Supplementary Figure 1

Similar documents
Methods. Experimental Stimuli: We selected 24 animals, 24 tools, and 24

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

The Neural Basis of Intuitive Best Next-Move Generation in Board Game Experts

S1 Table. Characterization of the articles (n=20) included for systematic review. (A) population, acquisition and analysis parameters; (B)

SIEMENS MAGNETOM Skyra syngo MR D13

MRI Summer Course Lab 2: Gradient Echo T1 & T2* Curves

2014 M.S. Cohen all rights reserved

H uman perception is not a sequence of snapshots of the outer world but a constructive process to cope with

Supplementary Material

Distributed representation of objects in the human ventral visual pathway (face perception functional MRI object recognition)

Supplementary Information for Common neural correlates of real and imagined movements contributing to the performance of brain machine interfaces

Functional MRI with variable echo time acquisition

Bodies are Represented as Wholes Rather Than Their Sum of Parts in the Occipital-Temporal Cortex

Clear delineation of optic radiation and very small vessels using phase difference enhanced imaging (PADRE)

Image Quality/Artifacts Frequency (MHz)

1 Introduction. 2 The basic principles of NMR

It Takes Two Skilled Recognition of Objects Engages Lateral Areas in Both Hemispheres

Simultaneous Multi-Slice (Slice Accelerated) Diffusion EPI

a. Use (at least) window lengths of 256, 1024, and 4096 samples to compute the average spectrum using a window overlap of 0.5.

3T Unlimited. ipat on MAGNETOM Allegra The Importance of ipat at 3T. medical

First-level fmri modeling. UCLA Advanced NeuroImaging Summer School, 2010

Image-Invariant Responses in Face-Selective Regions Do Not Explain the Perceptual Advantage for Familiar Face Recognition

Human Brain Mapping. Face-likeness and image variability drive responses in human face-selective ventral regions

Page 1 of 9. Protocol: adult_other_adni3_study_human_ge_3t_25w_ _ _1. 3 Plane Localizer. 3 Plane Localizer PATIENT POSITION

Here I present more details about the methods of the experiments which are. described in the main text, and describe two additional examinations which

The Physiology of the Senses Lecture 3: Visual Perception of Objects

Functional Connectivity Mapping for Correlated Resting State Image Volumes

M R I Physics Course. Jerry Allison Ph.D., Chris Wright B.S., Tom Lavin B.S., Nathan Yanasak Ph.D. Department of Radiology Medical College of Georgia

MARP. MR Accreditation Program Quality Control Beyond Just the Scans and Measurements July 2005

Stimulus-dependent position sensitivity in human ventral temporal cortex

The Representation of Parts and Wholes in Faceselective

Enhancing Gray-to-White Matter Contrast in 3T T1 Spin-Echo Brain Scans by Optimizing Flip Angle

fmri design efficiency

Towards the development of cognitive robots

The SENSE Ghost: Field-of-View Restrictions for SENSE Imaging

SECTION I - CHAPTER 2 DIGITAL IMAGING PROCESSING CONCEPTS

Attenuation Correction in Hybrid MR-BrainPET Imaging

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Fusiform Face Area in Chess Expertise

Haptic study of three-dimensional objects activates extrastriate visual areas

Off-line EEG analysis of BCI experiments with MATLAB V1.07a. Copyright g.tec medical engineering GmbH

Advanced MSK MRI Protocols at 3.0T. Garry E. Gold, M.D. Associate Professor Department of Radiology Stanford University

SNR and functional sensitivity of BOLD and perfusion-based fmri using arterial spin labeling with spiral SENSE at 3 T

Downloaded from by on 02/07/18 from IP address Copyright ARRS. For personal use only; all rights reserved

COMPUTED TOMOGRAPHY 1

PET Performance Evaluation of MADPET4: A Small Animal PET Insert for a 7-T MRI Scanner

A Real-World Size Organization of Object Responses in Occipitotemporal Cortex

How Gamma Camera s Head-Tilts Affect Image Quality of a Nuclear Scintigram?

Noise Characteristics of the FORE+OSEM(DB) Reconstruction Method for the MiCES PET Scanner

Radionuclide Imaging MII Single Photon Emission Computed Tomography (SPECT)

MR Advance Techniques. Flow Phenomena. Class II

Face Perception. The Thatcher Illusion. The Thatcher Illusion. Can you recognize these upside-down faces? The Face Inversion Effect

Optimizing the Magnetization-Prepared Rapid Gradient-Echo (MP-RAGE) Sequence

Methods for functional magnetic resonance imaging in normal and lesioned behaving monkeys

MR Basics: Module 8 Image Quality

Reinventing movies How do we tell stories in VR? Diego Gutierrez Graphics & Imaging Lab Universidad de Zaragoza

MRI imaging in neuroscience Dr. Thom Oostendorp Lab class: 2 hrs

Alae Tracker: Tracking of the Nasal Walls in MR-Imaging

fmri-derived cortical maps for haptic shape, texture, and hardness

TimTX TrueShape. The parallel transmit architecture of the future. Answers for life.

Salient features make a search easy

WU-Minn HCP 1200 Subjects Release: Reference Manual

Image Interpretation System for Informed Consent to Patients by Use of a Skeletal Tracking

PSY 310: Sensory and Perceptual Processes 1

from signals to sources asa-lab turnkey solution for ERP research

The fusiform face area is not sufficient for face recognition: Evidence from a patient with dense prosopagnosia and no occipital face area

The shape of luminance increments at the intersection alters the magnitude of the scintillating grid illusion

Brain Computer Interfaces for Full Body Movement and Embodiment. Intelligent Robotics Seminar Kai Brusch

The Usefulness of Simultaneously Excited Magnetic Resonance Signals from Diffusion Tensor Image

CoE4TN4 Image Processing. Chapter 3: Intensity Transformation and Spatial Filtering

(N)MR Imaging. Lab Course Script. FMP PhD Autumn School. Location: C81, MRI Lab B0.03 (basement) Instructor: Leif Schröder. Date: November 3rd, 2010

Non-Invasive Brain-Actuated Control of a Mobile Robot

12/21/2016. Siemens Medical Systems Research Agreement Philips Healthcare Research Agreement AAN and ASN Committees

Simulating Biological Motion Perception Using a Recurrent Neural Network

Pulse Sequence Design and Image Procedures

Active Mechatronic Interface for Haptic Perception Studies with Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Compatibility and Design Criteria

Radionuclide Imaging MII 3073 RADIONUCLIDE IMAGING SYSTEM

High Field MRI: Technology, Applications, Safety, and Limitations

Chapter 8: Perceiving Motion

NEMA Standards Publication MS (R2014) Determination of Signal-to-Noise Ratio (SNR) in Diagnostic Magnetic Resonance Imaging

IMAGE ENHANCEMENT - POINT PROCESSING

Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion

Evaluation of sliding window correlation performance for characterizing dynamic functional connectivity and brain states

A Sequence of Object-Processing Stages Revealed by fmri in the Human Occipital Lobe

KYMATA DATASET 3.01: README

Spiral MRI on a 9.4T Vertical-bore Superconducting Magnet Using Unshielded and Self-shielded Gradient Coils

BOLD fmri: signal source, data acquisition, and interpretation

Digital Image Processing

NEUROIMAGING DATA ANALYSIS SOFTWARE

Prof. Vidya Manian Dept. of Electrical and Comptuer Engineering

Echo-Planar Imaging for a 9.4 Tesla Vertical-Bore Superconducting Magnet Using an Unshielded Gradient Coil

Reach-to-Grasp Actions Under Direct and Indirect Viewing Conditions

ROBOT VISION. Dr.M.Madhavi, MED, MVSREC

Takeharu Seno 1,3,4, Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2, Stephen Palmisano 5 1

The visual and oculomotor systems. Peter H. Schiller, year The visual cortex

Vision V Perceiving Movement

Passive Tracking Exploiting Local Signal Conservation: The White Marker Phenomenon

Vision V Perceiving Movement

Further Customization of Dot Engines: AutoCoverage

A Three-Dimensional Evaluation of Body Representation Change of Human Upper Limb Focused on Sense of Ownership and Sense of Agency

Transcription:

Supplementary Figure 1 Left aspl Right aspl Detailed description of the fmri activation during allocentric action observation in the aspl. Averaged activation (N=13) during observation of the allocentric clips in the aspl ROIs. The hand preference (for the ipsilateral hand) can be seen in both visual fields. Thus, right hand actions generate greater activation in the right aspl than left hand actions, in both visual fields (and vice versa in the left aspl). Indeed, an analysis of variance of the beta weights assigned to each condition during observation of the four allocentric conditions in the ROIs revealed that there was no significant interaction between the visual field and the hand identity (F 1,12 =0.4 in the left aspl and F 1,12 =0.18 in the right aspl). A significant hand identity effect was found in both hemispheres (F 1,12 = 27.43, P<0.001 in the left aspl, and F 1,12 =11.88, P<0.005 in the right aspl). Additionally, significant visual field effect was found in the right aspl (F 1,12 =7.89, P<0.02).

Supplementary Figure 2 Hand identity effect in the aspl ROIs can be seen in the two visual fields Comparison between the activation during observation of the allocentric conditions in the ipsilateral and contralateral visual fields, pooled across hemispheres. Each condition was named according to the seen hand (ipsilateral/contralateral) and the visual field (ipsilateral/contralateral). In each subject, activation was normalized by subtracting the mean activation level during all 4 allocentric conditions. This was done separately for the right aspl and the left aspl. The results show the averaged activation (N=13) across the two hemispheres. Although on average there is greater activation in the contralateral visual field (blue bars) than in the ipsilateral one (orange bars) (i.e. a visual field effect), the differences between the activation elicited by viewing the ipsilateral hand and the contralateral one are clear and statistically significant in both visual fields (paired t-test, P<0.04 for the ipsilateral field, P<0.001 in the contralateral field).

Supplementary Figure 3 Action observation effects Direct contrast between the object manipulation clips presented in the right and left visual field revealed a strong visual field effect in the occipito-temporal cortex. Thus, clips that were presented in the contralateral visual field elicited significantly higher activation than clips that were presented in the ipsilateral visual field. Contralateral preference to the location of the clip in the visual field is shown in green. The viewpoint effect was calculated by comparing the activation during observation of the hand actions shown from an egocentric and allocentric points of view. Preference for the allocentric clips can be seen in the lateral occipital complex of the left hemisphere (orange clusters). No significant preference for the egocentric clips was found. Hand identity effect was calculated by comparing the activation during observation of left hand action and right hand action. This contrast revealed a small cluster in the left frontal cortex. This cluster demonstrated a preference for right hand action over left hand actions. No significant opposite preference was found. Finally, mirror like representation, i.e, differential activation during right egocentric and left allocentric clips (compared to left egocentric and right allocentric clips), can be seen in the superior parietal lobule (purple clusters). Consistent with figure 1 in the main text, the purple clusters in the left hemisphere represent preference for the right egocentric and left allocentric clips (compared with the left egocentric and right allocentric clips) and the cluster in the right SPL represents the opposite preference.

Supplementary Figure 4 Representation of observed hand actions in the primary motor cortex. One concern is that the differential activation in the aspl stems from unconscious execution of movements during observation of the clips. To address this issue, we measured the activation in the primary motor cortex during observation of hand actions. First, we identified the primary motor cortex of the 9 subjects that participated in the somatomotor mapping experiment by looking for voxels that show significantly higher activation during execution of actions with the contralateral hand than the ipsilateral hand in the central sulcus (P<0.001). Then, we measured the activation in this ROI during the second experiment, which was consisted of imitation and observation conditions. The only significant activation was found in the contralateral (left) M1 during imitation of action (one sample t-test, P<0.001). During the observation of action, the activation in M1 did not differ significantly from zero. This clear distinction makes it extremely unlikely that the observation epochs were unconscious imitation epochs.

Methods Subjects Thirteen right handed volunteers without neurological, psychiatric or visual deficits history (seven women and seven men, ages 25 49) gave their written consent and participated in the present fmri experiments. The Hadassah Ein Karem Medical Center Ethics Committee approved the experimental procedure. MRI acquisition The BOLD fmri measurements were performed in a whole-body 1.5-T Magnetom Avanto SIMENS scanner. The functional MRI protocols were based on a multi-slice gradient echo-planar imaging and a standard head coil. The functional data were obtained under the optimal timing parameters: TR = 3sec, TE = 50ms, flip angle = 90, imaging matrix = 64*64, FOV = 20cm. The 32 slices with slice thickness 3mm (with 0.3mm gap) were oriented in the axial position. The scan covered the whole brain. Experimental setup Footages were taken using a digital camera (Sony trv60e), edited (using the program: "windows movie maker"), and projected via LCD projector (Epson MP 7200, Japan) onto a tangent screen located inside the scanner in front of the subject. Subjects viewed the screen through a tilted mirror. Visual stimuli A set of 32 object manipulation clips, 12 sec long, and 2 visual control clips were shown in this experiment. Right hand clips were composed of twelve 600-1400 ms, black and white footages, of a right hand approaching grasping and releasing an object (total time 12 sec). The footages were taken in two different settings. In the first, the camera was behind the acting hand, and in the second, it was in front of the acting hand. The footages were taken using a set of 20 man-made small graspable objects, such as different cubes, cylinders, pens, scissors, etc. Six to ten footages were taken with each object, showing different grasping movements. Left hand clips were generated by mirror-image transformation of the footages showing right hand actions.

In all clips the objects remained stationary, and did not move throughout the grasping movements. In the control "scramble" conditions, a 12-sec object-manipulation clip was decomposed to frames (30 frames a second), every frame was spatially scrambled (36*24 fragments in each frame, ensuring that the hand and the objects could no longer be identified) and then the frames were recomposed to a 12-sec clip. A fixation point appeared in the middle of the screen throughout the experiment. Experimental paradigm: Action observation experiment: The experiment was carried out using a block design format. Ten blocks (consisting of 8 different object grasping conditions, and 2 "scramble" conditions) were interleaved, and were repeated 4 times, in two different runs, using different stimuli, in a counter-balanced manner. Each block lasted 12 s followed by a blank period of 9 s. The first and last blank periods were longer (21 and 15 s, respectively). The clips were centered 16.5 degrees to the right or the left of the fixation point, and subtended 20*15 degrees. Subjects were trained before the experiment to make sure they maintain fixation and understand the instructions. Action observation and imitation experiment: Nine out of the 13 subjects (that participated in the original action observation experiment) did an additional action observation and imitation experiment, as well as a somatomotor mapping run. The experiment was carried out using a block design format. Ten blocks were interleaved, and were repeated 6 times, in two different runs. Each block lasted 12 s followed by a blank period of 9 s. The first and last blank periods were longer (21 and 15 s, respectively). The clips were presented in the middle of the screen and subtended 20*15 degrees. The color of the fixation point indicated the task: red fixation point indicated a perceptual task, in which the subjects had to covertly assess if the current hand action is the same as the previous one. Green fixation point served as a cue for an imitation task the subjects had to imitate the observed grasping movement with their right hand. Due to their supine posture, the subjects could not see their hands. Two control conditions were introduced. In the visual control red fixation the subjects observed a scrambled version of the clips. In the motor execution control, the subjects observed the scrambled version of the clips, and had to open and close their palm according to the dimming of the green

fixation point. The fixation point dimmed 12 times during the block, as the required frequency of imitation in the action observation blocks. Somatomotor mapping During this session, subjects had to manipulate a cube with their right or left hand for 12 seconds, according to aural instructions. The subjects focused on a fixation point at the center of the screen throughout the experiment. Due to the supine posture of the subjects, they could not see their actions. Data analysis Data analysis was performed using the BrainVoyager Qx software package (Brain Innovation, Maastricht, The Netherlands, 2000). For each scan, the 2D functional data were examined for motion and signal artifacts. Head motion correction, high-pass temporal smoothing in the frequency domain (cutoff frequency: 3 cycles/ total scan time), and slice scan time correction were applied in order to remove drifts and to improve the signal-to-noise ratio. The complete data set was transformed into Talairach space, Z normalized and concatenated. A general linear model (GLM) approach was used to generate statistical parametric maps; the hemodynamic response function was modeled using a two-gamma hemodynamic response function. Significance levels were calculated, taking into account the probability of a false detection for any given cluster, by requiring that statistically significant activation is seen in contiguous groups of voxels. The implemented method is based on the approach of Forman and colleagues 1 and accomplished by Monte Carlo simulations (using a BV QX plug-in). Across-subject statistical parametric maps (Fig. 1 and 3, Sup. Fig. 1) were calculated using a hierarchical random-effects model analysis 2. The cortical surface was reconstructed from the T1-weighted isovoxel scan. The procedure included segmentation of the white matter using a grow-region function, the smooth covering of a sphere around the segmented region, and the expansion of the reconstructed white matter into the gray matter. Region of interest analysis

The multi-subject ROIs were identified in the parietal lobe based on their significant preference for observation of the contralateral hand, when shown from an egocentric point of view (compared to observation of the ipsilateral hand from the same viewpoint, P<0.05, cluster-size correction). The activation time course of individual subjects was obtained from the ROIs. The time course of each run was Z-normalized. For each subject, beta weights assigned to each condition were calculated based on a GLM analysis. These were then averaged across subjects, to yield the shown bar histograms (Fig. 2 and 3, Sup. Fig. 1,2,4). Paired two-tailed t-test was applied to test for significant differences between conditions. ANOVA for repeated measures was applied to calculate the significance of the different effects (hand identity and viewpoint) and the interaction between them. Reference 1. Forman, S. D., Cohen, J. D., Fitzgerald, M., Eddy, W. F., Mintun, M. A., Noll, D. C. Magn. Reson. Med. 33. 636 647 (1995). 2. Friston, K. J., Holmes, A. P., Worsley, K. J. Neuroimage 10. 1-5 (1999).