The Shape-Weight Illusion

Similar documents
First-order structure induces the 3-D curvature contrast effect

Salient features make a search easy

Muscular Torque Can Explain Biases in Haptic Length Perception: A Model Study on the Radial-Tangential Illusion

This is a postprint of. The influence of material cues on early grasping force. Bergmann Tiest, W.M., Kappers, A.M.L.

Haptic Perception of Real and Virtual Curvature

The Haptic Perception of Spatial Orientations studied with an Haptic Display

The influence of exploration mode, orientation, and configuration on the haptic Mu«ller-Lyer illusion

Context effects in haptic perception of roughness

The horizon line, linear perspective, interposition, and background brightness as determinants of the magnitude of the pictorial moon illusion

Using curvature information in haptic shape perception of 3D objects

Report. Experience Can Change Distinct Size-Weight Priors Engaged in Lifting Objects and Judging their Weights

If you are searching for a book Haptics: Generating and Perceiving Tangible Sensations, Part II: 7th International Conference, EuroHaptics 2010,

Expression of 2DOF Fingertip Traction with 1DOF Lateral Skin Stretch

Visual influence on haptic torque perception

Haptic perception of spatial relations

The vertical-horizontal illusion: Assessing the contributions of anisotropy, abutting, and crossing to the misperception of simple line stimuli

The Influence of Visual Illusion on Visually Perceived System and Visually Guided Action System

Image Enhancement in Spatial Domain

Here I present more details about the methods of the experiments which are. described in the main text, and describe two additional examinations which

Egocentric reference frame bias in the palmar haptic perception of surface orientation. Allison Coleman and Frank H. Durgin. Swarthmore College

A reexamination of the size weight illusion induced by visual size cues

Spatial Judgments from Different Vantage Points: A Different Perspective

Texture recognition using force sensitive resistors

Spatial Low Pass Filters for Pin Actuated Tactile Displays

Thresholds for Dynamic Changes in a Rotary Switch

Discrimination of Virtual Haptic Textures Rendered with Different Update Rates

Development of a Finger Mounted Type Haptic Device Using a Plane Approximated to Tangent Plane

The Willows Primary School Mental Mathematics Policy

Year 5 Problems and Investigations Spring

The Effect of Opponent Noise on Image Quality

PUBLICATIONS Journal articles, books, book chapters

Haptic Invitation of Textures: An Estimation of Human Touch Motions

Misjudging where you felt a light switch in a dark room

A Perceptual Study on Haptic Rendering of Surface Topography when Both Surface Height and Stiffness Vary

Häkkinen, Jukka; Gröhn, Lauri Turning water into rock

THE SIZE-WEIGHT ILLUSION IN A NATURAL AND AUGMENTED ENVIRONMENT WITH CONGRUENT AND INCONGRUENT SIZE INFORMATION

Vision, haptics, and attention: new data from a multisensory Necker cube

The Effect of Frequency Shifting on Audio-Tactile Conversion for Enriching Musical Experience

THE POGGENDORFF ILLUSION WITH ANOMALOUS SURFACES: MANAGING PAC-MANS, PARALLELS LENGTH AND TYPE OF TRANSVERSAL.

Differences in Fitts Law Task Performance Based on Environment Scaling

The fragile edges of. block averaged portraits

Reducing the motor response in haptic parallel matching eliminates the typically observed gender difference

Unit IV: Sensation & Perception. Module 19 Vision Organization & Interpretation

Influence of stimulus symmetry on visual scanning patterns*

There's More to Touch Than Meets the Eye: The Salience of Object Attributes for Haptics With and Without Vision

Independence of perceptual and sensorimotor predictions in the size weight illusion

Haptic presentation of 3D objects in virtual reality for the visually disabled

Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion

Discriminating direction of motion trajectories from angular speed and background information

Influence of Shape Elements on Performance during Haptic Rotation

Unit 9: May/June Solid Shapes

Effect of the number of loudspeakers on sense of presence in 3D audio system based on multiple vertical panning

Perception of pitch. Importance of pitch: 2. mother hemp horse. scold. Definitions. Why is pitch important? AUDL4007: 11 Feb A. Faulkner.

Psychophysics of night vision device halo

Perception. What We Will Cover in This Section. Perception. How we interpret the information our senses receive. Overview Perception

GROUPING BASED ON PHENOMENAL PROXIMITY

Wearable Haptic Display to Present Gravity Sensation

A Pilot Study: Introduction of Time-domain Segment to Intensity-based Perception Model of High-frequency Vibration

Module 2. Lecture-1. Understanding basic principles of perception including depth and its representation.

Perception of the horizontal and vertical in tangible displays: minimal gender differences

The Effect of Haptic Feedback on Basic Social Interaction within Shared Virtual Environments

Efficient Bimodal Haptic Weight Actuation

Haptic Cueing of a Visual Change-Detection Task: Implications for Multimodal Interfaces

From Encoding Sound to Encoding Touch

Answers Solving mathematical problems

Uniformity and Crosstalk in MultiAnode Photomultiplier Tubes

Müller-Lyer Illusion Effect on a Reaching Movement in Simultaneous Presentation of Visual and Haptic/Kinesthetic Cues

IOC, Vector sum, and squaring: three different motion effects or one?

Object Perception. 23 August PSY Object & Scene 1

Integration of force and position cues for shape perception through active touch

Second Quarter Benchmark Expectations for Sections 4 and 5. Count orally by ones to 50. Count forward to 50 starting from numbers other than 1.

IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON HAPTICS, VOL. 1, NO. 1, JANUARY-JUNE Haptic Processing of Facial Expressions of Emotion in 2D Raised-Line Drawings

Abstract shape: a shape that is derived from a visual source, but is so transformed that it bears little visual resemblance to that source.

the human chapter 1 Traffic lights the human User-centred Design Light Vision part 1 (modified extract for AISD 2005) Information i/o

Chapter 3. Adaptation to disparity but not to perceived depth

How Many Pixels Do We Need to See Things?

Perception of Haptic Force Magnitude during Hand Movements

Mathematics. Foundation. Set E Paper 2 (Calculator)

Ebbinghaus Illusion in the Tactile Modality

Haptic Cues: Texture as a Guide for Non-Visual Tangible Interaction.

Haptic perception of linear extent

On Contrast Sensitivity in an Image Difference Model

Proprioception & force sensing

Collaborative Pseudo-Haptics: Two-User Stiffness Discrimination Based on Visual Feedback

Size Illusion on an Asymmetrically Divided Circle

A comparison of learning with haptic and visual modalities.

Perception of pitch. Definitions. Why is pitch important? BSc Audiology/MSc SHS Psychoacoustics wk 5: 12 Feb A. Faulkner.

Perception: From Biology to Psychology

The Grand Illusion and Petit Illusions

Visuohaptic Discrimination of 3D Gross Shape

Perception of pitch. Definitions. Why is pitch important? BSc Audiology/MSc SHS Psychoacoustics wk 4: 7 Feb A. Faulkner.

TERM 2 MATHS NOTES COMMON FRACTIONS

Nonuniform multi level crossing for signal reconstruction

ABSTRACT. Keywords: Color image differences, image appearance, image quality, vision modeling 1. INTRODUCTION

Rendering Moving Tactile Stroke on the Palm Using a Sparse 2D Array

Apparent depth with motion aftereffect and head movement

Tactile Vision Substitution with Tablet and Electro-Tactile Display

NAVIGATIONAL CONTROL EFFECT ON REPRESENTING VIRTUAL ENVIRONMENTS

No symmetry advantage when object matching involves accidental viewpoints

Development of A Finger Mounted Type Haptic Device Using A Plane Approximated to Tangent Plane

Transcription:

The Shape-Weight Illusion Mirela Kahrimanovic, Wouter M. Bergmann Tiest, and Astrid M.L. Kappers Universiteit Utrecht, Helmholtz Institute Padualaan 8, 3584 CH Utrecht, The Netherlands {m.kahrimanovic,w.m.bergmanntiest,a.m.l.kappers}@uu.nl Abstract. In the present experiment, we investigated the influence of the shape of 3-dimensional objects on haptic perception of weight. A systematic shapeweight illusion was found when subjects compared a tetrahedron to a cube: a cube was perceived as being heavier than a tetrahedron of the same physical mass and volume. However, when subjects compared a sphere to a tetrahedron or to a cube, some subjects perceived the sphere as heavier than the other objects, while other subjects perceived the sphere as being lighter. These results indicate that the influence of shape on haptic perception of weight is not mediated only by the perceived volume, as would be predicted from previous studies, but that some subject dependent factors are involved. Keywords: Weight, Size, 3-D shape, Touch, Haptics. 1 Introduction When a human observer wants to lift or to move an object, weight information is necessary for the determination of the force that has to be applied in order to perform efficiently the required manipulation of that object. The initial weight percept can be biased by different physical object properties. For example, when human observers compare two objects of the same mass but of different size, they perceive the smaller object as being heavier. This is known as the size-weight illusion [1]. Dresslar [2] and Ellis [3] proposed that weight perception is also influenced by the shape of objects. To study this illusion, Dresslar used a set of objects that were made of sheet lead. They were all of the same unreported area and weight. Their shape varied from regular figures, such as a circle or a square, to irregular cornered figures. The objects could be explored unrestricted. On the other hand, Ellis used a set of solid objects with the same weight (350 g) and volume (132 cm³). The objects were rectangular cuboids with different dimensions and cylindrical objects with different lengths and diameters. The objects were explored in separate visual, haptic and bimodal conditions. The exploration was restricted to a four-finger/opposing-thumb grasp and the subjects were allowed only to lift the objects in a vertical motion. These two studies revealed contradictory results. Dresslar showed that objects that appeared to be the smallest or of the most compact form were judged to be the heaviest. In contrast, Ellis showed that the largest objects were perceived to be the heaviest. Regardless of which of these results is more convincing, it is more important to question if these experiments are an appropriate way of measuring the shape-weight illusion. The main problem that we have with Dresslar s study is that the objects varied A.M.L. Kappers et al. (Eds.): EuroHaptics 2010, Part I, LNCS 6191, pp. 17 22, 2010. Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2010

18 M. Kahrimanovic, W.M.B. Tiest, and A.M.L. Kappers only along two dimensions. On the other hand, Ellis used 3-dimensional objects, but the exploration was restricted in such a way that mainly the width of the objects was perceived. The present experiment was designed to omit these problems, and to study the possible existence of a shape-weight illusion in a more appropriate way. We used a set of cubes, tetrahedrons and spheres, which could fit in the hand when explored. Subjects were blindfolded and the exploration was not restricted, encouraging the subjects to perceive shape, volume and weight of the objects as thoroughly as possible. The bias for the shape-weight illusion can be predicted from the size-weight [4] and the shape-size [5] illusions. Ellis and Lederman [4] investigated a purely haptic size-weight illusion and showed that a doubling of the volume of cubes with the same physical weight resulted in a 26 % decrease of their estimated weight. If we assume that this relationship holds for the whole range of volumes and weights, then it can be written as W new W old = V new V old 0.43, (1) where W is the perceived weight and V the perceived volume of objects. Kahrimanovic et al. [5] demonstrated the occurrence of a shape-size illusion when subjects compared differently shaped objects. A tetrahedron was perceived as being larger than a cube or a sphere of the same volume, and a cube was perceived as larger than a sphere. The measured biases are shown in the second column of Table 1. If the shape-weight illusion is mediated by the perceived size of objects, as proposed by Dresslar [2], then we can predict that a sphere will be perceived as heavier than a cube or a tetrahedron of the same physical mass, and that a cube will be perceived as heavier than a tetrahedron. The magnitude of these biases can be predicted by substituting the biases found in Kahrimanovic et al. [5] for the ratio V new /V old in Equation (1). The biases predicted from these studies are shown in the third column of Table 1. On the other hand, if the shape-weight illusion is not mediated by the perceived volume, as proposed by Ellis [3], then the biases will be in the opposite direction and/or their magnitude will deviate from the expected magnitude. The present study will investigate these possibilities. Table 1. Predictions for the shape-weight illusion. The magnitude of the biases indicates the relative difference between the physical volumes/weights of two differently shaped objects that are perceived as equal in volume/weight. The biases are expressed with respect to the first mentioned object in each object-pair. The direction of the bias shows which object is expected to be perceived as lighter/heavier if objects of the same physical weight are compared. Condition Magnitude of the bias Direction of the bias Shape Size [5] Shape Weight (prediction) Dresslar [2] Ellis [3] - 32 % - 11 % < > - 11 % - 4 % < > - 21 % - 8 % < >

The Shape-Weight Illusion 19 2 Methods 2.1 Subjects Eight subjects participated in this experiment. Subjects 2, 3, 6, 7 and 8 were male and subjects 1, 4 and 5 were female (mean age 20 years). All subjects were strongly righthanded, as established by Coren s handedness test [6]. 2.2 Stimuli and Procedure Tetrahedrons, cubes and spheres were used as stimuli (see Fig. 1). They were made of brass and their mass ranged from 16.8 to 117.6 g, in steps of 8.4 g. The volume of the objects co-varied consistently with their mass. Hence, the volume ranged from 2 to 14 cm³, in steps of 1 cm³. These stimuli are the same as those used in our previous paper on the effects of shape on volume perception [5]. The stimuli were placed in the centre of the hand palm of blindfolded subjects (see Fig. 1). The subjects were instructed to enclose the stimuli, thereby perceiving their 3-D shape and volume. During enclosure, they were allowed to perform an up and down movement with the hand, in order to perceive the weight. These instructions were based on the stereotypic exploratory procedures associated with the perceptual encoding of these object properties [7]. The subjects were instructed to maintain the same exploratory procedure during the complete experiment. The period of exploration was not restricted, but was often just a few seconds. After exploration of the first stimulus, this stimulus was replaced by a second stimulus, which was explored in the same way as the first one. The order of the reference and comparison stimuli was randomized. The participants judged which of the two explored stimuli was heavier. Fig. 1. On the left: the stimulus set. On the right: stimulus placed on the hand of the subject. 2.3 Conditions The experiment consisted of 9 conditions; 3 object-pairs (tetrahedron-sphere, tetrahedron-cube, cube-sphere) and 3 reference-weights (small, medium, large). The reference weights were for the tetrahedrons 25.2, 42 and 58.8 g, for the cubes 33.6, 50.4 and 67.2 g, and for the spheres 42, 58.8 and 75.6 g. These references were the same as in the study on haptic volume perception [5]. The conditions were performed within 3 sessions. Object-pair was randomized between sessions and reference-weight was randomized within sessions. For each combination of object-pair and referenceweight, each shape was the reference stimulus in half of the trials and test in the remainder. For each reference, 35 trials were performed, resulting for each subject in a total of 210 trials per session and 630 trials for the complete experiment. On average, 2 hours per subject were needed to perform all the conditions.

20 M. Kahrimanovic, W.M.B. Tiest, and A.M.L. Kappers 2.4 Data Collection The data were collected by way of a computer-driven 1-up-1-down staircase procedure (see Fig. 2A). For each object-pair by reference-weight combination, two staircases were intermingled, each starting at one end of the stimulus range, i.e. at 16.8 and 117.6 g. For each stimulus combination, the fraction was calculated with which the subject selected the test stimulus to be heavier than the reference stimulus. This calculation was performed for all test weights. A cumulative Gaussian distribution (f) as function of the physical mass (x) was fitted to the data with the maximumlikelihood procedure [8], using the following equation: f (x) = 1 2 1 + erf x μ, (2) σ 2 where σ is a measure of the discrimination threshold, and µ a measure of the Point of Subjective Equality (PSE). This point indicates the physical weight of the test stimulus that is perceived to be of the same heaviness as the reference stimulus (see Fig. 2B). Fig. 2. (A) An example of the staircase. (B) The psychometric curve fitted to the data. Relative biases were calculated from these data. For each matched object pair (tetrahedron-sphere, tetrahedron-cube and cube-sphere), the mass of the object mentioned first was subtracted from that mentioned second and expressed as a percentage of the mass of the object mentioned first. Hence, the magnitude of these biases indicated the relative difference between the physical weights of two differently shaped objects that are perceived as equal in weight. These relative biases were used for the statistical analysis. 3 Results Fig. 3 shows the individual and average perceptual biases for the different object comparisons. The biases from the different reference-weights are taken together, since no significant effect of reference-weight was measured (F 2,14 = 3.7, p = 0.08). A t-test revealed only significant biases for the tetrahedron-cube comparison (t 7 = -9.3, p < 0.001). The left part of the figure shows that these biases are consistently negative, with an average of about -18 %, SE 2 %. A negative bias in this condition means that

The Shape-Weight Illusion 21 a cube is perceived as being heavier than a tetrahedron of the same volume. The analysis revealed that the average biases in the other conditions were not significantly different from zero (t 7 = -0.4, p = 0.7 and t 7 = 0.8, p = 0.4 for the tetrahedron-sphere and cube-sphere comparisons respectively). However, we cannot ignore the large individual differences in these conditions. As the middle and right parts of Fig. 3 demonstrate, the sphere is perceived as being either heavier (negative bias) or lighter (positive bias) than a tetrahedron or a cube of the same physical mass. This pattern is consistent within subjects. Fig. 3. Individual and mean data (M) for the different conditions. The subjects are ordered from the subject with the most negative bias to the most positive bias in the tetrahedron-sphere condition. The error bars for the individual subjects represent the standard deviations, and for the average data they represent the standard errors of the mean. 4 Discussion The present study demonstrates that the weight of a tetrahedron is consistently underestimated compared to the weight of a cube with the same physical mass and volume. The average bias in that condition was -18 %. The direction of the bias corresponds to the prediction from the study by Dresslar [2], who showed that (2-D) objects that appear smaller are perceived to be heavier. The magnitude of the measured bias is larger than the predicted bias of -4 % (see Table 1). This indicates that the shapeweight illusion cannot be explained only on the basis of a direct influence of perceived volume on perceived weight. This conclusion is supported by the large individual differences observed in the tetrahedron-sphere and cube-sphere comparisons. Ellis [3] concluded that, instead of perceived volume, the profile area (defined as the area of the largest face) is the best predictor for perceived weight. However, the influence of the profile area may be triggered by the restriction of the exploration strategy to a four-finger/opposing-thumb grasp. In our experiment, the exploration was not restricted and each subject could select its own strategy, which probably caused the large individual differences. An interesting observation is that the individual differences in the direction of the bias are only observed in the conditions including a sphere. This may be caused by the variation in the size of the object s area that is in contact with the skin during exploration. This contact area for a sphere is smaller when the hand is stretched out than when it is cupped. How far the hand is cupped could differ between subjects. For a tetrahedron or a cube the contact area did not differ because of a flat base. This may be a relevant point to investigate in further experiments.

22 M. Kahrimanovic, W.M.B. Tiest, and A.M.L. Kappers The present study emphasizes that we cannot ignore the influence of an object s shape on weight perception, since large perceptual biases are observed. Nevertheless, with these results we cannot explain the shape-weight illusion in detail, and more research is needed to understand this phenomenon. A comprehensive understanding of these effects can be important for different problems of more applied nature. As we noted in the introduction, a proper estimation of the weight of objects is necessary for the determination of the force that has to be applied in order to manipulate the object efficiently. This object could also be, for example, a handle of a machine. For the construction of such handles, it is necessary to consider the factors that could influence the perception and action of a human operator. The shape of objects seems to be one of them. Acknowledgments. This research was supported by a grant from The Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO). References 1. Murray, D.J., Ellis, R.R., Bandomir, C.A., Ross, H.E.: Charpentier (1891) on the sizeweight illusion. Perception & Psychophysics 61, 1681 1685 (1891) 2. Dresslar, F.B.: Studies in the psychology of touch. American Journal of Psychology 6, 313 368 (1894) 3. Ellis, R.R.: Haptic weight illusions with and without vision. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Queen s University Kingston, Ontario, Canada (1996) 4. Ellis, R.R., Lederman, S.J.: The role of haptic versus visual volume cues in the size-weight illusion. Perception & Psychophysics 53, 315 324 (1993) 5. Kahrimanovic, M., Bergmann Tiest, W.M., Kappers, A.M.L.: Haptic perception of volume and surface area of 3-D objects. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics 72, 517 527 (2010) 6. Coren, S.: The left-hander syndrome. Vintage Books, New York (1993) 7. Lederman, S.J., Klatzky, R.L.: Hand movements: A window into haptic object recognition. Cognitive Psychology 19, 342 368 (1987) 8. Wichmann, F.A., Hill, N.J.: The psychometric function: I. Fitting, sampling, and goodness of fit. Perception & Psychophysics 63, 1293 1313 (2001)