Contrast sensitivity in the presence of a glare light. Theoretical concepts and preliminary clinical studies. L.-E. Paulsson and J.

Similar documents
Retinal stray light originating from intraocular lenses and its effect on visual performance van der Mooren, Marie Huibert

PERIMETRY A STANDARD TEST IN OPHTHALMOLOGY

Visual Requirements for High-Fidelity Display 1

Vision Science I Exam 2 31 October 2016

Lighting Terminologies Introduction

Simple method of determining the axial length of the eye

APPENDIX GLOSSARY OF TERMS

ophthalmoscope in eyes with cataract

Visibility, Performance and Perception. Cooper Lighting

Electrical Illumination and Design

Choices and Vision. Jeffrey Koziol M.D. Friday, December 7, 12

QUANTITATIVE STUDY OF VISUAL AFTER-IMAGES*

fringes were produced on the retina directly. Threshold contrasts optical aberrations in the eye. (Received 12 January 1967)

Introduction to Lighting

Choices and Vision. Jeffrey Koziol M.D. Thursday, December 6, 12

Photometry for Traffic Engineers...

Measurement of Visual Resolution of Display Screens

Visual Perception of Images

Thomas G. Cleary Building and Fire Research Laboratory National Institute of Standards and Technology Gaithersburg, MD U.S.A.

Section 2 concludes that a glare meter based on a digital camera is probably too expensive to develop and produce, and may not be simple in use.

Efficacy of the Pelli-Levi Dual Acuity Chart in diagnosing amblyopia

BAT Brightness Acuity Tester. Product No. BAT Instruction Manual

4K Resolution, Demystified!

Neural contrast sensitivity measurements with a laser interference system for clinical and screening application

P-35: Characterizing Laser Speckle and Its Effect on Target Detection

OCULAR MEDIA* PHOTOGRAPHIC RECORDING OF OPACITIES OF THE. development by the control of diabetes, the supply of a deficient hormone

CCVIP Early Intervention Pearls

Photometry for Traffic Engineers...

Vision and Visibility. Human Eye. Eye Components. Cones and Rods. Typical Vision Impairments. CVEN 457 & 696 Lecture #3 Gene Hawkins

Retinal stray light originating from intraocular lenses and its effect on visual performance van der Mooren, Marie Huibert

Module 3. Illumination Systems. Version 2 EE IIT, Kharagpur 1

scotopic, or rod, vision, and precise information about the photochemical

Raise your expectations. Deliver theirs.

Recovery of Foveal Dark Adaptation

(Day)light Metrics. Dr.- Ing Jan Wienold. epfl.ch Lab URL: EPFL ENAC IA LIPID

The Appearance of Images Through a Multifocal IOL ABSTRACT. through a monofocal IOL to the view through a multifocal lens implanted in the other eye

Radiometric and Photometric Measurements with TAOS PhotoSensors

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Refract Surg. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2007 January 8.

Measurement of Visual Resolution of Display Screens

NFMS THEORY LIGHT AND COLOR MEASUREMENTS AND THE CCD-BASED GONIOPHOTOMETER. Presented by: January, 2015 S E E T H E D I F F E R E N C E

Work environment. Retina anatomy. A human eyeball is like a simple camera! The way of vision signal. Directional sensitivity. Lighting.

Report. Evaluating Diffusion of Light in the Eye by Objective Means Gerald Westheimer and Junzhong Liang

07-Lighting Concepts. EE570 Energy Utilization & Conservation Professor Henry Louie

ORIGINAL ARTICLE. Correlation between Optical and Psychophysical Parameters as a Function of Defocus

Practice Problems for Chapter 25-26

Dr. Magda Rau Eye Clinic Cham, Germany

Normal Wavefront Error as a Function of Age and Pupil Size

Chapter 34 The Wave Nature of Light; Interference. Copyright 2009 Pearson Education, Inc.

Lighting for seniors

Spatial coding: scaling, magnification & sampling

Work environment. Vision. Human Millieu system. Retina anatomy. A human eyeball is like a simple camera! Lighting. Eye anatomy. Cones colours

Physics of Light. Light: electromagnetic radiation that can produce a visual sensation.

A reduction of visual fields during changes in the background image such as while driving a car and looking in the rearview mirror

ORIGINAL ARTICLE. Vision Evaluation of Eccentric Refractive Correction. LINDA LUNDSTRÖM, PhD, JÖRGEN GUSTAFSSON, OD, PhD, and PETER UNSBO, PhD

Image formation in fundus cameras

Optoliner NV. Calibration Standard for Sighting & Imaging Devices West San Bernardino Road West Covina, California 91790

Evaluation of High Intensity Discharge Automotive Forward Lighting

10.2 Images Formed by Lenses SUMMARY. Refraction in Lenses. Section 10.1 Questions

KODAK VISION Expression 500T Color Negative Film / 5284, 7284

Spectral and Temporal Factors Associated with Headlight Glare: Implications for Measurement

College, Cambridge. (Three Figures in Text.)

SUSPENSION CRITERIA FOR IMAGE MONITORS AND VIEWING BOXES.

This question addresses OPTICAL factors in image formation, not issues involving retinal or other brain structures.

Light and sight. Sight is the ability for a token to "see" its surroundings

Human Vision and Human-Computer Interaction. Much content from Jeff Johnson, UI Wizards, Inc.

Visual computation of surface lightness: Local contrast vs. frames of reference

Spatial pooling of contrast in contrast gain control

NEW THE WORLD S FIRST AND ONLY SINUSOIDAL TRIFOCAL IOL

Degradation of vision through a simulated cataract. Joel L. Zuckerman, David Miller, William Dyes, and Michael Keller

Spherical and irregular aberrations are important for the optimal performance of the human eye

Lumen lm 1 lm= 1cd 1sr The luminous flux emitted into unit solid angle (1 sr) by an isotropic point source having a luminous intensity of 1 candela

STUDY NOTES UNIT I IMAGE PERCEPTION AND SAMPLING. Elements of Digital Image Processing Systems. Elements of Visual Perception structure of human eye

BLUE LIGHT: MIND YOUR PATIENT S EYES... AND YOURS!

APPLICATION OF VIDEOPHOTOMETER IN THE EVALUATION OF DGI IN SCHOLASTIC ENVIRONMENT

American National Standard for Ophthalmics. Extended Depth of Focus Intraocular Lenses

EFFECT OF FLUORESCENT LIGHT SOURCES ON HUMAN CONTRAST SENSITIVITY Krisztián SAMU 1, Balázs Vince NAGY 1,2, Zsuzsanna LUDAS 1, György ÁBRAHÁM 1

Colorimetry and Color Modeling

UMTRI EFFECTS OF OVERALL LOW-BEAM INTENSITY ON SEEING DISTANCE IN THE PRESENCE OF GLARE

The User Experience: Proper Image Size and Contrast

LIGHT ABSORPTION AND SCATTER IN THE HUMAN LENS

NOW. Approved for NTIOL classification from CMS Available in Quar ter Diopter Powers. Accommodating. Aberration Free. Aspheric.

Role of Mandelbaum-like effect in the differentiation of hyperopes and myopes using a hologram

Chapter 23 Study Questions Name: Class:

Effects of Photographic Gamma on Hologram Reconstructions*

Vision Science I Exam 1 23 September ) The plot to the right shows the spectrum of a light source. Which of the following sources is this

Basic Lighting Terms Glossary (Terms included in the basic lighting course are italicized and underlined)

Instruments Commonly Used For Examination of the Eye

Wide-Band Enhancement of TV Images for the Visually Impaired

Visual Effects of Light. Prof. Grega Bizjak, PhD Laboratory of Lighting and Photometry Faculty of Electrical Engineering University of Ljubljana

DEFECTS OF VISION THROUGH APHAKIC SPECTACLE LENSES*t

An Implantable Microfluidic Device for Self Monitoring of Intraocular Pressure

daylight Spring 2014 College of Architecture, Texas Tech University 1

Color Outline. Color appearance. Color opponency. Brightness or value. Wavelength encoding (trichromacy) Color appearance

FIA PERFORMANCE SPECIFICATION Motor racing circuit floodlighting for competition vehicles without headlights

Vision. The eye. Image formation. Eye defects & corrective lenses. Visual acuity. Colour vision. Lecture 3.5

Radiographic viewing conditions at Johannesburg Hospital

KODAK PRIMETIME 640T Teleproduction Film / 5620,7620

We have already discussed retinal structure and organization, as well as the photochemical and electrophysiological basis for vision.

Visual Effects of. Light. Warmth. Light is life. Sun as a deity (god) If sun would turn off the life on earth would extinct

Spatial Vision: Primary Visual Cortex (Chapter 3, part 1)

Transcription:

Contrast sensitivity in the presence of a glare light Theoretical concepts and preliminary clinical studies L.-E. Paulsson and J. Sjostrand A method is presented for quantitative measurements of the glare effect of light scattered, in the ocular media. The contrast sensitivity function is measured loith a television display system. A bright light source is introduced into the field of vision, and the resultant decrease in contrast sensitivity is measured. It is further used to calculate a scattering factor which is a direct measure of the intraocular light scattering. The scattering factor shows a marked, increase in patients with early cataracts even if their visual acuity is not affected. Key words: contrast sensitivity, glare light, intraocular light scattering The commonly used visual acuity tests measure only the spatial resolution of very fine targets at high contrast. This resembles the situation in technical optics 20 to 30 years ago, when it was common to describe the quality of an optical system, e.g., a camera lens, in terms of its ability to resolve fine details its resolving power. Since then it has been found necessary to use a more extensive description of the optical properties of a lens, and the modulation transfer function (MTF) is now in common use.' The MTF describes the transformation of contrast vs. spatial frequency of an optical system. Lately the same concept has been introduced into the field of visual optics. 2 ' 3 In recent years, several investigators have used contrast sensitivity vs. spatial frequency as a description From the Research Laboratory of Electronics, Chalmers University of Technology, and the Department of Ophthalmology, University of Goteborg, Goteborg, Sweden. This project was supported by the Swedish Medical Research Council (grant No. 02226). Submitted for publication Mar. 26, 1979. Reprint requests: L.-E. Paulsson, National Institute of Radiation Protection, S-104 01 Stockholm, Sweden. of visual performance under various experimental and clinical conditions (for review see refs. 3 to 5). In a recent study of vision through cataracts, Hess and Woo 6 have demonstrated impairment of contrast sensitivity in cataract subjects. The contrast sensitivity function when measured under ordinary illumination, however, gives only limited information relevant to the impairment of vision in daily life caused by intraocular light scattering. In order to enhance the effects of light scattering on the contrast sensitivity, we tried to measure the decrease in contrast sensitivity when a bright light source is introduced into the visual field. This procedure was tried on six patients who performed well on the standard visual acuity test and who showed only moderate decrease in the normal contrast sensitivity test, but who complained about visual impairment, especially at work. Their disabilities clearly showed up as extreme suppressions of their contrast sensitivity in the presence of a glare light. Only patients with incipient posterior subcapsular cataracts were selected for the study presented here. Five normal subjects were included as controls. A 0146-0404/80/040401+06$00.60/0 1980 Assoc. for Res. in Vis. and Ophthal., Inc. 401

402 Paulsson and Sjostrand Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. April 1980 TV-SCREEN L. *max GLARE LIGHT Fig. 1. Definitions of optical parameters. Television screen luminances = L,, iax, Lmin, and L; spatial frequency at TV screen = ; r = test disx 180 tance; x = cycle length; 1 = glare light intensity; glare illumination = E = ; equivalent veiling luminance = L^,. preliminary report of this study was presented at the Second World Congress of Ergophthalmology, 1977. Material and methods Subjects. Five normal controls (age 30 to 61 years, visual acuity to 2.0) without evidence of ocular disease and six patients with various degrees of posterior subcapsular cataracts (age 46 to 68 years, visual acuity 0.5 to 1.5) took part in the study. Refractive errors were corrected optimally for the test distance, and natural pupil was used. Apparatus. A pattern of light and dark vertical bars with a horizontally sinusoidal light distribution was generated electronically and displayed on a black and white TV screen as described by Sjostrand and Frisen. 7 The screen was masked with white cardboard to subtend a 1 x 1 angle at the eye when viewed at a distance of 5 m. The mask, which subtended 4 x 4, was illuminated to approximately the same space average luminance as the TV screen. The spatial frequency was varied between 0.7 and 38 cy/deg and the pattern contrast (defined as the modulation M = (Lmax ~ Lmin):(Lmax + Unin), where L max and Lm,,, denote the maximum and minimum luminances of the sinusoidal pattern, respectively) was varied in steps in the range 0.65 to 0.0007. The steps were individually calibrated with a photometer at low frequencies. The space average luminance of the TV screen L = (Lmax + Lm in ): 2 was 135 cd/m 2 independent of both contrast and frequency. A small 110 cd incandescent lamp, the glare light, was placed to the right of the TV screen at distances corresponding to,, or 5.4 from the center of the TV screen. The TV screen and the ambient light gave an illumination of 5.1 lux falling towards the patient. This was increased by 12.7 to 17.8 lux when the glare light was on. Only 5.4 lux of the increase falls directly from the lamp into the eye; the rest is a result of an increase in the ambient illumination. Procedure. The contrast sensitivity (reciprocal of contrast' threshold) was determined monocularly by raising the contrast from a subthreshold level at a selected spatial frequency until a pattern was just visible to the patient. Sixteen frequencies were explored twice in a random order. Thereafter the contrast sensitivity function was determined in the presence of the glare light. The entire procedure was repeated for at least one distance ( ) between the lamp and the center of the TV screen and in a few cases for all distances indicated above. In those cases where the effect of the glare light was too small to be conveniently measured, neutral density filters (transmission 0.1 or 0.01) were placed immediately in front of the screen. The TV screen luminance was thereby reduced to L = 13.5 or 1.35 cd/m 2, respectively. Theory. It is possible to describe the light scattering properties of the ocular media as an equivalent veiling luminance Le q (cd/m 2 ), expressed as a fraction 17 = Le tl /E, where E (lux) is the direct illuminance onto the eye from the glare light source and TJ, which is a function of the angle 6 between the object (the center of the TV screen) and the light source and which has the dimension (1/steradian), is a characteristic measure of the intraocular light scattering. The relevant parameters are illustrated in Fig. 1. Let Mi and M 2 denote the contrast modulation presented at the TV screen at the detection threshold without and with the glare light. Let M/ and M 2 ' be the resultant contrast modulation on the retina. Because of the conservation rule for luminances, the equivalent veiling lumi-

Volume 19 Number 4 Contrast sensitivity in glare light 403 nance L e(, can be considered either in the eye or at the TV screen (TV screen is used here). Assuming a constant relationship between M and M', e.g., M,' = k Mj we can write and M,' = k- J W _ L2min 2(L+L eq ) Ll ~ " L '""" = k M, 2L 1 1 L 2n nax /L 2L k 2m n ^2 J-ieq/J-i If we assume that psychophysical effects such as adaptation and pupil size can be neglected, we may set the threshold values equal M 2 ' = M/, and we obtain M, k- M, = k 1 + CONT RAST TlHRESHO The light scattering factor rj can then be expressed as In order to work within the contrast range of the TV, it was necessary to adjust the ratio L/E by adjusting L by means of neutral density filters. In our studies L/E = was found useful for the cataract patients whereas L/E = or 0. was needed to get reliable results for the normal subjects. Results Compared to controls, most of the cataract subjects had a loss of contrast sensitivity in the high- and/or middle-frequency range as seen in Fig. 2, with normal contrast sensitivity in the low-frequency range. In other cataract patients an impairment of the contrast sensitivity function was found over the whole frequency range in agreement with the findings detailed in ref. 6. Typical contrast sensitivity curves for one normal and one cataract patient are given in Fig. 2. In the presence of a glare light the normal subject (visual acuity ) showed an insignificant decrease in contrast sensitivity with L/E =, whereas the contrast sensitivity was markedly decreased in the patient with incipient cataract (visual acuity SPATIAL FREQUENCY cycles/deg Fig. 2. Threshold contrast (reciprocal of contrast sensitivity) at various spatial frequencies for normal Subject 3 (N) and Patient 6 with posterior capsular cataract (C), both with visual acuity. The dashed curves represent the threshold contrast when a 110 cd lamp is placed to the right of the center of the 135 cd/m 2 screen. The test distance was 5 m. ). With L/E = some sensitivity loss was found even for the normal. In both cases the glare light source was positioned 15 cm ( ) from the center of the TV screen. It was observed for many of the subjects that decrease in contrast sensitivity in the presence of the glare light was most distinct at low and medium frequencies. At high frequencies the decrease was less pronounced. The scattering factor was therefore calculated as an average value at five frequencies in the range 2 to 7 cy/deg. The results are summarized in Tables I and II. In a few cases the measurements were extended to larger angles (0 = and 5.2 ). These results are also included in Tables I and II. Discussion Our study demonstrates that the quality of vision that a patient with cataract experiences

404 Paulsson and Sjostrand Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. April 1980 Table I. Summary of contrast sensitivity decrease for normal subjects in the presence of a glare light Patient No. 1 2 3 4 5 Age 30 32 52 60 61 Visual acuity 2.0 1.5 LIE 0. 0. 0. 0. 0. Contrast* sensitivity decrease (M 2 /Mt) 1.98 3.34 2 1.46 2.07 24.1 1.22 2.24 1 1.23 3.78 17.5 1.36 3.37 18.2 = k(mi_a 5.9 5.4 2.7 5.8 3.1 2.9 7.0 4.1 5.9 4.3 Luminance of the TV was L (cd/m 2 ), and the flux upon the eye from the light source E = 5.4 lux. The angular distance between the center of the TV screen and the light source was 0 =. * Expressed as a ratio of the contrast threshold with (M 2 ) and without (MJ a glare light source. t Change in threshold contrast was in these cases too small to provide useful results, due to the stepwise variations of M, and M s. The smallest steps were 58%. The averaging over 5 frequencies (2 to 7 cy/deg) partially smoothes out the steps. cannot be assessed by visual acuity tests alone. In spite of clinically normal visual acuity ( or 20/20) a marked impairment of contrast vision can occur due to intraocular light scattering. Using a similar grating pattern without a glare light, Hess and Woo 6 have compared acuity tests with contrast thresholds for patients with uniocular senile cataract. They found evidence for two types of contrast sensitivity loss in cataract patients, one group with impairment over the whole frequency range and another with spared low-frequency vision. These results indicate that the visual impairment varies among cataract subjects with the same visual acuity. Many of the patients in the present study complained about visual problems in daily life. Most of them performed well on the standard visual acuity test and met the legal requirements for a driving license. By slit-lamp microscopy and ophthalmoscopy it was in all cases possible to find lens cataracts of various degrees; those reported here were all of the posterior subcapsular type. The obvious drawback with slit-lamp microscopy or ophthalmoscopy is the lack of a quantitative estimate possible to relate to the degree of visual impairment. One attempt to quantify the slit-lamp observations was made by Wolf and Gardiner, 8 who suggested the use of a photometer to measure the luminance of the slit-lamp image in the lens. By such arrangements it is possible to measure the amount of light which is scattered in the near-backward direction, i.e., out of the eye. The vision is, however, mainly affected by light scattered in the forward direction, toward the retina. The ratio of forward scattering to backward scattering is largely dependent on the size and distribution of the scattering elements. The backscattered light may therefore not give relevant information concerning the visual impairment of cataract patients. We have in the present paper used the retina itself as a photometer for measuring the intraocular forward light scattering. The use of sine-wave contrast sensitivity and the fact that the measured threshold values always appear as ratios in the formulas greatly reduce the influence of subjective factors. Some idea of the accuracy of the method

Volume 19 Number 4 Contrast sensitivity in glare light 405 Table II. Summary of contrast sensitivity decrease for patients with posterior capsular cataract in the presence of a glare light Patient No. Age Visual acuity e (degrees) Contrast* sensitivity decrease (MJMJ L (M 2 \ V " E U, / 6 7 8 9 10 11 46 46 56 58 66 68 0.7 1.5 0.5 0.7 5.2 21.9 3.52 32.0 9.97 4.8 3 7.26 2.15 3.93 B 2.06 2.37 1.38 523 63 775 174 95 c 157 c 73.3 26.6 34.3 9.5 C Luminance of the TV (L) was 135 cd/m 2, and the flux upon the eye from the light source was E = 5.4 lux. The angular distance between the center of the TV screen and the light source was 8 L/E =. A Expressed as a ratio of the contrast threshold with (M 2 ) and without (M t ) a glare light source. B In this case a test with L = 13.5 cd/m 2 (L/E ) was made which gave M 2 /M, = 38 and t\ = 9. c See second footnote to Table I. and of the validity of the assumptions made in the Theory section can be obtained by comparison with the previous studies on retinal light profiles from point sources. Most of these studies were summarized by Vos et al., 9 who gave an empirical relation of TJ VS. 6. '=? = FW uf< ' <81 where K = 29 and a = 2.8 for the normal eye. This relation gives r) = 5.4 for 6 =. For the normal subjects we found as an average 17 = 4.9 for the same 6. The small material and the uncertainty in 6 due to the large TV screen (1 ) may fully account for the difference. The exponent for the ^-dependence (a) was determined for the most advanced cataracts and was found to be in the range 2.0 to 2.6. This shows that the dependence of 17 on 6 does not change very much from the normal (a = 2.8) in the case of posterior subcapsular cataracts. The ^-dependence has also earlier been found to be constant with age and between individuals. 10 The large 77's for the cataract patients (range 34 to 775) can thus be ascribed mainly to an increase of the proportionality factor (K). Therefore it seems possible to use the value of 17 at a single specified angle 6 as a convenient measure of the intraocular light scattering. For our normal subjects we found that the frequency of the contrast sensitivity peak was shifted toward higher frequencies when the glare light was switched on. The shift was in the range 0 to 40%, with a mean value of 12%. The total light flux falling upon the eye changed from 5.1 lux to 17.8 lux, i.e., 3.1 times (0.5 decade) more. De Valois et al. 11 have studied the contrast sensitivity at different target luminances, and from their results we find a shift of 26% toward higher frequencies when the target luminance is increased by one decade. Our data shows an increase of about 24% per decade of total light flux, indicating that the adaptation may merely be a function of total light flux than of target luminance. The shift in peak frequency may be responsible for the observed differences between the contrast sensitivity decrease at high and low frequencies. The error which is introduced by this shift is probably partly canceled at low frequencies by a simultaneous shift upward. By using the data from ref. 11, we estimate that the total error in 17 due to adaptation would be about 5% to 15%. It should be noted that we have not paid

406 Paulsson and Sjostrand Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. April 1980 any attention at the present stage to the color differences between the TV screen and the incadescent lamp. The effect of this still remains to be explored. In conclusion we propose a relatively simple and clinically useful method for determining the degree of visual impairment due to intraocular light scattering. The method may prove useful in vision tests for occupations with high demands on visual ability, perhaps including car driving. It may also be valuable for detecting prestages of cataract in patients occupationally exposed to potential cataractogens, e.g., infrared and microwave radiation. The method may in its present stage be too cumbersome for routine tests, but the concepts outlined may be useful as a starting point for further work, especially as contrast sensitivity measurements become more and more used. We thank Dr. O. Nilsson, Dr. L. Frisen and Dr. E. Maartmann-Moe for valuable discussions and Mrs. M. Nilsson for technical assistance. We also acknowledge the opportunity to use the television equipment constructed by Dr. L. Frisen. REFERENCES 1. Brock GC: A review of current image evaluation techniques. J Photogr Sci 16:241, 1968. 2. Campbell FW and Robson IG: Application of Fourier analysis to the visibility of gratings. J Physiol (Lond) 197:551, 1968. 3. Campbell FW: The transmission of spatial information through the visual system. In The Neurosciences Third Study Program, Schmidt FO and Wooden FG, editors. Cambridge, Mass., 1974, The MIT Press, pp. 95-103. 4. Sekular R: Spatial vision. Annu Rev Psychol :195, 1974. 5. Arden FP: The importance of measuring contrast sensitivity in cases of visual disturbance. Br J Ophthalmol 62:198, 1978. 6. Hess R and Woo G: Vision through cataracts. INVEST OPHTHALMOL VIS SCI 17:428, 1978. 7. Sjostrand J and Frisen L: Contrast sensitivity in macular disease a preliminary report. Acta Ophthalmol 55:507, 1977. 8. Wolf E and Gardiner JS: Studies on the scatter of light in the dioptric media of the eye as a basis of visual glare. Acta Ophthalmol 74:338, 1965. 9. Voss JJ, Walraven J, and Van Meeteren A: Light profiles of the foveal image of a point source. Vision Res 16:215, 1976. 10. Fischer AJ and Christie AW: A note on disability glare. Vision Res 5:565, 1965. 11. De Valois RL, Morgan H, and Snodderly DM: Psychophysical studies of monkey vision. III. Spatial luminance contrast sensitivity tests of Macaque and Human observers. Vision Res. 14:75, 1974.