A New Method for Estimating Effects of Visual Field Loss in a Panoramic Driving Environment

Similar documents
A reduction of visual fields during changes in the background image such as while driving a car and looking in the rearview mirror

Novel 3D Computerized Threshold Amsler Grid Test CA, USA

Frey AP-300 Humphrey Zeiss HFA II 740 perimeters

PERIMETRY A STANDARD TEST IN OPHTHALMOLOGY

Automated Perimeter PTS 1000

The Perception of Optical Flow in Driving Simulators

THRESHOLD AMSLER GRID TESTING AND RESERVING POWER OF THE POTIC NERVE by MOUSTAFA KAMAL NASSAR. M.D. MENOFIA UNIVERSITY.

Instruments Commonly Used For Examination of the Eye

Video Test to Evaluate Detection Performance of Drivers with Hemianopia: Preliminary Results

Driving Simulators for Commercial Truck Drivers - Humans in the Loop

AGING AND STEERING CONTROL UNDER REDUCED VISIBILITY CONDITIONS. Wichita State University, Wichita, Kansas, USA

Low Vision Assessment Components Job Aid 1

Comparison of Wrap Around Screens and HMDs on a Driver s Response to an Unexpected Pedestrian Crossing Using Simulator Vehicle Parameters

Computer analysis of optic disc images. Comparison with HRT data

Human Vision and Human-Computer Interaction. Much content from Jeff Johnson, UI Wizards, Inc.

The Appearance of Images Through a Multifocal IOL ABSTRACT. through a monofocal IOL to the view through a multifocal lens implanted in the other eye

Introduction. scotoma. Effects of preferred retinal locus placement on text navigation and development of adventageous trained retinal locus

GROUPING BASED ON PHENOMENAL PROXIMITY

Steering a Driving Simulator Using the Queueing Network-Model Human Processor (QN-MHP)

NANOS Patient Brochure

OCULUS Twinfield 2. Perimeter. We focus on progress

Three-dimensional computer-automated threshold Amsler grid test

SAfety VEhicles using adaptive Interface Technology (SAVE-IT): A Program Overview

PERCEIVING MOVEMENT. Ways to create movement

Examination of the ten degrees of visual field surrounding fixation

We focus on progress OCULUS CENTERFIELD 2

Insights into High-level Visual Perception

OCULUS Easyfield. Perimeter. We focus on progress

Evaluation of High Intensity Discharge Automotive Forward Lighting

Design Process. ERGONOMICS in. the Automotive. Vivek D. Bhise. CRC Press. Taylor & Francis Group. Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business

Peripheral Prism Glasses for Hemianopia Giorgi et al. APPENDIX 1

Wide-Band Enhancement of TV Images for the Visually Impaired

Retina. Convergence. Early visual processing: retina & LGN. Visual Photoreptors: rods and cones. Visual Photoreptors: rods and cones.

Perceived depth is enhanced with parallax scanning

CB Database: A change blindness database for objects in natural indoor scenes

Image Characteristics and Their Effect on Driving Simulator Validity

Driving simulators in hemianopia rehabilitation research

Fitting Peripheral Prisms for Patients with Hemianopia. The workshop is sponsored by Chadwick Optical, Inc.

EXAMINATION OF THE CENTRAL VISUAL FIELD AT

TRAFFIC SIGN DETECTION AND IDENTIFICATION.

3D Space Perception. (aka Depth Perception)

Developments of a peripheral vision system using immersive virtual environment

PERIPHERAL VISON PATTERN DETECTION DYNAMIC TEST

Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion

Psych 333, Winter 2008, Instructor Boynton, Exam 1

3-D Computer-Automated Threshold Amsler Grid Test

Simulation and Animation Tools for Analysis of Vehicle Collision: SMAC (Simulation Model of Automobile Collisions) and Carmma (Simulation Animations)

The Design and Assessment of Attention-Getting Rear Brake Light Signals

THE RELATIVE IMPORTANCE OF PICTORIAL AND NONPICTORIAL DISTANCE CUES FOR DRIVER VISION. Michael J. Flannagan Michael Sivak Julie K.

REPLICATING HUMAN VISION FOR ACCURATE TESTING OF AR/VR DISPLAYS Presented By Eric Eisenberg February 22, 2018

Using Driving Simulator for Advance Placement of Guide Sign Design for Exits along Highways

Suppression in strabismus

The effect of 3D audio and other audio techniques on virtual reality experience

Iowa Research Online. University of Iowa. Robert E. Llaneras Virginia Tech Transportation Institute, Blacksburg. Jul 11th, 12:00 AM

Development of a 24 GHz Band Peripheral Monitoring Radar

Experiments on the locus of induced motion

Development and Validation of Virtual Driving Simulator for the Spinal Injury Patient

JOHANN CATTY CETIM, 52 Avenue Félix Louat, Senlis Cedex, France. What is the effect of operating conditions on the result of the testing?

CX-1 digital retinal camera mydriatic & non-mydriatic. Redefining true versatility.

Discriminating direction of motion trajectories from angular speed and background information

USE YOUR PC TO QUICKLY MAP REMAINING VISION AFTER FOVEAL VISION LOSS

studies on the cup/disc ratio

the human chapter 1 Traffic lights the human User-centred Design Light Vision part 1 (modified extract for AISD 2005) Information i/o

This question addresses OPTICAL factors in image formation, not issues involving retinal or other brain structures.

Object Perception. 23 August PSY Object & Scene 1

Vision Science I Exam 2 31 October 2016

VISUOMOTOR PROGRAM DESCRIPTION AND OVERVIEW

Driver Education Classroom and In-Car Curriculum Unit 3 Space Management System

binocular projection by electrophysiological methods. An account of some METHODS

Modified Bagolini striated glass test: clinical applications of starlight test in binocular visual field screening

Motion Perception II Chapter 8

scotopic, or rod, vision, and precise information about the photochemical

CAN GALVANIC VESTIBULAR STIMULATION REDUCE SIMULATOR ADAPTATION SYNDROME? University of Guelph Guelph, Ontario, Canada

Chapter 5: Sensation and Perception

Vision: How does your eye work? Student Advanced Version Vision Lab - Overview

Interventions for vision impairments post brain injury: Use of prisms and exercises. Dr Kevin Houston Talia Mouldovan

Vision V Perceiving Movement

Vision V Perceiving Movement

4K Resolution, Demystified!

Multi variable strategy reduces symptoms of simulator sickness

Vision. The eye. Image formation. Eye defects & corrective lenses. Visual acuity. Colour vision. Lecture 3.5

Retinal stray light originating from intraocular lenses and its effect on visual performance van der Mooren, Marie Huibert

NAVIGATIONAL CONTROL EFFECT ON REPRESENTING VIRTUAL ENVIRONMENTS

Work Domain Analysis (WDA) for Ecological Interface Design (EID) of Vehicle Control Display

Chapter 6. Experiment 3. Motion sickness and vection with normal and blurred optokinetic stimuli

Visual computation of surface lightness: Local contrast vs. frames of reference

The Effect of Opponent Noise on Image Quality

Advances in Vehicle Periphery Sensing Techniques Aimed at Realizing Autonomous Driving

Research Article Testing of Visual Field with Virtual Reality Goggles in Manual and Visual Grasp Modes

IMPLANTABLE MINIATURIZED TELESCOPE (IMT) FOR LOW-VISION

Lecture 8. Human Information Processing (1) CENG 412-Human Factors in Engineering May

Clinical evaluation and management of glaucoma is largely

THE SCHOOL BUS. Figure 1

The impact of non-immersive head-mounted displays (HMDs) on the visual field

Collision judgment when viewing minified images through a HMD visual field expander

FOR PRECISE ASTIGMATISM CORRECTION.

VNG Balance Testing. VN415 / VO425 - Complete solutions for balance testing

WHY EDOF INTRAOCULAR LENSES? FOR EXCELLENT VISION QUALITY TO SUPPORT AN ACTIVE LIFESTYLE PATIENT INFORMATION. Cataract treatment

Image processing of computerised visual field data

The User Experience: Proper Image Size and Contrast

Transcription:

University of Iowa Iowa Research Online Driving Assessment Conference 2017 Driving Assessment Conference Jun 27th, 12:00 AM A New Method for Estimating Effects of Visual Field Loss in a Panoramic Driving Environment David E. Anderson Department of Neurological Sciences, University of Nebraska Medical Center,Omaha, NE Deepta A. Ghate Truhlsen Eye Institute, College of Medicine, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE Sachin Kedar Department of Neurological Sciences, University of Nebraska Medical Center,Omaha, NE Matthew Rizzo Department of Neurological Sciences, University of Nebraska Medical Center,Omaha, NE Follow this and additional works at: http://ir.uiowa.edu/drivingassessment Anderson, David E.; Ghate, Deepta A.; Kedar, Sachin; and Rizzo, Matthew. A New Method for Estimating Effects of Visual Field Loss in a Panoramic Driving Environment. In: Proceedings of the Ninth International Driving Symposium on Human Factors in Driver Assessment, Training and Vehicle Design, June 26-29, 2017, Manchester Village, Vermont. Iowa City, IA: Public Policy Center, University of Iowa, 2017: 2-8. https://doi.org/10.17077/drivingassessment.1607 This Presentation is brought to you for free and open access by the Public Policy Center at Iowa Research Online. It has been accepted for inclusion in Driving Assessment Conference by an authorized administrator of Iowa Research Online. For more information, please contact lib-ir@uiowa.edu.

A NEW METHOD FOR ESTIMATING EFFECTS OF VISUAL FIELD LOSS IN A PANORAMIC DRIVING ENVIRONMENT David E. Anderson 1*, Deepta A. Ghate 2, Sachin Kedar 1,2, Matthew Rizzo 1 1 Department of Neurological Sciences, College of Medicine 2 Truhlsen Eye Institute, College of Medicine University of Nebraska Medical Center Omaha, NE, USA * Email: david.anderson@unmc.edu Summary: Glaucoma is a key cause of peripheral visual field loss and increases risk of a vehicle crash. Patients may be unaware of their visual loss and of hazards in the driving panorama. Standard clinical automated perimetry, the gold standard for monitoring glaucoma progression, lacks external validity to evaluate functional effect of visual field loss in driving environments. We developed and piloted a new technique to study the effects of glaucoma in a panoramic (290 forward FOV) simulated driving environment. Preliminary results in 11 drivers (7 with glaucoma and 4 with suspected glaucoma): (1) demonstrate the relationship between standard clinical perimetry and driving simulator visual fields, (2) replicate clinical evidence of glaucoma-related peripheral visual field loss, and (3) show added visual field loss due to visual occlusion by in-cab geometry. INTRODUCTION Glaucoma is a major public health concern, affecting 2% of the U.S. population over the age of 40 (Friedman et al., 2004). This potentially blinding optic neuropathy is characterized by peripheral vision loss. Visual field defects in glaucoma affects driving performance (Crabb et al., 2013). U.S. drivers with glaucoma had 1.65 times greater MVC rates compared to the general population, with severe visual field loss being the highest ranked risk factor for MVC (Kwon, 2016). A case-control study showed a 3.6-fold increased risk of an injurious crash in glaucoma. Peripheral (30-60 degrees) visual field defects greater than 10 db led to a 2.4 times increased risk (Owsley et al., 1998). Glaucoma patients were 6 times more likely to need a critical intervention from a driving instructor compared to controls, particularly for unnoticed pedestrian (Haymes, 2008). Glaucoma patient symptoms may provide clues to mechanisms of increased driving risk. Some glaucoma patients report needing more light or blurry vision (Hu et al., 2014). In a forced choice experiment, patients with bilateral glaucoma described their visual experience as blurred (54%) or normal (26%) when viewing a naturalistic display (Crabb et al., 2013); contradicting what is commonly taught, none reported a black spot or hole in vision. One hypothesis suggests glaucoma patients fill in their visual field defects, as in perceptual completion of physiologic blind spot (where the optic nerve enters the back of retina). Evidence from physiologic, artificial, and retinal scotomas (areas of vision loss surrounded by preserved vision) suggest drivers with glaucoma do not appreciate the role of their vision impairment in their failure to perceive critical objects or events (Hoste, 2003; Figure 1). 2

Figure 1. Simulating perceptual experiences of full visual fields and visual field defects To study the effects of glaucomatous visual field loss on driving, it is critical to measure visual fields in an immersive driving environment. While standard clinical automated perimetry for the assessment of visual field loss such as the Humphrey visual field () test are essential for monitoring disease progression, their unknown external validity limits extrapolation to real world effects. This study sought to adapt the into a full-sized high-fidelity driving simulator. Our goals were to: (1) better understand the relationship between standard clinical measures of visual field loss and effective fields of view in naturalistic settings; (2) replicate evidence of glaucomarelated peripheral visual field loss in a driving simulator visual field (DSVF) test; (3) evaluate added effects of visual occlusion due by vehicle geometry. METHODS Subjects. Eleven drivers (45% women), including 7 with glaucoma and 4 with suspected glaucoma (diagnosed using standard clinical criteria), participated. Mean ages were 66.1 17.2 (SD) and 65.5 10.1, for glaucoma patients and suspects, respectively. All subjects were recruited from the Truhlsen Eye Institute at the University of Nebraska Medical Center (UNMC) in Omaha, NE. Glaucoma suspects had normal visual fields; glaucoma patients had a range of visual field defects. All had corrected visual acuity of at least 20/40 in each eye and were experienced visual field takers with at least three stable (reliability indices better than 20%) s over the last 2 years. Subjects were excluded if they had a diagnosis of neurological disorder or cognitive impairment. This study was approved by the UNMC IRB. Visual Field Mapping. Visual field testing was performed in the ophthalmology clinic using the 30-2 or the 24-2 SITA standard strategy in automated perimetry (Humphrey Field Analyzer II-i, Carl Zeiss Meditec Inc) as part of routine clinical care prior to recruitment for the study. Driving Simulator. SENSEI (Simulator for Ergonomics, Neuroscience, Safety Engineering and Innovation), a DriveSafety (Salt Lake City, UT) RS-600 Research Simulator, is a fully integrated, high performance, high fidelity driving simulation system with an authentic automotive cab designed for use in ground vehicle design and driving safety research applications (Figure 2a). Coupled with HyperDrive advanced scene and scenario authoring tools, the SENSEI simulation system provides an ideal environment to create purpose-built driving scenarios for a wide range of research studies. The RS-600 provides an out-the-window display environment of 290 degrees with 7 Ultra-HD (3840x2160 each) curved LED displays, a full-size automobile cab including windshield, front and rear seating compartments, center console, dash and instrumentation, automatic transmission gear select, two side and one center rear view LCD 3

mirrors. The car cab is based on a 2004 Ford Focus. This system provides a flexible and advanced data collection capability for recording vehicle and driver performance measures. a SENSEI b Stimulus Display Grid c Simulator Display Simulator for Ergonomics, Neuroscience, Safety Engineering and Innovation Stimulus Figure 2. SENSEI and Visual Field Environment A-Pillar Driving Simulator Visual Field. The Driving Simulator Visual Field (DSVF) was designed to simulate retinal loci in the 30-2 strategy (Figure 2b). Vertical fields were limited by SENSEI display sizes and in-cab geometry (Figure 2c), including obstruction by the A-pillar, precluding direct replication of retinal loci. DSVF maps out grid locations 60 degrees of total horizontal visual angle and 20 degrees of total vertical visual angle, spanning across three displays. Forty imaginary grid locations (10-colums by 4-rows) each separated by 6 degrees of visual angle across both vertical and horizontal axes were centered over a fixation dot presented on the center display. Stimulus size subtended a visual angle of 0.5 degrees, similar to the HFA stimulus size III in the HFA. Monocular (left and right) and binocular DSVFs were examined. DSVF Procedure. Red supra-threshold stimulus images were presented against a light-gray background. Grid locations were tested in random patterns and each grid location was tested 4 times, resulting in 160 serial visual stimulus presentations. Stimulus duration was 200 milliseconds, with a varying inter-stimulus interval (ISI) randomly sampled from a uniform distribution ranging from 1.2 to 1.7 seconds in increments of.017 seconds. Patients were instructed to press a red button on the steering wheel after detecting a stimulus presentation. Hits were counted if the button was pressed prior to subsequent stimulus presentation. Total testing duration was approximately 4 minutes. Each DSVF was repeated twice to ensure reproducibility of scotomas for a total of 6 DSVF tests. Order of testing was randomized. Data Analysis. Visual field index (VFI) estimates the unimpaired proportion of visual field, ranging between 0% (fully impaired) to 100% (fully intact). -VFI estimates were acquired from Humphrey Visual Field Analyzer. The Best Location algorithm was used to integrate monocular visual fields (Nelson-Quigg et al 2000) and a gray scale was used to construct the integrated binocular visual field. Response rates (0-4) and cortical magnification weights were used to calculate a global DS VF-index (DS-VFI; Bengtsson & Heijl, 2008) for constructed DSVF visual fields. The monocular and binocular gray scales and DS-VFI estimates from the DSVF trials were compared to the gray scales from clinic s. RESULTS One glaucoma patient had unstable visual fields, and was excluded from further analysis. Results from remaining patients (n=10) were all studied. and DSVF measures are displayed in Figure 3. 4

Left Visual Field Right Visual Field Binocular Visual Field Glaucoma Suspect (G2) A DS-VFI 89% DS-VFI 91% DS-VFI 88% DS-VFI 87% DS-VFI 96% DS-VFI 95% B A-Pillar Scotoma Blind Spot -VFI 100% -VFI 100% Response Rate: >50% 25% 0% Integrated Area Evaluated by DSVF Glaucoma (G7) C DS-VFI 44% DS-VFI 39% DS-VFI 68% DS-VFI 62% DS-VFI 51% DS-VFI 59% D -VFI 60% -VFI 89% Integrated Figure 3. Overview of and DSVF Outcome Measures. Left, right, and binocular visual fields for a selected glaucoma suspect (A,B) and glaucoma patient (C,D) are depicted. Visual field performance is summarized by gray scale representations. Each cell depicts performance in each visual field location. In DSVFs (A,C), intensities within each cell correspond to the proportion of trials in which a response was observed. Performance is summarized by the DS-VFI. Visual occlusion by the in-cab A-pillar are apparent in all three visual fields (blue box). Physiologic scotomas imposed by the optic nerve were confirmed in left and right visual fields (green box). In s (B,D), intensities in each cell represent the threshold (db) values for detecting stimulus presentations. Performance is summarized by the -VFI. For comparison, the 20-degree x 60-degree region of the evaluated in the DSVF (red box) is provided. Comparing and DSVF. In glaucoma suspects, monocular -VFI estimates were >99% (Figure 3B); DS-VFI estimates were 88.2±2.9% (mean ±SD) for left eye, 89.0±1.0% for right eye, and 94.9±1.0% for binocular visual fields (Figure 3A, 4A). In glaucoma patients, -VFI estimates were 60.7±23.9% for worst eye and 88.8±12.5% for best eye visual fields (Figure 3D, 5); DS-VFI estimates were 37.6±32.2% for worst eye, 58.4±30.7% for best eye, and 64.2±27.8% for binocular visual fields (Figure 3C, 5). Reductions in DSVF performance relative to performance resulted from visual occlusion by the in-cab A-pillar in the left visual hemifield (discussed below in Artificial Scotomas). Consistent with blind spots mapped in (Figure 3A), DSVF mapped blind spots at 15 loci (Figures 3B). Indeed, blind spots mapped in all 16 monocular visual fields in glaucoma suspects (Figure 3A, 4A). Estimations of binocular visual fields from (Figure 3B, 3D) were confirmed in DSVF binocular visual fields (Figure 3A, 3C, 5). Split-half reliability (trial-1 vs trial-2) was greater than.99 across left, right, and binocular fields. Absolute DS-VFI differences between each of 2 trials per visual field were 2.2±1.2% for glaucoma suspects and 5.8±5.3% for glaucoma patients, respectively; furthermore, strong correlations between -VFI and DS-VFI estimates were observed for left (r=.77, p<.01) and right (r=.99, p<.0001) visual fields (Figure 4B). 5

A DSVF Left Visual Field DSVF Right Visual Field DSVF Binocular Visual Field Trial 1 Trial 2 Trial 1 Trial 2 Trial 1 Trial 2 DS-VFI 67% DS-VFI 64% DS-VFI 89% DS-VFI 84% DS-VFI 95% DS-VFI 90% -VFI 88% -VFI 100% Integrated B DS-VFI 33% DS-VFI 31% DS-VFI 20% DS-VFI 41% DS-VFI 47% DS-VFI 54% -VFI 44% -VFI 68% Integrated C DS-VFI 73% DS-VFI 71% DS-VFI 1% DS-VFI 12% DS-VFI 75% DS-VFI 74% -VFI 96% -VFI 42% Integrated Figure 5: Comparing and DSVF in Glaucoma Patients. (A, B, C). DSVF gray scales observed for left, right, and binocular visual fields in three glaucoma patients. For each glaucoma patient and visual field, DSVF (top) and (bottom) gray scales are provided. DS-VFI values are presented above each DSVF gray scale for first (left) and second (right) trials. Full gray scales (left) are provided with the region tested by the DSVF outlined (red box) and scaled (right) to inspect similarities between measures. 6

Artificial Scotomas. In glaucoma suspects, DSVF revealed consistent impairments in stimulus detection at the 21 retinal locus in the right eye and 27 locus in the left eye (Figure 3A, 4A) which persists in all 8 binocular visual field trials; in glaucoma patients, impaired stimulus detection is observed across monocular and binocular visual fields (Figure 3C, 5). This apparent visual field defect, corresponding to visual occlusion imposed by the in-cab A-pillar, is approximately 10 horizontally, reduces binocular VFI by 4.9±.8%, and is unlikely to cause significant functional impairment in normal visual fields. In contrast, glaucoma patients showed a larger reduction in binocular VFI (11.3±15.9) due to A-pillar occlusion. Thus, glaucoma patients may experience significant impairment as a result of A-pillar occlusion in an already compromised visual field. DISCUSSION Our DSVF task replicated clinical s in a naturalistic driving environment. We successfully mapped physiologic blind spots caused by lack of photoreceptors at the optic disk, peripheral field loss due to optic neuropathy in glaucoma, and estimated binocular visual fields. We observed consistent visual occlusion by the in-cab A-pillar, suggesting vehicle geometry restricts the spatial extent of visual fields in typical populations. Glaucoma negatively effects driving performance (Crabb et al., 2013; Tanabe, 2011; Kwon, 2016; Owsley et al., 1998; Haymes, 2008). The links between reports of visual experience (Crabb et al., 2013; Hoste, 2003) and physiological effects of optic neuropathy and visual field loss are unclear. The current method offers the opportunity allow us to better understand these links in a naturalistic setting, providing a platform for developing rehabilitation and compensatory strategies to overcome real-world effects of visual field loss. Visual occlusion by vehicle geometry impairs drivers with normal visual fields and introduces additive impairment to glaucoma drivers with a history of visual field loss. Additional impairments imposed by vehicle geometry during driving may lead to further declines in visual and behavioral function, resulting in reduced driver safety. Further studies are needed to understand how additive visual field loss impairs driving in glaucoma patients and how compensatory behaviors may be developed to overcome these putative limitations. Fluctuations in visual fields are common and well-studied as in the Ocular Hypertension Treatment Study (Keltner et al., 2000) and were mitigated in this study because our subjects were experienced visual field takers. Variability increases with increasing test sensitivity (Heijl et al., 1989) and the magnitude of visual field defects, and can be reduced by repeating visual fields. Future studies can also combine the DSVF with eye tracking, to map visual fields during driving and determine compensatory effect of eye and head position on field of view. CONCLUSION Our pilot study shows that the DSVF maps out visual fields in participants with and without visual field defects. Field defects (physiologic and occlusive) in the DVSF provide added value in terms of demonstrating ecological effects of visual field defects in operational settings, beyond the impairments demonstrated with conventional clinic-based automated perimetry. The 7

occlusive scotoma may have added safety implications in patients with peripheral field loss and may be considered in vehicle safety design. The results of this study may also inform development of in vehicle systems for alerting and warning drivers when obstacles appear in compromised visual fields. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We thank the subjects for their participation and patience. This study was supported by a Pilot Grant award from the Department of Neurological Sciences at UNMC and by NIH R01 AG017177. REFERENCES Crabb, D.P., Smith, N.D., Glen, F.C., et al. (2013). How does glaucoma look? Patient perception of visual field loss. Ophthalmology, 120, 1120 1126. Friedman, D.S., Wolfs, R.C., O'Colmain, B.J., et al. (2004). Eye Diseases Prevalence Research Group. Prevalence of open-angle glaucoma among adults in the United States. Arch Ophthalmol, 122, 532-538. Haymes, S.A., LeBlanc, R.P., Nicoleia, M.T., et al. (2008). Glaucoma and on-road driving performance. IOVS, 49, 3035-3041. Hoste, A.M. (2003). New insights into the subjective perception of visual field defects. Bull Soc Belge Ophthalmol, 287, 65-71. Hu, C.X., Zangalli, C., Hsieh, M., Gupta, L., et al. (2014). What do patients with glaucoma see? Visual symptoms reported by patients with glaucoma. Am J Med Sci, 348, 403-409. Keltner, J.L., Johnson, C.A., Quiqq, J.M., et al. (2000). Confirmation of visual field abnormalities in the Ocular Hypertension Treatment Study. Arch Ophthalmol, 118, 1187-94. Kwon, M., Huisingh, C., Rhodes, L.A., McGwin, G., et al. (2016). Association between glaucoma and at-fault motor vehicle collision involvement among older drivers: a population-based study. Ophthalmology, 123, 109-116. Nelson-Quigg, J.M.,Cello, K., & Johnson, C.A. (2000). Predicting binocular visual field sensitivity from monocular visual field results. IOVS, 41, 2212-2221. Owsley, C., McGwin, G., & Ball, K. (1998). Vision impairment, eye disease, and injurious motor vehicle crashes in the elderly. Ophthalmic Epidemiology, 5, 101-113. Tanabe, S., Yuki, K., Ozeki, N., Shiba, D., et al. (2011). The association between primary open angle glaucoma and motor vehicle collisions. IOVS, 52, 4177-81. 8