Illusory displacement of equiluminous kinetic edges

Similar documents
Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion

Background stripes affect apparent speed of rotation

Chapter 8: Perceiving Motion

IOC, Vector sum, and squaring: three different motion effects or one?

The peripheral drift illusion: A motion illusion in the visual periphery

Human Vision and Human-Computer Interaction. Much content from Jeff Johnson, UI Wizards, Inc.

Simple Figures and Perceptions in Depth (2): Stereo Capture

Perceived depth is enhanced with parallax scanning

Chapter 73. Two-Stroke Apparent Motion. George Mather

PERCEIVING MOVEMENT. Ways to create movement

B.A. II Psychology Paper A MOVEMENT PERCEPTION. Dr. Neelam Rathee Department of Psychology G.C.G.-11, Chandigarh

Self-motion perception from expanding and contracting optical flows overlapped with binocular disparity

Perception. What We Will Cover in This Section. Perception. How we interpret the information our senses receive. Overview Perception

Takeharu Seno 1,3,4, Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2, Stephen Palmisano 5 1

Beau Lotto: Optical Illusions Show How We See

Measurements of Geometric Illusions, Illusory Contours and Stereo-depth at Luminance and Colour Contrast

7Motion Perception. 7 Motion Perception. 7 Computation of Visual Motion. Chapter 7

Vision V Perceiving Movement

the dimensionality of the world Travelling through Space and Time Learning Outcomes Johannes M. Zanker

Vision V Perceiving Movement

Perception: From Biology to Psychology

CYCLOPEAN MOTION AFTEREFFECTS USING SPIRAL PATTERNS: DISSOCIATION BETWEEN LOCAL AND GLOBAL PROCESSING JASON ALAN ROGERS

Limitations of the Oriented Difference of Gaussian Filter in Special Cases of Brightness Perception Illusions

vertical horizonta fovea Figure by MIT OCW.

Vision Research 48 (2008) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage:

PERCEIVING MOTION CHAPTER 8

The eye, displays and visual effects

The cyclopean (stereoscopic) barber pole illusion

Object Perception. 23 August PSY Object & Scene 1

Bottom-up and Top-down Perception Bottom-up perception

Visual Effects of Light. Prof. Grega Bizjak, PhD Laboratory of Lighting and Photometry Faculty of Electrical Engineering University of Ljubljana

Vision. Definition. Sensing of objects by the light reflected off the objects into our eyes

Visual computation of surface lightness: Local contrast vs. frames of reference

Apparent depth with motion aftereffect and head movement

Footsteps and inchworms: Illusions show that contrast affects apparent speed

The occlusion illusion: Partial modal completion or apparent distance?

Stereoscopic occlusion and the aperture problem for motion: a new solution 1

Monocular occlusion cues alter the influence of terminator motion in the barber pole phenomenon

Munker ^ White-like illusions without T-junctions

Stereoscopic Depth and the Occlusion Illusion. Stephen E. Palmer and Karen B. Schloss. Psychology Department, University of California, Berkeley

THE BASIS OF AREA AND DOT NUMBER EFFECTS IN RANDOM DOT MOTION PERCEPTION

Sensation. Our sensory and perceptual processes work together to help us sort out complext processes

the human chapter 1 Traffic lights the human User-centred Design Light Vision part 1 (modified extract for AISD 2005) Information i/o

Unit IV: Sensation & Perception. Module 19 Vision Organization & Interpretation

For Peer Review Journal of Vision -

Visual Effects of. Light. Warmth. Light is life. Sun as a deity (god) If sun would turn off the life on earth would extinct

The Persistence of Vision in Spatio-Temporal Illusory Contours formed by Dynamically-Changing LED Arrays

AD-A lji llllllllllii l

Design III CRAFTS SUPPLEMENT

TRAFFIC SIGN DETECTION AND IDENTIFICATION.

The shape of luminance increments at the intersection alters the magnitude of the scintillating grid illusion

Factors affecting curved versus straight path heading perception

Introduction to Psychology Prof. Braj Bhushan Department of Humanities and Social Sciences Indian Institute of Technology, Kanpur

Lecture Outline. Basic Definitions

Behavioural Realism as a metric of Presence

UC Irvine UC Irvine Previously Published Works

The Influence of Visual Illusion on Visually Perceived System and Visually Guided Action System

The human visual system

Detection of external stimuli Response to the stimuli Transmission of the response to the brain

70 The Fraser-Wilcox illusion and its extension

Outline 2/21/2013. The Retina

Moving Cast Shadows and the Perception of Relative Depth

Vision. The eye. Image formation. Eye defects & corrective lenses. Visual acuity. Colour vision. Lecture 3.5

Analysis of Gaze on Optical Illusions

Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones.

CS 544 Human Abilities

Why hearts flutter: distorted dim motions

Simple reaction time as a function of luminance for various wavelengths*

Lecture 3: Grey and Color Image Processing

Perception. Selective Attention focus of conscious awareness on a particular stimulus. Cocktail Party Effect

Perception of scene layout from optical contact, shadows, and motion

Vision: How does your eye work? Student Advanced Version Vision Lab - Overview

Perceiving binocular depth with reference to a common surface

Vision: How does your eye work? Student Version

THE POGGENDORFF ILLUSION WITH ANOMALOUS SURFACES: MANAGING PAC-MANS, PARALLELS LENGTH AND TYPE OF TRANSVERSAL.

Demonstrations of spatio-temporal integration and what they tell us about the visual system

On the intensity maximum of the Oppel-Kundt illusion

Abstract shape: a shape that is derived from a visual source, but is so transformed that it bears little visual resemblance to that source.

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Report

Chapter 3. Adaptation to disparity but not to perceived depth

Leonardo s Constraint: Two Opaque Objects Cannot Be Seen in the Same Direction

Lecture 4 Foundations and Cognitive Processes in Visual Perception From the Retina to the Visual Cortex

Modulation of perceived contrast by a moving surround

First-order structure induces the 3-D curvature contrast effect

The fragile edges of. block averaged portraits

Gestalt Principles of Visual Perception

AP PSYCH Unit 4.2 Vision 1. How does the eye transform light energy into neural messages? 2. How does the brain process visual information? 3.

Thinking About Psychology: The Science of Mind and Behavior 2e. Charles T. Blair-Broeker Randal M. Ernst

Brightness with and without perceived transparency: When does it make a difference?

Learned Stimulation in Space and Motion Perception

Psych 333, Winter 2008, Instructor Boynton, Exam 1

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I

T-junctions in inhomogeneous surrounds

The Perception of Apparent Motion

Vision. Sensation & Perception. Functional Organization of the Eye. Functional Organization of the Eye. Functional Organization of the Eye

Peripheral Color Vision and Motion Processing

Spatial Judgments from Different Vantage Points: A Different Perspective

Vision: Distance & Size Perception

Depth-dependent contrast gain-control

Perceptual Organization

Transcription:

Perception, 1990, volume 19, pages 611-616 Illusory displacement of equiluminous kinetic edges Vilayanur S Ramachandran, Stuart M Anstis Department of Psychology, C-009, University of California at San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA Received 20 May 1989, in revised form 2 August 1990 Abstract. A stationary window was cut out of a stationary random-dot pattern. When a field of dots was moved continuously behind the window (a) the window appeared to move in the same direction even though it was stationary, (b) the position of the 'kinetic edges' defining the window was also displaced along the direction of dot motion, and (c) the edges of the window tended to fade on steady fixation even though the dots were still clearly visible. The illusory displacement was enhanced considerably if the kinetic edge was equiluminous and if the 'window' region was seen as 'figure' rather than 'ground'. Since the extraction of kinetic edges probably involves the use of direction-selective cells, the illusion may provide insights into how the visual system uses the output of these cells to localize the kinetic edges. 1 Introduction Most object boundaries in the real world are defined by a luminance difference across their border. However, in many cases edges can be defined without such luminance differences, and these equiluminous edges have interesting properties (Ramachandran and Gregory 1978; Livingstone and Hubel 1987; Troscianko 1989, personal communication). Examples include: an equiluminous chromatic edge, where two areas of the same luminance but different hues meet; or a texture edge such as the border between fine and coarse textures of the same mean luminance; or a kinetic edge (Julesz 1971; Nakayama 1983) such as the border between two random-dot textures which are identical save that one is moving and the other is stationary. We have recently observed that equiluminous texture borders and kinetic edges tend to fade completely on prolonged steady fixation even though the elements defining the edges remain clearly visible (Ramachandran 1988; 1989; Ramachandran and Gregory 1991). Perhaps the fading occurs because of selective adaptation or fatigue of neural channels that are specialized for extracting such edges (Albright 1987). Since most object boundaries in the world have luminance edges associated with them, one wonders why the visual system takes the trouble of extracting all these different kinds of edges. One possibility is that using multiple mechanisms allows the visual system to defeat camouflage, and to cope with 'noisy' images (Ramachandran 1990). For example, consider a leopard seen against a screen of fluttering foliage while chasing one of our arboreal ancestors. The leopard may be completely invisible when it is standing still but as soon as it starts moving its shape becomes instantly visible as a result of the kinetic edges associated with its borders. While studying kinetic edges we came across a striking new visual illusion that forms the subject of this paper. To create the edges, we began with a stationary window cut out of a stationary random-dot pattern. Behind the window we displayed a random-dot pattern 'conveyor belt' that moved continuously to the right. Surprisingly, the moving dots appeared to drag the window with them so that it appeared to move in the same direction an example of a class of illusions that we call 'motion capture' (Ramachandran and Inada 1985; Ramachandran 1985, 1987). It was almost as though the strong luminance-based motion signals from the drifting dots were being misapplied to the edges of the stationary window, so that they appeared to

612 V S Ramachandran, S M Anstis move as well. Furthermore, the window also appeared to be displaced to the right (Ramachandran and Anstis 1987). This striking positional displacement occurred optimally at equiluminance, that is, when the moving and static regions shared the same mean luminance. 2 Experiment 1: Drifting dots affect perceived position Stimuli were displayed on a monitor screen, 25 deg widexl8deg high, under the control of a microcomputer, and were viewed from a distance of 57 cm in a dimly lit room. The entire screen was filled with sparse grey random dots on a black background. Each dot (pixel) subtended 4.7 min arc. A central fixation spot was provided. Four small square windows, each of side 1.5 deg, were arranged at the corners of a larger square of side 6 deg which was centered on the fixation point. Each square window was stationary, but contained a field of sparse random grey dots that continually appeared at one edge of the window, drifted horizontally across it at a speed of 2.3 deg s _1, and disappeared. About twenty-four grey dots were visible within a window at any one time. The edges of the windows were defined only by motion; if the dots were stopped the windows vanished. The dots in the upper two windows drifted inward toward the midline, while the dots in the lower two windows drifted outward away from the midline (figure 1). It was found that the square array looked trapezoidal, with the two upper windows appearing closer together than the two bottom windows. In other words the static positions of the windows appeared to be displaced in the direction of the dot drifts. The display was shown to sixty-two naive subjects who were unaware of the purpose of the experiment, and all of them reported seeing the displacement. The perceived spatial offset is not an artifact of the mean position of sparse dots within each window over time, because when the dot drift direction was reversed by reversing the time sequence (keeping the spatial patterns the same) the perceived offset was in the opposite direction. This apparent static misalignment was measured by a null method on twelve naive subjects. Note that the dots in the top left and bottom right windows in figure 1 Figure 1. The stimulus used in the experiment. The four small squares were actually windows cut out of a stationary random-dot pattern through which one could see dots moving either towards the midline (upper two windows) or away from the midline (lower two windows) as indicated by the arrows. This caused the static positions of the windows to appear displaced in the direction of the moving dots. Note that the margins of the windows are visible only as a result of relative motion, so that if the moving dots were stationary the windows would become invisible. The outlines of the windows shown in the illustration here were not present in the original display.

Illusory displacement of equiluminous kinetic edges 613 drifted to the right; these two windows were always kept in a fixed position on the screen. The dots in the other two windows (bottom left and top right) drifted to the left, so these two windows both appeared to be statically displaced to the left. The subjects were able to adjust the position of this pair of windows on the screen by moving a hand-operated mouse. By moving the mouse to the left (or right) they could make the separation between the two upper windows greater (or smaller) than the separation between the two lower windows. Each subject adjusted the mouse, maintaining central fixation, until satisfied that the separations were equal such that the four windows lay at the corners of an imaginary square. The setting was then recorded. To avoid any positional bias the entire display was mirror-reversed top to bottom in half the trials. The dots in the windows were always grey (7 cd m~ 2 ). However, three different surround conditions were used; the surround dots were either grey (7 cd m~ 2 ), white (15 cd m~ 2 ), or black, that is, invisible. Twenty readings per subject were obtained for each background luminance setting. We found that the windows always appeared to be displaced in the direction of the dot drift (figure 2). Each datum point on the graph represents the mean of two hundred and forty readings (twelve subjects and twenty readings). The apparent static displacement of the kinetic windows was 26 min arc with grey surround dots, 20.5 min arc with white surround dots, and 7.5 min arc with no surround dots. Since these are the perceived offsets between two kinetic windows moving in opposite directions, a single window compared to a stationary landmark would show half these values. Thus the apparent displacement was greatest when the surround dots were grey, the same as the drifting dots, so that the kinetic edges were defined only by motion. The apparent displacement was less for white or black surround dots, against which the kinetic windows stood out as darker or brighter so that their positions could be seen independently of motion information. 35 30 A 8 25 20 1 15 10 5 15 Background luminance/cd m -2 Figure 2. Illusory displacement between upper and lower squares plotted against background luminance. The square luminance was always held constant at 7 cd m~ 2. The solid line represents the graph for experiment 1 in which the background dots were stationary, whereas the dotted line represents the graph for experiment 3 (twinkling background). Notice that illusory displacement is greatest at equiluminace. Each datum point on the solid line graph represents the mean of two hundred and forty readings (twelve subjects, twenty readings each). For the dotted line graph each datum point represents the mean of one hundred and twenty readings (six subjects, twenty readings each).

614 V S Ramachandran, S M Anstis 3 Experiment 2: Drifting dots affect perceived size To rule out eye movements as a possible explanation, we created a display consisting of two annular windows containing radially drifting dots. The annuli were side by side with their centers 5.6 deg to the left and right of a fixation spot. The inner diameter of each annulus was 3.9 deg and the outer diameter was 7 deg. The dots in the left annulus drifted out radially from the center of the annulus in an expanding pattern, and the dots in the right annulus drifted inward toward the center in a contracting pattern. As a result it was found that the contracting annulus looked subjectively smaller than the expanding annulus, even though both annuli were the same size. Five subjects (three of whom were naive) were asked to null out this apparent size difference by adjusting the actual size of one annulus using a hand operated mouse until both annuli looked the same size. Subjects adjusted the expanding annulus in half the trials and the contracting annulus in the others. When the expanding annulus was adjusted its diameter was 7.8% smaller than the contracting one. When the contracting annulus was adjusted its diameter was 8.1% larger than the expanding one. Since the eyeballs cannot contract or expand (Nakayama 1988, personal communication) we conclude that the illusory displacement of kinetic edges cannot be explained by eye movements. 4 Experiment 3: Background of twinkling dynamic noise This experiment was identical to experiment 1 except that we replaced the stationary dots in the background with twinkling dynamic noise. The number of dots per unit area was identical inside and outside the windows in any one movie frame. Again, the frame alternation rate was identical inside and outside the windows, so that if the 'movie' was stopped no windows were seen. We measured the perceived displacement of the windows in six additional naive subjects and found that the degree of mislocalization was even greater than in experiment 1 (see figure 2, dotted lines). Why is the illusory displacement slightly less in experiment 1 (stationary dots background) than in experiment 3 (twinkling dots background)? One possibility is that stationary dots generate strong position signals which provide a frame of reference for the visual system when judging the location of the illusory edges. The position signals are removed by using dynamic noise background, and this enhances the illusory displacement. 5 Experiment 4: Titchener circles Titchener (1902) showed that a test circle looks apparently smaller when it is surrounded by large inducing circles than when it is surrounded by small circles. We produced a Titchener illusion by manipulating only the subjective size of the inducing circles as described in experiment 2. Two identical test annuli were filled with twinkling random noise against a sparse random-dot background. The left test annulus was surrounded by six inducing annuli which contained expanding drifting dots, and which therefore appeared larger than they really were. The right test annulus was surrounded by six inducing annuli which contained contracting drifting dots, and therefore appeared smaller than they really were. In fact, all fourteen annuli had the same physical diameter. Result: The left test annulus looked slightly smaller than the right test annulus. Thus the illusory size change produced by the dot drift was able in its turn to induce a secondary illusory size change in the test annuli. More formal experiments are needed to determine the magnitude of the effect.

Illusory displacement of equiluminous kinetic edges 615 6 Discussion The observations described here are closely related to a class of effects that we call motion capture (Ramachandran 1987; Ramachandran and Inada 1985; Ramachandran and Anstis 1986). In a recent study of motion capture (Ramachandran 1987), a stationary yellow square was superimposed on an equiluminous grey surround, and viewed in peripheral vision. A sparse pattern of coarse black dots was optically superimposed on this yellow square and moved back and forth across it at about 0.5 Hz. The square appeared to move vividly along with the dots even though it was always stationary. Since equiluminous chromatic borders excite the motion system only weakly (Ramachandran and Gregory 1978; Cavanagh et al 1985; Livingstone and Hubel 1987; Ramachandran 1987; Schiller et al 1990) the visual system has no way of 'knowing' that the yellow square has not moved. Consequently the square is 'assumed' to have moved in the same direction as the dots (Ramachandran 1987). If the yellow square is given a different luminance from the grey background the motion capture collapses. So, equiluminous chromatic edges can undergo motion capture from nearby (nonequiluminous) moving objects. We believe that the illusory motion of the kinetic edges is caused by a similar sequence of events, in which strong motion signals derived from moving dots get misapplied to the kinetic edge so that it appears to move as well (figure 1). But what causes the position of these kinetic edges to be displaced in the direction of the drifting dots? One possibility is that the location attributed to an edge that stimulates a direction selective cell is offset in its preferred direction. This offset would help to anticipate the location of the edge and would compensate for the actual displacement of the object that would occur during the inevitable neural delays of visual processing. Thus the perceived position is not the position of the stimulus when it actually triggers the unit but its anticipated position when the signal must interact with other sensory or motor processes. This offset would have played a role in our experiments to the extent that signals from directionally selective units are used to localize an edge. In the case of kinetic edges, for example, about half the units involved in identifying the edge are direction selective and the edge would therefore appear substantially displaced. When there is also a luminance contrast across the kinetic edge, nondirectional units with no offset can also be used to localize the edge and so it appears less displaced. Finally, although the illusions described here may be mediated, at least in part, by early physiological mechanisms (as outlined above) we found that they could be strongly modulated by higher perceptual effects such as stereoscopic figure-ground reversal. The margins of a kinetic window appeared to drift much more when the texture enclosed by it was perceived as a patch in front of the static surround than when it was perceived as a hole through which a moving texture was visible. The moving patch could be made to appear in front of or behind the static surround either by voluntary figure-ground reversal or else by introducing appropriate retinal disparities between the eyes. In either case both motion-capture and positional misalignment were especially striking when the patch was in front. This implies that the assignment of motion to a contour depends strongly on whether that contour is seen as belonging to the figure or to the stationary ground. Acknowledgements. We thank Nicolaus Bourbaki for stimulating discussions. VSR was supported by the Airforce Office of Scientific Research Grant No: 89-0414 and SMA by the Canadian Research Council. Preliminary accounts of these experiments were presented at two meetings: V S Ramachandran and S M Anstis (1987), 11th" ECVP, Bristol, England, and S M Anstis (1989), Symposium on Neural Mechanisms of Visual Perception, Houston, TX [D M K Lam and C Gilbert (Eds) Neural Mechanisms of Visual Perception (Houston, TX: Gulf Publishing) pp 247-260].

616 V S Ramachandran, S M Anstis References Albright T, 1987 "Isoluminant motion processing in macaque visual area MT" Society of Neuroscience. Abstracts (Washington, DC: Society for Neurosciences) CavanaghP, BoeglinMJ, Favreau O, 1985 "Perception of equiluminous kinematograms" Perception 14 151-162 Julesz B, 1971 Foundations of Cyclopean Perception (Chicago: University of Chicago Press) Livingstone M S, Hubel D H, 1987 "Psychophysical evidence for separate channels for the perception of form, colour, movements and depth" Journal of Neuroscience 7 3416-3468 Nakayama K, 1983 "Motion processing: a review" Vision Research 21 1475-1481 Ramachandran V S, 1985 "Apparent motion of subjective surfaces" Perception 14 127-134 Ramachandran V S, 1987 "Interaction between colour and motion in human vision" Nature (London) 328 645-647 Ramachandran V S, 1988 "Interactions between motion, colour, texture and stereopsis" paper presented at Woods Hole Meeting on Computational Vision Ramachandran V S, 1989, paper presented at the NEI Symposium on Perception and Art, Bristol, England (abstract in 1990 Perception 19 273) Ramachandran V S, 1990 "Visual perception in people and machines" AI and the Eye Eds A Blake, T Troscianko (Chichester, UK: John Wiley and Sons) pp 21-77 Ramachandran V S, Anstis S M, 1986 "Perception of apparent motion" Scientific American 254(6)102-109 Ramachandran V S, Anstis S M, 1988 "Position displacement caused by motion capture" Perception 17(3) 406 (abstract) Ramachandran V S, Gregory RL, 1978 "Does colour provide an input to human motion perception?" Nature (London) 215 55-56 Ramachandran V S, Gregory R L, 1991 "Perceptual filling in of artificially induced scotomas in human vision" Nature (London) 350699-702 Ramachandran V S, InadaV, 1985 "Spatial phase and frequency in motion capture of randomdot patterns" Spatial Vision 157-67 Schiller P, Logotethis N K, Charles E R, 1990 "Functions of the color-opponent and broad-band channels of the visual system" Nature (London) 343 68-70 Titchener E B, 1902 Experimental Psychology (New York: MacMillan) P 1990 a Pion publication printed in Great Britain