inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

Similar documents
Multiple Audio Spots Design Based on Separating Emission of Carrier and Sideband Waves

Perception of pitch. Definitions. Why is pitch important? BSc Audiology/MSc SHS Psychoacoustics wk 4: 7 Feb A. Faulkner.

Perception of pitch. Definitions. Why is pitch important? BSc Audiology/MSc SHS Psychoacoustics wk 5: 12 Feb A. Faulkner.

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

3D Intermodulation Distortion Measurement AN 8

Perception of pitch. Importance of pitch: 2. mother hemp horse. scold. Definitions. Why is pitch important? AUDL4007: 11 Feb A. Faulkner.

The role of intrinsic masker fluctuations on the spectral spread of masking

ANALYSIS AND EVALUATION OF IRREGULARITY IN PITCH VIBRATO FOR STRING-INSTRUMENT TONES

The Association of Loudspeaker Manufacturers & Acoustics International presents

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

Effect of the number of loudspeakers on sense of presence in 3D audio system based on multiple vertical panning

Perception of low frequencies in small rooms

Loudspeaker Distortion Measurement and Perception Part 2: Irregular distortion caused by defects

3D Distortion Measurement (DIS)

ENGINEERING STAFF REPORT. The JBL Model L40 Loudspeaker System. Mark R. Gander, Design Engineer

AUDL GS08/GAV1 Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Loudness & Temporal resolution

Complex Sounds. Reading: Yost Ch. 4

AXIHORN CP5TB: HF module for the high definition active loudspeaker system "NIDA Mk1"

Sampling and Reconstruction

IMPULSE RESPONSE MEASUREMENT WITH SINE SWEEPS AND AMPLITUDE MODULATION SCHEMES. Q. Meng, D. Sen, S. Wang and L. Hayes

Tone-in-noise detection: Observed discrepancies in spectral integration. Nicolas Le Goff a) Technische Universiteit Eindhoven, P.O.

What is Sound? Part II

An unnatural test of a natural model of pitch perception: The tritone paradox and spectral dominance

A STUDY ON NOISE REDUCTION OF AUDIO EQUIPMENT INDUCED BY VIBRATION --- EFFECT OF MAGNETISM ON POLYMERIC SOLUTION FILLED IN AN AUDIO-BASE ---

APPLICATION NOTE MAKING GOOD MEASUREMENTS LEARNING TO RECOGNIZE AND AVOID DISTORTION SOUNDSCAPES. by Langston Holland -

DESIGN OF VOICE ALARM SYSTEMS FOR TRAFFIC TUNNELS: OPTIMISATION OF SPEECH INTELLIGIBILITY

Pre- and Post Ringing Of Impulse Response

Convention Paper Presented at the 112th Convention 2002 May Munich, Germany

MUS 302 ENGINEERING SECTION

Music Instruments That Produce Sounds with Inaudible High-Frequency Components

Combining Subjective and Objective Assessment of Loudspeaker Distortion Marian Liebig Wolfgang Klippel

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

United States Patent 5,159,703 Lowery October 27, Abstract

Silent subliminal presentation system

Since the advent of the sine wave oscillator

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Audio Testing. application note. Arrakis Systems inc.

3.2 Measuring Frequency Response Of Low-Pass Filter :

DETERMINATION OF EQUAL-LOUDNESS RELATIONS AT HIGH FREQUENCIES

HARMONIC INSTABILITY OF DIGITAL SOFT CLIPPING ALGORITHMS

PROFESSIONAL. EdgeMax EM90 and EM180 In-Ceiling Loudspeakers. Design Guide

Acoustics, signals & systems for audiology. Week 9. Basic Psychoacoustic Phenomena: Temporal resolution

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007

EXPERIMENTAL AND NUMERICAL ANALYSIS OF THE MUSICAL BEHAVIOR OF TRIANGLE INSTRUMENTS

Sound waves. septembre 2014 Audio signals and systems 1

Psycho-acoustics (Sound characteristics, Masking, and Loudness)

Directivity Controllable Parametric Loudspeaker using Array Control System with High Speed 1-bit Signal Processing

datasheet TCS-40 TCS SERIES ENGINEERING INFORMATION FEATURES APPLICATIONS Dual 8 LF drivers Wide dispersion High power rating Down-fill enclosure

2920 J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 102 (5), Pt. 1, November /97/102(5)/2920/5/$ Acoustical Society of America 2920

The Effect of Frequency Shifting on Audio-Tactile Conversion for Enriching Musical Experience

6-channel recording/reproduction system for 3-dimensional auralization of sound fields

Influence of fine structure and envelope variability on gap-duration discrimination thresholds Münkner, S.; Kohlrausch, A.G.; Püschel, D.

Spatial Audio Reproduction: Towards Individualized Binaural Sound

Week 1. Signals & Systems for Speech & Hearing. Sound is a SIGNAL 3. You may find this course demanding! How to get through it:

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007

Fundamentals of Digital Audio *

Version (0) Sept. 28, LTD East 5th Street Superior, WI USA tel: fax:

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

Version 1.01 CRANE SONG LTD East 5th Street Superior, WI USA tel: fax:

A102 Signals and Systems for Hearing and Speech: Final exam answers

Causes for Amplitude Compression AN 12

Earl R. Geddes, Ph.D. Audio Intelligence

Acoustic Calibration Service in Automobile Field at NIM, China

Chapter 2. Meeting 2, Measures and Visualizations of Sounds and Signals

MUSC 316 Sound & Digital Audio Basics Worksheet

PIV STUDY OF STANDING WAVES IN A RESONANT AIR COLUMN

THE PERCEPTION OF ALL-PASS COMPONENTS IN TRANSFER FUNCTIONS

Loudspeaker Data Reliable, Comprehensive, Interpretable

Transmitter Identification Experimental Techniques and Results

Binaural Hearing. Reading: Yost Ch. 12

ULTRASOUND DETECTOR D240x OPERATING INSTRUCTIONS. Pettersson. Phone: Fax:

14 fasttest. Multitone Audio Analyzer. Multitone and Synchronous FFT Concepts

Audio Engineering Society. Convention Paper. Presented at the 122nd Convention 2007 May 5 8 Vienna, Austria

An evaluation of discomfort reduction based on auditory masking for railway brake sounds

You know about adding up waves, e.g. from two loudspeakers. AUDL 4007 Auditory Perception. Week 2½. Mathematical prelude: Adding up levels

An evaluation on comfortable sound design of unpleasant sounds based on chord-forming with bandlimited sound

XLA 3200 Line Array Loudspeakers

datasheet IMPACT 50 IMPACT SERIES ENGINEERING INFORMATION FEATURES APPLICATIONS Attractive injection moulded enclosure 100 watt power rating

Principles of Musical Acoustics

datasheet TCS-35 FEATURES APPLICATIONS Compact enclosure Wide dispersion Full range response OmniMount compatible Surround sound Background sound

datasheet TQ-315 QLIGHT SERIES ENGINEERING INFORMATION FEATURES APPLICATIONS CEW technology Trapezoidal cabinet Rotatable HF waveguide

Introduction Unique Electronics

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

The Steering for Distance Perception with Reflective Audio Spot

Speech/Music Change Point Detection using Sonogram and AANN

datasheet THL-4.3 HILIGHT SERIES ENGINEERING FEATURES APPLICATIONS Controlled dispersion Seamless mid range Ultra-low distortion Sound contracting

Reduction of Musical Residual Noise Using Harmonic- Adapted-Median Filter

40 Hz Event Related Auditory Potential

A R T A - A P P L I C A T I O N N O T E

Impulse response. Frequency response

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007 A MODEL OF THE HEAD-RELATED TRANSFER FUNCTION BASED ON SPECTRAL CUES

SGN Audio and Speech Processing

FLAMINGO.1 OPERATOR'S MANUAL. Version

Music 171: Sinusoids. Tamara Smyth, Department of Music, University of California, San Diego (UCSD) January 10, 2019

Audio Noise Reduction and Masking

Digital Signal Processing Audio Measurements Custom Designed Tools. Loudness measurement in sone (DIN ISO 532B)

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

AM404. architectual. Large format mid/high horn. features. applications

Temporal resolution AUDL Domain of temporal resolution. Fine structure and envelope. Modulating a sinusoid. Fine structure and envelope

datasheet TQ-310 QLIGHT SERIES ENGINEERING INFORMATION FEATURES APPLICATIONS CEW technology Trapezoidal cabinet Rotatable HF waveguide

Transcription:

Copyright SFA - InterNoise 2000 1 inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering 27-30 August 2000, Nice, FRANCE I-INCE Classification: 6.1 AUDIBILITY OF COMPLEX TONES ABOVE 20 KHZ K. Ashihara Electrotechnical Laboratory, 1-1-4 Umezono, 305-8568, Tsukuba, Japan Tel.: +81-298-61-5127 / Fax: +81-298-61-5592 / Email: ashihara@etl.go.jp Keywords: ULTRASONIC, AUDIBLE AREA, INTERMODULATION DISTORTION, COMPLEX TONE ABSTRACT Audibility of ultrasonic components contained in a harmonic complex tone were investigated. The level of the components varied adaptively and the maximum level investigated was 80 db SPL / component. All subjects distinguished between sounds with and without ultrasounds only when the stimulus was presented through a single loudspeaker. When the stimulus was divided into six bands of frequencies and presented through 6 loudspeakers in order to reduce intermodulation distortions of loudspeakers, no subject could detect any ultrasounds. It was concluded that ultrasonic components that were inaudible as a single tone could not have significant influence on impression of complex sounds. 1 - INTRODUCTION The audible area has been believed to range between 20 Hz and 20 khz. It has been reported that the detection threshold for tonal stimuli starts to raise quite abruptly when the frequency of the tone exceeds about 15 khz and it reaches 80 db SPL at the frequency of 20 khz [1], [2]. In the last decade, however, several groups of researchers have used complex sounds (pulse train, musical sounds, environmental sounds, etc.) as the stimuli and claimed that subjective impression of sounds could be significantly affected by addition of ultrasounds [3], [4], [5]. Possible influence of ultrasounds does not seem to be considered in the conventional minimum auditory field and equal loudness contours. If the ultrasonic components could have significant influence on impression of complex sounds, it might be necessary to reconsider the concept of the minimum auditory field and equal loudness contours. If the ultrasounds could affect sound impression only when they were contained in complex sounds, it can be suspected that the influence is due to some non-linear interactions that take place somewhere between the sound source and the auditory cortex. They can occur in the air between the sound source and eardrums, and somewhere in the auditory system. They may also occur at the sound source including amplifiers and loudspeakers. If the interactions would occur in the air or in the auditory system, it may be at least practically appropriate to say that ultrasounds are audible. If they would occur at the sound source, they were merely the experimental artifact and should have been eliminated by somehow. In the previous studies, however, possible effects caused by non-linear characteristics of the equipment were not sufficiently discussed. In the present study, not only the audibility of ultrasounds but also the effects possibly caused by experimental artifacts were investigated. 2 - METHOD 2.1 - Stimuli A harmonic complex tone consisted of only odd number harmonics was used as the stimulus. Its fundamental frequency (f0) was 2 khz. The sound spectrogram of the stimuli is shown in Fig. 1. The lower 5 components that were the fundamental, 3rd, 5th, 7th and 9th harmonics were defined as non-target components and the higher 5 components that were the 11th, 13th, 15th, 17th and 19th harmonics were defined as target components. Frequency of the highest harmonics was 38 khz. The duration of the stimulus was 2000 ms including linear onset and offset ramps of 100 ms each. The question was if subjects could discriminate between stimuli with and without the target components. In order to make discrimination easy, only the target components were modulated by a sinusoid of 2 Hz.

Copyright SFA - InterNoise 2000 2 Figure 1: Sound spectrogram of the stimuli. If the ultrasonic components were audible, subjects would perceive fluctuation of 2 Hz only when there were the target components. The level of the non-target components was fixed at 60 db SPL while that of the target components varied adaptively so that the discrimination threshold was estimated. In order to investigate effects caused by intermodulation distortions of loudspeakers, the discrimination threshold was estimated in two different conditions. The one was called the single-loudspeaker condition and the other was the six-loudspeaker condition. These conditions are illustrated in Fig. 2. Only one loudspeaker (loudspeaker A) was used to present stimuli in the single-loudspeaker condition. In this case, addition of the target components increased amplitude of a total input to the loudspeaker and might induce intermodulation distortions of the loudspeaker. Six loudspeakers were used in the other condition. The non-target components were presented through the first loudspeaker and each target component was presented through each of the other 5 loudspeakers (loudspeaker B, C, D, E and F), so that no intermodulation would be induced by addition of the target components. Figure 2: Single-loudspeaker condition and six-loudspeaker condition. 2.2 - Equipment and subjects Six loudspeakers were arranged in 2 vertical and 3 horizontal rows at the distance of 150 cm from the listening point as illustrated in Fig. 3. All of them were Victor SX-V05. All stimuli were synthesized at a sampling rate of 88.2 khz and 16-bit resolution. They were generated by 3 stereo D/A converters (Sek d ADDA2496S). Three stereo amplifiers were used, two of which were Luxman L-507s and the other one was Accuphase E-406. Ten males and 3 females participated as the subjects. All of them were either undergraduate or graduate students and had normal hearing. Their ages ranged between 19 and 26 years. They were paid for their

Copyright SFA - InterNoise 2000 3 participation. None of them could detect any stimulus above 22 khz when it was presented as a single tone and its level did not exceed 85 db SPL. 2.3 - Procedure A three-interval two-alternative forced-choice paradigm was adopted with a three-down, one-up transformed up-down method that tracked 79.4% correct [6]. The reference stimulus was presented in the first interval and was followed by two test stimuli. The reference and one of the test stimuli always contained the target components while the other test stimulus did not. There were silent intervals of 300 ms between each stimulus. The subjects were required to judge which test stimulus was the same as the reference stimulus. A visual feedback was given immediately after every response. The level of the target components was 72 db SPL / component at the beginning of each run. It decreased after three consecutive correct responses and increased after every wrong response. A single run consisted of 10 reversals. The discrimination threshold was defined as the mean level at the last 6 reversals. The minimum step size was 1 db. If the level of the target components exceeded 80 db SPL before 10 reversals were completed, the run automatically terminated and no estimation was made. Figure 3: Loudspeakers and the subject. 3 - RESULTS AND DISCUSSION The discrimination threshold in the present experiment was the level at which 79.4% correct response was achieved. The discrimination threshold did not exceed 70 db SPL in the single-loudspeaker condition. The average threshold in this condition was 65.76 db SPL while no threshold below 80 db SPL was obtained in the 6-loudspeaker condition. In order to investigate why addition of the target components had significant influence on the sound impression only in the single-loudspeaker condition, the stimuli were acoustically analyzed via a 1/2 inch microphone (B&K 4133). Fig. 4 shows the power-spectrum of the stimulus without the target components. Fig. 5 and Fig. 6 show the stimuli with the target components in the single-loudspeaker condition and the 6-loudspeaker condition, respectively. The even number harmonics were observed even in the audible area (indicated by an arrow) only in the single-loudspeaker condition (Fig. 4). They seem to be intermodulation distortions of the loudspeaker because they were not observed in the 6-loudspeaker condition (Fig. 5). There have been controversy about audibility of ultrasounds. Muraoka et al. [7] used musical sounds and reported that only a few out of 176 subjects distinguished the sounds with and without components above 20 khz. Several recent studies, on the other hand, revealed that the ultrasonic components would significantly affect the sound impression of the normal listeners. In the present study, audibility of the components above 22 khz was investigated under two conditions in order to observe effects caused by the experimental artifacts.

Copyright SFA - InterNoise 2000 4 Figure 4: Power-spectrum of the stimuli without target components. Figure 5: Stimuli with the target components in the single-loudspeaker condition. If the components above 22 khz were audible, the subjects should have discriminated the stimuli with and without the target components in both experimental conditions. However, they discriminated only in the single-loudspeaker condition where the intermodulation distortions were induced by addition of the target components. The linearity of the loudspeaker used here was about 50 db when the input was 1 W. This value is average or even better than average consumer loudspeakers [8]. It was indicated from the results that the non-linear interaction of ultrasounds in the air or in the auditory system was, if any, not so much as that in the average loudspeakers as far as the level of the signal did not exceed 80 db SPL. Components above 22 khz did not significantly affect the impression of a complex tone when the experimental artifacts were adequately eliminated in the present experiment. In this study, however, only a synthesized complex tone was used and no data have been available for musical sounds nor natural environmental sounds. It requires further investigations in which great care has to be taken to remove artifacts due to non-linear characteristics of the equipment especially the loudspeakers. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS A part of this study was supported by the NEDO International Joint Research Grant Program. REFERENCES 1. S.A.Fausti et al., System for evaluating auditory function from 8, 000-20, 000 Hz, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 66, pp. 1713-1718, 1979 2. P.G.Stelmachowicz et al., Normative thresholds in the 8- to 20-kHz range as a function of age, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 86, pp. 1384-1391, 1989 3. H.Sato et al., Evaluation of sensitivity of the frequency range higher than the audible frequency range, Pioneer R&D, Vol. 7(2), pp. 10-16, 1997 4. S.Yoshikawa et al., Does high sampling frequency improve perceptual time-axis resolution of digital audio signal?, In 103rd AES convention, 1997 5. K.Itoh et al., Frequency analysis of natural sound by 96kHz DAT, Transactions of Technical Committee on Musical Acoustics, Vol. MA-98-22, 1998 6. H.Levitt, Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, Vol. 49, pp. 467-477, 1971

Copyright SFA - InterNoise 2000 5 Figure 6: Stimuli with the target components in the 6-loudspeaker condition. 7. T.Muraoka et al., Examination of audio-bandwidth requirements for optimum sound signal transmission, Journal of the Audio Engineering Society, Vol. 29, pp. 2-9, 1981 8. K.Ashihara and S.Kiryu, Influence of expanded frequency band of signals on non-linear characteristics of loudspeakers, Journal of the Acoustical Society of Japan (in print), 2000