CONTENTS

GENERAL PROTOCOL CONTENTS GENERAL PROTOCOL CONTENTS GENERAL PROTOCOL...3.2.2 Summary of protocols...3.2.2 Survey recommendations and tips...3.2.3 Forest bird recordings...3.2.5 Cowbirds and nest predators...3.2.6 Nests...3.2.6

More information

RAPTOR STUDY. raptor charts and posters

RAPTOR STUDY. raptor charts and posters TYPE: MOSTLY INDOOR GRADE: 3-7 TIME: 1½ - 2 Hours RAPTOR STUDY OBJECTIVE Students will be able to: 1) describe characteristics common to raptors; 2) describe some special adaptations of raptors; 3) name

More information

Columbina talpacoti (Ruddy Ground Dove)

Columbina talpacoti (Ruddy Ground Dove) Columbina talpacoti (Ruddy Ground Dove) Family: Columbidae (Pigeons and Doves) Order: Columbiformes (Pigeons, Doves and Dodos) Class: Aves (Birds) Fig. 1. Ruddy ground dove, Columbina talpacoti. [http://www.azfo.net/gallery/2009/html2/rgdo_patons_baxter_20090619.html,

More information

Behavioral Adaptations for Survival 1. Co-evolution of predator and prey ( evolutionary arms races )

Behavioral Adaptations for Survival 1. Co-evolution of predator and prey ( evolutionary arms races ) Behavioral Adaptations for Survival 1 Co-evolution of predator and prey ( evolutionary arms races ) Outline Mobbing Behavior What is an adaptation? The Comparative Method Divergent and convergent evolution

More information

2010 Ornithology (B/C) - Training Handout

2010 Ornithology (B/C) - Training Handout This event will test knowledge of birds. 2010 Ornithology (B/C) - Training Handout KAREN LANCOUR National Bio Rules Committee Chairman karenlancour@charter.net The Official National List will be used for

More information

Raptors at a Glance. Small birds, some mammals

Raptors at a Glance. Small birds, some mammals Accipiters Common Name Sharp-shinned Hawk Cooper's Hawk Northern Goshawk Scientific Name Accipiter striatus Accipiter cooperii Accipiter gentilis Woodlands Woods, adapts well to urban areas; MF build Woodlands,

More information

REPORT OF AN OBSERVATION OF BARBARY FALCON Falco peregrinus pelegrinoides IN AGDZ (MOROCCO) ON DECEMBER 2010

REPORT OF AN OBSERVATION OF BARBARY FALCON Falco peregrinus pelegrinoides IN AGDZ (MOROCCO) ON DECEMBER 2010 REPORT OF AN OBSERVATION OF BARBARY FALCON Falco peregrinus pelegrinoides IN AGDZ (MOROCCO) ON DECEMBER 2010 Beneharo Rodríguez 1 and Nazaret Carrasco 1 C/La Malecita S/N, Buenavista del Norte 38480, S/C

More information

NOTES Partial albino African Marsh Harrier at Langebaan, South Africa

NOTES Partial albino African Marsh Harrier at Langebaan, South Africa NOTES Partial albino African Marsh Harrier at Langebaan, South Africa Robert E. Simmons 1, Trevor Hardaker 2 & William S. Clark 3 * 1 FitzPatrick Institute, University of Cape Town, Rondebosch 7701, RSA;

More information

Lasiurus blossevillii (Red Bat)

Lasiurus blossevillii (Red Bat) Lasiurus blossevillii (Red Bat) Family: Vespertilionidae (Vesper or Evening Bats) Order: Chiroptera (Bats) Class: Mammalia (Mammals) Fig. 1. Red bat, Lasiurus blossevillii. [http://www.inaturalist.org/taxa/40520-lasiurus-blossevillii,

More information

Lecture 6 Behavior ethology Communication

Lecture 6 Behavior ethology Communication Lecture 6 Behavior ethology Communication Rich history of behavioral studies of birds Birds display a wide range of innate and learned behavior patterns From Science 325: 1323, 2009 Overview - Behavior

More information

Recent work on the development of feeding skills by young birds has

Recent work on the development of feeding skills by young birds has Interactions between House Sparrows and Sparrowhawks C.J. Barnard How does an efficient Sparrowhawk catch a sparrow? How does a wily House Sparrow avoid being eaten? Recent work on the development of feeding

More information

Owl: A Year in the Lives of North American Owls Evergreen Audubon

Owl: A Year in the Lives of North American Owls Evergreen Audubon evergreenaudubon.org Owl: A Year in the Lives of North American Owls Evergreen Audubon 6-8 minutes I attended Paul Bannick s talk about owls at the February 2017 meeting of the Denver Field Ornithologists.

More information

The 'raptor-flight' of the Cuckoo. By J. S. Ash

The 'raptor-flight' of the Cuckoo. By J. S. Ash British Birds Vol. 58 No. 1 JANUARY 1965 The 'raptor-flight' of the Cuckoo By J. S. Ash ON FOUR OCCASIONS in the past eleven years I have noticed Cuckoos Cuculus canorus flying in a rather unusual manner,

More information

Red-winged blackbird calls sound like loud check and a high slurred tee-err sound when alarmed. Their song is a liquid gurgling konk-ke-ree...

Red-winged blackbird calls sound like loud check and a high slurred tee-err sound when alarmed. Their song is a liquid gurgling konk-ke-ree... Introduction This bird nests and breeds in wetlands across North America is one of the first signs of spring in Canada is named for the male s bright red shoulders called epaulettes defends its territory

More information

What is a Bird of Prey?

What is a Bird of Prey? 2 Topic What is a Bird of Prey? beak talons Birds of prey are predators. Like all predators, they hunt and kill other animals for food. Birds of prey have specific adaptations to help them hunt, capture,

More information

Nocturnally Singing Northern Mockingbirds Orient Toward Lights

Nocturnally Singing Northern Mockingbirds Orient Toward Lights Nocturnally Singing Northern Mockingbirds Orient Toward Lights Kate A. Miskell Department of Psychology East Carolina University Greenville, NC 27858 Michael J. Justice Department of Psychology East Carolina

More information

Abstract The American Redstart is a wood warbler that is in population decline in northern Michigan.

Abstract The American Redstart is a wood warbler that is in population decline in northern Michigan. Abstract The American Redstart is a wood warbler that is in population decline in northern Michigan. This study investigates the effect understory vegetation density has on the distribution of American

More information

2011 Ornithology (B/C) - Training Handout

2011 Ornithology (B/C) - Training Handout 2011 Ornithology (B/C) - Training Handout KAREN L. LANCOUR National Bio Rules Committee Chairman Life Science DISCLAIMER - This presentation was prepared using draft rules. There may be some changes in

More information

Female in nestbox. Common Name: SOUTHEASTEN AMERICAN KESTREL. Scientific Name: Falco sparverius paulus Linnaeus

Female in nestbox. Common Name: SOUTHEASTEN AMERICAN KESTREL. Scientific Name: Falco sparverius paulus Linnaeus Female in nestbox Common Name: SOUTHEASTEN AMERICAN KESTREL Scientific Name: Falco sparverius paulus Linnaeus Other Commonly Used Names: Sparrow Hawk Previously Used Names: None Family: Falconidae Rarity

More information

Phaetusa simplex (Large-billed Tern)

Phaetusa simplex (Large-billed Tern) Phaetusa simplex (Large-billed Tern) Family: Laridae (Gulls and Terns) Order: Charadriiformes (Shorebirds and Waders) Class: Aves (Birds) Fig. 1. Large-billed tern, Phaetusa simplex. [http://www.discoverlife.org/mp/20p?see=i_lht6688&res=640,

More information

NEST BOX TRAIL HISTORY

NEST BOX TRAIL HISTORY NEST BOX TRAIL HISTORY 1985-2016 by KEITH EVANS and JACK RENSEL INTRODUCTION In August of 1984, members of the Wasatch Audubon Society (Ogden, Utah) held a workshop to construct bluebird nesting boxes.

More information

Mobbing of South Polar Skua by Cory's Shearwaters the North Carolina Coast

Mobbing of South Polar Skua by Cory's Shearwaters the North Carolina Coast The Chat, Vol. 62, No.1, Winter 1998 33 Mobbing of South Polar Skua by Cory's Shearwaters the North Carolina Coast off Edward S. Brinkley 108 Cocke Hall University of Virginia Charlottesville, VA 22903

More information

British Birds of Prey. British Birds of Prey Published on LoveTheGarden.com (https://www.lovethegarden.com)

British Birds of Prey. British Birds of Prey Published on LoveTheGarden.com (https://www.lovethegarden.com) Have you spotted a bird of prey near where you live recently? And if you have, were you able to tell which bird it was? The Big Garden Birdwatch is coming up so we should get to know our types of birds

More information

SIERRA NEVADA ADAPTIVE MANAGEMENT PLAN

SIERRA NEVADA ADAPTIVE MANAGEMENT PLAN SIERRA NEVADA ADAPTIVE MANAGEMENT PLAN Study Plan and Inventory Protocol For the California Spotted Owl Study Tahoe NF Study Site Douglas J. Tempel, Project Supervisor Professor Ralph J. Gutiérrez, P.I.

More information

Owls & Turkeys. Literacy Centers For 2 nd & 3 rd Grades. FREE from The Curriculum Corner

Owls & Turkeys. Literacy Centers For 2 nd & 3 rd Grades. FREE from The Curriculum Corner Owls & Turkeys Literacy Centers For 2 nd & 3 rd Grades FREE from The Curriculum Corner Wild Turkey Barn Owl facts opinions Barn owls are nocturnal. Barn owls are the most interesting species of owls. Barn

More information

PSY 2364 Animal Communication

PSY 2364 Animal Communication PSY 2364 Animal Communication Jumping Spider (Phidippus putnami) http://www.utdallas.edu/~assmann/polc/polc_092014.html Peacock Spider (Maratus speciosus) Peacock Spider (Maratus mungaich) https://www.youtube.com/user/peacockspiderman/videos

More information

Notes on a Breeding Population of Red-headed Woodpeckers in New York State. Jacob L. Berl and John W. Edwards

Notes on a Breeding Population of Red-headed Woodpeckers in New York State. Jacob L. Berl and John W. Edwards Notes on a Breeding Population of Red-headed Woodpeckers in New York State Jacob L. Berl and John W. Edwards Division of Forestry and Natural Resources, West Virginia University Morgantown, WV 26505 The

More information

Birding at a Slower Pace - An Holistic Approach to Observing Birds: Why atlasing will improve your field skills AND your birding experience

Birding at a Slower Pace - An Holistic Approach to Observing Birds: Why atlasing will improve your field skills AND your birding experience Birding at a Slower Pace - An Holistic Approach to Observing Birds: Why atlasing will improve your field skills AND your birding experience Bill Mueller, Director, Western Great Lakes Bird and Bat Observatory

More information

ANIMAL COMMUNICATION and INFORMATION TRANSFER

ANIMAL COMMUNICATION and INFORMATION TRANSFER ANIMAL COMMUNICATION and INFORMATION TRANSFER COMMUNICATION some definitions. - Transfer of information from a signaler to a receiver (textbooks) - Actors use specifically designed signals or displays

More information

Listed Birds along the Stony Brook Corridor Impacted by BMS Zoning Change

Listed Birds along the Stony Brook Corridor Impacted by BMS Zoning Change Listed Birds along the Stony Brook Corridor Impacted by BMS Zoning Change Washington Crossing Audubon Society (WCAS) opposes the zoning change to allow high density housing on the Bristol-Meyers Squibb

More information

Golden Eagle (Aquila chrysaetos)

Golden Eagle (Aquila chrysaetos) Golden Eagle (Aquila chrysaetos) NMPIF level: Biodiversity Conservation Concern, Level 2 (BC2) NMPIF assessment score: 12 NM stewardship responsibility: Low National PIF status: No special status New Mexico

More information

Bat Species of the Years 2016 and Noctule (Nyctalus noctula)

Bat Species of the Years 2016 and Noctule (Nyctalus noctula) Bat Species of the Years 2016 and 2017 Noctule (Nyctalus noctula) Facts compiled for BatLife Europe by Eeva-Maria Kyheröinen, Javier Juste, Kit Stoner and Guido Reiter Biology and distribution The Noctule

More information

Review of the Illegal Killing, Trapping and Trade (IKB) of Birds of Prey in the Mediterranean

Review of the Illegal Killing, Trapping and Trade (IKB) of Birds of Prey in the Mediterranean Review of the Illegal Killing, Trapping and Trade (IKB) of Birds of Prey in the Mediterranean Nick P Williams, MSc (Ecology) Head of the Coordinating Unit, CMS Raptors MoU CMS Raptors MoU Multilateral

More information

How Are an Owl's Adaptations Different From Other Birds'?

How Are an Owl's Adaptations Different From Other Birds'? How Are an Owl's Adaptations Different From Other Birds'? We have been learning about owls habits, hearing, use of talons, life cycles, nesting habits, prey choices and camouflage coloring. Now, we will

More information

Arizona s Raptor Experience, LLC October 2018 ~Newsletter~ Greetings from Chino Valley! Jay s Bird Barn!

Arizona s Raptor Experience, LLC October 2018 ~Newsletter~ Greetings from Chino Valley! Jay s Bird Barn! Arizona s Raptor Experience, LLC October 2018 ~Newsletter~ Greetings from Chino Valley! We hope you are well and enjoying the beautiful fall weather. Remember at this time of year to keep your eyes to

More information

Nikon. Nikon Photo Quiz. Sponsored by Nikon Canada ONTARIO BIRDS DECEMBER 2005

Nikon. Nikon   Photo Quiz. Sponsored by Nikon Canada ONTARIO BIRDS DECEMBER 2005 154 Nikon Photo Quiz Sponsored by Nikon Canada Nikon www.nikon.ca 155 December 2005 Quiz Glenn Coady You are walking along a local beach when you notice a bird taking flight directly away from you. Even

More information

Understanding avian collisions: a birds eye view

Understanding avian collisions: a birds eye view Understanding avian collisions: a birds eye view Graham Martin University of Birmingham UK Graham Martin Centre for Ornithology School of Biosciences Why are these a problem to birds? In the majority of

More information

Cooper's & Sharp-shinned Hawk: Study skins + ID Notes

Cooper's & Sharp-shinned Hawk: Study skins + ID Notes Cooper's & Sharp-shinned Hawk: Study skins + ID Notes On 23 January 2014 I spent a few hours at the Canadian Museum of Nature Collections Facility in Gatineau, QC. The focus of the visit was Accipter ID;

More information

Instructor Guide: Birds in Human Landscapes

Instructor Guide: Birds in Human Landscapes Instructor Guide: Birds in Human Landscapes Authors: Yula Kapetanakos, Benjamin Zuckerberg Level: University undergraduate Adaptable for online- only or distance learning Purpose To investigate the interplay

More information

Bird Watching Basics. Size & Shape. Color Pa7ern. Behavior. Habitat. These characteris>cs will help you iden>fy birds.

Bird Watching Basics. Size & Shape. Color Pa7ern. Behavior. Habitat. These characteris>cs will help you iden>fy birds. Bird Watching Bird Watching Basics These characteris>cs will help you iden>fy birds. Size & Shape Color Pa7ern Behavior Habitat Great Blue Heron Size & Shape Determine the rela>ve size of the bird compared

More information

February 2014 Volume 4 Issue #1

February 2014 Volume 4 Issue #1 February 2014 Volume 4 Issue #1 Mike the Eagle Recently, you may have heard of the West Nile Virus that is affecting Utah s bald eagles. Here at Great Basin we have cared for many of these sick birds.

More information

Peregrine Falcon Falco peregrinus

Peregrine Falcon Falco peregrinus Plant Composition and Density Mosaic Distance to Water Prey Populations Cliff Properties Minimum Patch Size Recommended Patch Size Home Range Photo by Christy Klinger Habitat Use Profile Habitats Used

More information

REINTRODUCTION TECHNIQUES FOR REHABILITATED AND CAPTIVE BRED BIRDS OF PREY

REINTRODUCTION TECHNIQUES FOR REHABILITATED AND CAPTIVE BRED BIRDS OF PREY / REINTRODUCTION TECHNIQUES FOR REHABILITATED AND CAPTIVE BRED BIRDS OF PREY Walter C. Crawford. Jr. Executive Director Raptor Rehabilitation and Propagation Project Inc. Tyson Research Center P. O. Box

More information

Greenlaw Mountain Hawk Watch Fall 2014

Greenlaw Mountain Hawk Watch Fall 2014 Greenlaw Mountain Hawk Watch Fall 2014 Another season has come to an end. Much was learned, volunteer participation remained strong and several rarities were recorded including two new raptor species.

More information

Belize: In a Lagoon. by Gregory and Jacalyn Willis Copyright 2012

Belize: In a Lagoon. by Gregory and Jacalyn Willis Copyright 2012 Belize: In a Lagoon by Gregory and Jacalyn Willis Copyright 2012 Belize is a small country in Central America, next to Guatemala and Mexico. We go to Belize because it has high populations of the native

More information

PDX Wildlife Program. January 15 th, Nick Atwell Aviation Wildlife Manager

PDX Wildlife Program. January 15 th, Nick Atwell Aviation Wildlife Manager PDX Wildlife Program January 15 th, 2014 Nick Atwell Aviation Wildlife Manager 1 Overview Program Background Eco-regional Context Four Pillars of the Wildlife Hazard Management Program Short-term Research

More information

PROPERTY ASSESSMENT OWL HABITAT AND USE PROPOSED DEVELOPMENT PROPERTY ECHO HEIGHTS, CHEMAINUS, BC

PROPERTY ASSESSMENT OWL HABITAT AND USE PROPOSED DEVELOPMENT PROPERTY ECHO HEIGHTS, CHEMAINUS, BC PROPERTY ASSESSMENT OWL HABITAT AND USE PROPOSED DEVELOPMENT PROPERTY ECHO HEIGHTS, CHEMAINUS, BC for: The Corporation of the District of North Cowichan Box 278, 7030 Trans Canada Highway Duncan, BC, V9L

More information

Wild Bird Lessons for John E. Conner Museum

Wild Bird Lessons for John E. Conner Museum Wild Bird Lessons for John E. Conner Museum These lessons were created by graduate students in the Spring 2015 WSCI 6390 Wildlife Teaching Methods class at Texas A&M University-Kingsville as part of their

More information

Birds and their Adaptations Student Activity Book Answer Key

Birds and their Adaptations Student Activity Book Answer Key Answer Key Page 3 -Activity #1 A Bird s Life 1. Bird s Life Cycle. Correct sequence for the bird and toad life cycles are shown here. Because these are cycles the top picture does not have to be the egg

More information

Industry perspective: Monitoring non-target effects of anticoagulants in the UK - impacts and outcomes

Industry perspective: Monitoring non-target effects of anticoagulants in the UK - impacts and outcomes Vertebrate Pests Unit, School of Biological Sciences Industry perspective: Monitoring non-target effects of anticoagulants in the UK - impacts and outcomes Alan Buckle 2nd Fresenius Conference "Environmental

More information

Birds are the most vivid expression of life Roger Tory Peterson

Birds are the most vivid expression of life Roger Tory Peterson Birds are the most vivid expression of life Roger Tory Peterson Conspicuous airborne marvels... Food and sport... Birding! 1 The Arts Fashion and function... from: to: Religious and cultural symbols Birds

More information

U.S. Fish & Wildlife Service. My project. IPaC Trust Resource Report. Generated May 07, :40 AM MDT

U.S. Fish & Wildlife Service. My project. IPaC Trust Resource Report. Generated May 07, :40 AM MDT U.S. Fish & Wildlife Service My project Generated May 07, 2015 10:40 AM MDT US Fish & Wildlife Service Project Description NAME My project PROJECT CODE LOCATION Prince William County, Virginia No description

More information

APPENDIX A ANNUAL COMPLIANCE REPORT CONDITION REQUIREMENTS CONDITION 4.0

APPENDIX A ANNUAL COMPLIANCE REPORT CONDITION REQUIREMENTS CONDITION 4.0 APPENDIX A ANNUAL COMPLIANCE REPORT CONDITION REQUIREMENTS CONDITION 4.0 Condition 4: Migratory Birds 4.1.1 The Proponent shall carry out all phases of the Designated Project in a manner that avoids harming

More information

Pre-Visit Lesson Neotropical Migratory Birds Identifying Birds

Pre-Visit Lesson Neotropical Migratory Birds Identifying Birds Pre-Visit Lesson Neotropical Migratory Birds Identifying Birds Grade Level: 5-7 Summary: Students will identify birds using field marks and coloration. Teaching Methods: Analysis, Classification, Observation

More information

Hawks And Owls Of The Great Lakes Region And Eastern North America By Chris Earley READ ONLINE

Hawks And Owls Of The Great Lakes Region And Eastern North America By Chris Earley READ ONLINE Hawks And Owls Of The Great Lakes Region And Eastern North America By Chris Earley READ ONLINE Hawks & Owls of Eastern North America by Chris Earley, 9781554079995, Sparrows and Finches of the Great Lakes

More information

Turdus nudigenis (Spectacled Thrush or Big-eye Grieve)

Turdus nudigenis (Spectacled Thrush or Big-eye Grieve) Turdus nudigenis (Spectacled Thrush or Big-eye Grieve) Family: Turdidae (Thrushes) Order: Passeriformes (Perching Birds) Class: Aves (Birds) Fig. 1. Spectacled thrush, Turdus nudigenis. [http://www.hbw.com/species,

More information

Tyrannus melancholicus (Tropical Kingbird)

Tyrannus melancholicus (Tropical Kingbird) Tyrannus melancholicus (Tropical Kingbird) Family: Tyrannidae (Tyrant Flycatchers) Order: Passeriformes (Perching birds) Class: Aves (Birds) Fig. 1. Tropical Kingbird, Tyrannus melancholicus. [http://ibc.lynxeds.com/photo/tropical-kingbird-tyrannus-melancholicus/suiriri,

More information

Biodiversity Observations

Biodiversity Observations Biodiversity Observations http://bo.adu.org.za An electronic journal published by the Animal Demography Unit at the University of Cape Town The scope of Biodiversity Observations consists of papers describing

More information

WindWise Education. 2 nd. T ransforming the Energy of Wind into Powerful Minds. editi. A Curriculum for Grades 6 12

WindWise Education. 2 nd. T ransforming the Energy of Wind into Powerful Minds. editi. A Curriculum for Grades 6 12 WindWise Education T ransforming the Energy of Wind into Powerful Minds A Curriculum for Grades 6 12 Notice Except for educational use by an individual teacher in a classroom setting this work may not

More information

Thunder Bay's N.esting Merlins

Thunder Bay's N.esting Merlins 97 Perkins, IP. 1964-65. 17 flyways RyjJ. A. 1987. Smith's Longspur: a over the Great Lakes (in two case of neglect. Ontario Birds, in parts). Audubon 66: 294-299 and press. 67: 42-45. Wormington, A. 1986.

More information

A Practical Challenge for Atlasers: Identifying Juvenile Birds

A Practical Challenge for Atlasers: Identifying Juvenile Birds A Practical Challenge for Atlasers: Identifying Juvenile Birds Ageing sequence for birds: After hatching, a young bird s first plumage is called natal down. Prejuvenal (or postnatal) molt Juvenile (or

More information

Survey Protocol for the Yellow-billed Cuckoo Western Distinct Population Segment

Survey Protocol for the Yellow-billed Cuckoo Western Distinct Population Segment Survey Protocol for the Yellow-billed Cuckoo Western Distinct Population Segment Halterman, MD, MJ Johnson, JA Holmes, and SA Laymon. 2016. A Natural History Summary and Survey Protocol for the Western

More information

EEB 4260 Ornithology. Lecture Notes: Migration

EEB 4260 Ornithology. Lecture Notes: Migration EEB 4260 Ornithology Lecture Notes: Migration Class Business Reading for this lecture Required. Gill: Chapter 10 (pgs. 273-295) Optional. Proctor and Lynch: pages 266-273 1. Introduction A) EARLY IDEAS

More information

Learning about Forests (LEAF) Ireland. What Habitat do I Live in?

Learning about Forests (LEAF) Ireland. What Habitat do I Live in? Learning about Forests (LEAF) Ireland What Habitat do I Live in? Activity Instructions: Print each individual slide (Think Green! laminate if you wish to reuse them) 12 Species cards & 12 Habitat cards

More information

Birdify Your Yard: Habitat Landscaping for Birds. Melissa Pitkin Klamath Bird Observatory

Birdify Your Yard: Habitat Landscaping for Birds. Melissa Pitkin Klamath Bird Observatory Birdify Your Yard: Habitat Landscaping for Birds Melissa Pitkin Klamath Bird Observatory KBO Mission KBO uses science to promote conservation in the Klamath- Siskiyou region and beyond, working in partnership

More information

IDENTIFICATION CHALLENGE: LESSER VS. GREATER SCAUP BY PIERRE DEVICHE, PHOENIX, AZ 85048,

IDENTIFICATION CHALLENGE: LESSER VS. GREATER SCAUP BY PIERRE DEVICHE, PHOENIX, AZ 85048, IDENTIFICATION CHALLENGE: LESSER VS. GREATER SCAUP BY PIERRE DEVICHE, PHOENIX, AZ 85048, DEVICHE@ASU.EDU Winter in Arizona is the season to search for and study the many northern-breeding ducks that overwinter

More information

Florida Field Naturalist

Florida Field Naturalist Florida Field Naturalist Published by the Florida Ornithological Society Vol. 46, No. 3 September 2018 pages 65-91 Florida Field Naturalist 46(3):65-69, 2018. Murder Mystery at the Nest of a Brownheaded

More information

Appendix A Little Brown Myotis Species Account

Appendix A Little Brown Myotis Species Account Appendix 5.4.14A Little Brown Myotis Species Account Section 5 Project Name: Scientific Name: Species Code: Status: Blackwater Myotis lucifugus M_MYLU Yellow-listed species by the British Columbia Conservation

More information

Red-footed Falcon (Falco vespertinus) in Israel. Prof. Reuven Yosef International Birding & Research Centre in Eilat, Israel Photographs: Eyal Bartov

Red-footed Falcon (Falco vespertinus) in Israel. Prof. Reuven Yosef International Birding & Research Centre in Eilat, Israel Photographs: Eyal Bartov Red-footed Falcon (Falco vespertinus) in Israel Prof. Reuven Yosef International Birding & Research Centre in Eilat, Israel Photographs: Eyal Bartov Advanced Very High Resolution Radiometer (AVHRR) +

More information

Side & Subdued Lighting

Side & Subdued Lighting 1 Module # 2 Component # 3 Side Lighting There is not a lot to be said in this section - side-lighting is "half-way" between front and back lighting, so it combines characteristics of both. This lighting

More information

Emily Gillmore. Intern at the Beaverhill Bird Observatory

Emily Gillmore. Intern at the Beaverhill Bird Observatory Habitat use and spatial patterns of Myotis and large-bodied bat species assessed by the narrow-band acoustic method at the Beaverhill Bird Observatory, Final Report Emily Gillmore Intern at the Beaverhill

More information

Florida Field Naturalist

Florida Field Naturalist Florida Field Naturalist PUBLISHED BY THE FLORIDA ORNITHOLOGICAL SOCIETY VOL. 26, NO. 3 AUGUST 1998 PAGES 77-108 Florida Field Nat. 26(2):77-83, 1998. THE PROPORTION OF SNAIL KITES ATTEMPTING TO BREED

More information

Birds of Prey. Birds of Prey A Reading A Z Level P Benchmark Book Word Count: 685 BENCHMARK P.

Birds of Prey. Birds of Prey A Reading A Z Level P Benchmark Book Word Count: 685 BENCHMARK P. Birds of Prey A Reading A Z Level P Benchmark Book Word Count: 685 BENCHMARK P Birds of Prey Written by Fran Mason Visit www.readinga-z.com for thousands of books and materials. www.readinga-z.com Birds

More information

Many passerines defend territories in the non-breeding season, either as permanent residents (e.g., Verbeek 1973, Morton and Shalter 1977)

Many passerines defend territories in the non-breeding season, either as permanent residents (e.g., Verbeek 1973, Morton and Shalter 1977) J. Field Ornithol., 57(1):16-21 DEFENSE OF FALL TERRITORIES BY MATED AND UNMATED NORTHERN MOCKINGBIRDS IN SOUTHERN FLORIDA BY RANDALL BREITWISCH, MARILYN DIAZ, NATASHA GOTTLIEB, RONALD LEE, AND JULIA ZAIAS

More information

Six Decades of Migration Counts in North Carolina

Six Decades of Migration Counts in North Carolina Six Decades of Migration Counts in North Carolina Marilyn Westphal 230 Park Lane, Hendersonville, NC 28791 Introduction Might the day come when Turkeys are easier to come by than Northern Bobwhites? This

More information

Birding at Cylburn. For easy birding start on the grounds near the Cylburn Mansion... Proceed around the side of the Mansion...

Birding at Cylburn. For easy birding start on the grounds near the Cylburn Mansion... Proceed around the side of the Mansion... Birding at Cylburn For easy birding start on the grounds near the Cylburn Mansion... and scan the trees around the circle drive for Yellow-bellied Sapsucker in spring and fall and other woodpeckers year

More information

The Role of Avian Preen Gland Secretions in Host-Seeking Behavior of Culex pipiens Mosquitoes Research Advisor: Mary Garvin

The Role of Avian Preen Gland Secretions in Host-Seeking Behavior of Culex pipiens Mosquitoes Research Advisor: Mary Garvin Kevin Boyer (2018) Cella Wright (2018) Ben Lovett (2017) Mercedes Campos-Lopez (2018) Tempe Shrenker (2019) Scott Miller (2017) The Role of Avian Preen Gland Secretions in Host-Seeking Behavior of Culex

More information

Birds Foraging on Jerusalem Crickets, with Recent Observations in the Central Valley, California

Birds Foraging on Jerusalem Crickets, with Recent Observations in the Central Valley, California Birds Foraging on Jerusalem Crickets, with Recent Observations in the Central Valley, California Gary Woods, Fresno Audubon Society, P. O. Box 9324, Fresno, CA 93791, woodshots@earthlink.net Hannah Nadel,

More information

Animal Behaviour- Assignment 2

Animal Behaviour- Assignment 2 Animal Behaviour- Assignment 2 Roosting Behaviour in House Crows Group: Rachana Bhave (20101084), Prarabdha Jagdhane (20101021), V Saudamini (20101010) Species of Interest: Corvus splendens (House Crow)

More information

1. Summary DRAFT 1 24/01/2014 1

1. Summary DRAFT 1 24/01/2014 1 Project title: Conservation status of the Taita Falcon: further survey of the South African population Proponents: Andrew Jenkins (Raptor Research Programme, FitzPatrick Institute, University of Cape Town),

More information

Project Barn Owl. Title Project Barn Owl

Project Barn Owl. Title Project Barn Owl Project Barn Owl Title Project Barn Owl 1995-1997 Description and Summary of Results Throughout the 18th and early 19th centuries the Barn Owl Tyto alba was regarded as being the most common owl over much

More information

A presentation to: Rideau Lakes Municipal Services Committee Meeting March 14, A proposal for better cormorant control in Ontario

A presentation to: Rideau Lakes Municipal Services Committee Meeting March 14, A proposal for better cormorant control in Ontario A presentation to: Rideau Lakes Municipal Services Committee Meeting March 14, 2016 A proposal for better cormorant control in Ontario Background 30 species of cormorants worldwide Double-crested cormorant

More information

Introduction to Birding

Introduction to Birding Introduction to Birding Written by: Friends of the Arboretum Geo Activities and Education Committee Overland Park Arboretum & Botanical Gardens Illustrated by Andra Chase Funded by Friends of the Arboretum

More information

SCRUB AND MEXICAN JAYS

SCRUB AND MEXICAN JAYS COMPARATIVE MOBBING BEHAVIOR OF SCRUB AND MEXICAN JAYS JACK F. CULLY, JR., AND J. DAVID LIGON MOBBING behavior has been used in studying interspecific recognition (Hartley 1951), changes in the intensity

More information

The effect of interspecific competition on the foraging behavior of the Eastern Gray Squirrel

The effect of interspecific competition on the foraging behavior of the Eastern Gray Squirrel The effect of interspecific competition on the foraging behavior of the Eastern Gray Squirrel Jessica Dassen, Rachel Gerardy, Amberly Holcomb, and Lydia Nichols-Russell University of Maryland, Department

More information

Osprey Monitoring Guide

Osprey Monitoring Guide Audubon Society of Rhode Island Osprey Monitoring Guide Protecting Osprey Populations Through Volunteer Efforts Audubon Society of Rhode Island 12 Sanderson Road Smithfield, RI 02917 Tel: 401-949-5454

More information

Kestrels in Gloucestershire a factsheet (to be periodically updated as more records are received)

Kestrels in Gloucestershire a factsheet (to be periodically updated as more records are received) Kestrels in Gloucestershire a factsheet (to be periodically updated as more records are received) Records of Kestrel (courtesy of Richard Baatsen) give some indication of their fortunes over the past 15

More information

Bird Observations. Date Range: For. 1 of 5 2/29/2016 8:36 AM. Home About Submit Observations Explore Data My ebird Help

Bird Observations. Date Range: For. 1 of 5 2/29/2016 8:36 AM. Home About Submit Observations Explore Data My ebird Help 1 of 5 2/29/2016 8:36 AM Home About Submit Observations Explore Data My ebird Help Hello Steve Lombardi (brycesteve) Preferences Sign Out Language «Hotspot Explorer Bird Observations For [ Mt. Diablo SP--Mitchell

More information

Greenlaw Mountain Hawk Watch Fall 2011

Greenlaw Mountain Hawk Watch Fall 2011 Greenlaw Mountain Hawk Watch Fall 2011 Our third season of fall counts has been completed and it was an exciting year. We recorded 15 species of raptor, and had high season counts for several species and

More information

WILDLIFE REPORT JULY - SEPTEMBER 2016 TOP PINE HAWK MOTH BOTTOM PEPPERED MOTH

WILDLIFE REPORT JULY - SEPTEMBER 2016 TOP PINE HAWK MOTH BOTTOM PEPPERED MOTH WILDLIFE REPORT JULY - SEPTEMBER 2016 TOP PINE HAWK MOTH BOTTOM PEPPERED MOTH Introduction This season we have hosted a wide variety of visitors from local junior and senior schools to the Rotary Club

More information

Natural pest control: Barn Owls and diurnal raptors. Ground Squirrel & Gopher Management Workshop Ryan Bourbour & Emily Phillips September 11, 2018

Natural pest control: Barn Owls and diurnal raptors. Ground Squirrel & Gopher Management Workshop Ryan Bourbour & Emily Phillips September 11, 2018 Natural pest control: Barn Owls and diurnal raptors Ground Squirrel & Gopher Management Workshop Ryan Bourbour & Emily Phillips September 11, 2018 Information to share with you today 1. Barn Owls and natural

More information

Differential Timing of Spring Migration between Sex and Age Classes of Yellow-rumped Warblers (Setophaga coronata) in Central Alberta,

Differential Timing of Spring Migration between Sex and Age Classes of Yellow-rumped Warblers (Setophaga coronata) in Central Alberta, Differential Timing of Spring Migration between Sex and Age Classes of Yellow-rumped Warblers (Setophaga coronata) in Central Alberta, 1999-2015 By: Steven Griffeth SPRING BIOLOGIST- BEAVERHILL BIRD OBSERVATORY

More information

General Secretariat Delegations Problem of necrophagous birds in Spain because of shortage of natural food: a serious threat to biodiversity

General Secretariat Delegations Problem of necrophagous birds in Spain because of shortage of natural food: a serious threat to biodiversity COUNCIL OF THE EUROPEAN UNION Brussels, 24 October 2007 (25.10) (OR. en,es) 14301/07 ENV 555 NOTE from : to : Subject : General Secretariat Delegations Problem of necrophagous birds in Spain because of

More information

Central Valley Winter Raptor Survey ( ): Winter Raptor Population Estimates

Central Valley Winter Raptor Survey ( ): Winter Raptor Population Estimates Central Valley Winter Raptor Survey (2007-2010): Winter Raptor Population Estimates Edward R. Pandolfino, 5530 Delrose Court, Carmichael, CA 95608 Zachary Smith, 812 1/2 11th St., Davis, CA 95616 Estimating

More information

Tree Swallow (Tachycineta bicolour)

Tree Swallow (Tachycineta bicolour) Baker River Project Terrestrial Working Group Analysis Species Tree Swallow (Tachycineta bicolour) Drafted by: René Martin Habitat Type: Snag/Log Dependent Note: Bird Accounts from the Birds of North America

More information

Bald Eagle and Osprey Nest Survey Study Plan for Energy Northwest's Packwood Lake Hydroelectric Project FERC No Lewis County, Washington

Bald Eagle and Osprey Nest Survey Study Plan for Energy Northwest's Packwood Lake Hydroelectric Project FERC No Lewis County, Washington Revised Bald Eagle and Osprey Nest Survey Study Plan for Energy Northwest's Packwood Lake Hydroelectric Project FERC No. 2244 Lewis County, Washington Submitted to P.O. Box 968 Richland, Washington 99352-0968

More information

Greenlaw Mountain Hawk Watch Fall 2012

Greenlaw Mountain Hawk Watch Fall 2012 Greenlaw Mountain Hawk Watch Fall 2012 Our fourth season of data collection has been completed. There were numerous exciting moments and our season total was the second highest on record. Single-day high

More information

Caspian Gull. Caspian Gull at Hythe (Ian Roberts)

Caspian Gull. Caspian Gull at Hythe (Ian Roberts) 1985 1986 1987 1988 1989 199 1991 1992 1993 1994 1995 1996 1997 1998 1999 2 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 21 211 212 213 214 215 216 217 218 219 Caspian Gull Larus cachinnans Very rare vagrant Category A

More information

SEASONAL MIGRATION PATTERN OF OWLS AT BUKOWO-KOPAÑ STATION (N POLAND) IN

SEASONAL MIGRATION PATTERN OF OWLS AT BUKOWO-KOPAÑ STATION (N POLAND) IN SEASONAL MIGRATION PATTERN OF OWLS AT BUKOWO-KOPAÑ STATION (N POLAND) IN -3 Damiana Michalonek, Wojciech Busse and Przemys³aw Busse ABSTRACT Michalonek D.A., Busse W., Busse P. 4. Seasonal migration pattern

More information