Low-Frequency Transient Visual Oscillations in the Fly

Similar documents
Visual Coding in the Blowfly H1 Neuron: Tuning Properties and Detection of Velocity Steps in a new Arena

Wide Field Visual Information Encoding in the Blow Fly

Abstract. Introduction

CN510: Principles and Methods of Cognitive and Neural Modeling. Neural Oscillations. Lecture 24

Phase Synchronization of Two Tremor-Related Neurons

EE ELECTRICAL ENGINEERING AND INSTRUMENTATION

Interference in stimuli employed to assess masking by substitution. Bernt Christian Skottun. Ullevaalsalleen 4C Oslo. Norway

CHAPTER 7 HARDWARE IMPLEMENTATION

Limulus eye: a filter cascade. Limulus 9/23/2011. Dynamic Response to Step Increase in Light Intensity

COMMUNICATIONS BIOPHYSICS

The shape of luminance increments at the intersection alters the magnitude of the scintillating grid illusion

binocular projection by electrophysiological methods. An account of some METHODS

Human Vision and Human-Computer Interaction. Much content from Jeff Johnson, UI Wizards, Inc.

PREDICTION OF FINGER FLEXION FROM ELECTROCORTICOGRAPHY DATA

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Determination of human EEG alpha entrainment ERD/ERS using the continuous complex wavelet transform

332:223 Principles of Electrical Engineering I Laboratory Experiment #2 Title: Function Generators and Oscilloscopes Suggested Equipment:

EVLA Memo 108 LO/IF Phase Dependence on Antenna Elevation

Changing the sampling rate

Supporting Online Material for

PSYC696B: Analyzing Neural Time-series Data

Linear Polarisation Noise for Corrosion Monitoring in Multiple Phase Environments. (Patent Pending)

TIME encoding of a band-limited function,,

Chapter 8: Perceiving Motion

Neuronal correlates of pitch in the Inferior Colliculus

Receiver Design for Passive Millimeter Wave (PMMW) Imaging

Psych 333, Winter 2008, Instructor Boynton, Exam 1

Spectro-Temporal Methods in Primary Auditory Cortex David Klein Didier Depireux Jonathan Simon Shihab Shamma

III III 0 IIOI DID IIO 1101 I II 0II II 100 III IID II DI II

Biomimetic whiskers for shape recognition

CHAPTER 8: EXTENDED TETRACHORD CLASSIFICATION

Visual Perception of Images

The best retinal location"

Examples: Find the domain and range of the function f(x, y) = 1 x y 2.

Ambiguous Encoding of Stimuli by Primary Sensory Afferents Causes a Lack of Independence in the Perception of Multiple Stimulus Attributes

40 Hz Event Related Auditory Potential

Frog Vision. PSY305 Lecture 4 JV Stone

This tutorial describes the principles of 24-bit recording systems and clarifies some common mis-conceptions regarding these systems.

Object Perception. 23 August PSY Object & Scene 1

Lab #9: Compound Action Potentials in the Toad Sciatic Nerve

780. Biomedical signal identification and analysis

Validation & Analysis of Complex Serial Bus Link Models

10. Computer-Assisted Data Acquisition and Analysis

Computing with Biologically Inspired Neural Oscillators: Application to Color Image Segmentation

A Numerical Approach to Understanding Oscillator Neural Networks

A specialized face-processing network consistent with the representational geometry of monkey face patches

EE 791 EEG-5 Measures of EEG Dynamic Properties

The Speaker Study. By: Jay Bliefnick. Acoustical Testing 1. Attn: Dr. Dominique Chéenne, Dr. Lauren Ronsse. Group Members:

Lab in a Box Microwave Interferometer

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Interference [Hecht Ch. 9]

VISUAL NEURAL SIMULATOR

(Time )Frequency Analysis of EEG Waveforms

A CLOSER LOOK AT THE REPRESENTATION OF INTERAURAL DIFFERENCES IN A BINAURAL MODEL

Name Date: Course number: MAKE SURE TA & TI STAMPS EVERY PAGE BEFORE YOU START EXPERIMENT 10. Electronic Circuits

Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

Fig. 1. Electronic Model of Neuron

User s Manual for Integrator Long Pulse ILP8 22AUG2016

Simple reaction time as a function of luminance for various wavelengths*

iworx Sample Lab Experiment AN-2: Compound Action Potentials

Lab E5: Filters and Complex Impedance

Nature Neuroscience: doi: /nn Supplementary Figure 1. Optimized Bessel foci for in vivo volume imaging.

WIRELESS 20/20. Twin-Beam Antenna. A Cost Effective Way to Double LTE Site Capacity

A Vestibular Sensation: Probabilistic Approaches to Spatial Perception (II) Presented by Shunan Zhang

PAPER ANEMOMETER. Igor Marković1 Department of Physics, Faculty of Science, University of Zagreb, Croatia

Lab E5: Filters and Complex Impedance

TRANSFORMS / WAVELETS

Pre-Lab 10. Which plan or plans would work? Explain. Which plan is most efficient in regard to light power with the correct polarization? Explain.

Virtual Access Technique Extends Test Coverage on PCB Assemblies

Laboratory 1: Uncertainty Analysis

Experiment 19. Microwave Optics 1

Downloaded 09/04/18 to Redistribution subject to SEG license or copyright; see Terms of Use at

COGS 101A: Sensation and Perception

QUANTITATIVE STUDY OF VISUAL AFTER-IMAGES*

Design of Temporally Dithered Codes for Increased Depth of Field in Structured Light Systems

Chapter 2 A Silicon Model of Auditory-Nerve Response

Lab 15: Lock in amplifier (Version 1.4)

Physics 262. Lab #1: Lock-In Amplifier. John Yamrick

IMPLEMENTATION OF REAL TIME BRAINWAVE VISUALISATION AND CHARACTERISATION

Retina. last updated: 23 rd Jan, c Michael Langer

ELEC 0017: ELECTROMAGNETIC COMPATIBILITY LABORATORY SESSIONS

Supplementary Information for Common neural correlates of real and imagined movements contributing to the performance of brain machine interfaces

PAPER ANEMOMETER. Igor Marković 1 1 Department of Physics, Faculty of Science, University of Zagreb, Croatia

Estimation of speed, average received power and received signal in wireless systems using wavelets

FEATURE. Adaptive Temporal Aperture Control for Improving Motion Image Quality of OLED Display

Classifying the Brain's Motor Activity via Deep Learning

2 : AC signals, the signal generator and the Oscilloscope

Color Science. What light is. Measuring light. CS 4620 Lecture 15. Salient property is the spectral power distribution (SPD)

BIO-ELECTRIC MEASUREMENTS

RELIABILITY OF GUIDED WAVE ULTRASONIC TESTING. Dr. Mark EVANS and Dr. Thomas VOGT Guided Ultrasonics Ltd. Nottingham, UK

TED TED. τfac τpt. A intensity. B intensity A facilitation voltage Vfac. A direction voltage Vright. A output current Iout. Vfac. Vright. Vleft.

BIO 365L Neurobiology Laboratory. Training Exercise 1: Introduction to the Computer Software: DataPro

Tone-in-noise detection: Observed discrepancies in spectral integration. Nicolas Le Goff a) Technische Universiteit Eindhoven, P.O.

Techniques for Generating Sudoku Instances

Reduce Load Capacitance in Noise-Sensitive, High-Transient Applications, through Implementation of Active Filtering

Radar. Radio. Electronics. Television. .104f 4E011 UNITED ELECTRONICS LABORATORIES LOUISVILLE

Encoding of Naturalistic Stimuli by Local Field Potential Spectra in Networks of Excitatory and Inhibitory Neurons

Basic Microprocessor Interfacing Trainer Lab Manual

Detecting spread spectrum pseudo random noise tags in EEG/MEG using a structure-based decomposition

Transcription:

Kate Denning Biophysics Laboratory, UCSD Spring 2004 Low-Frequency Transient Visual Oscillations in the Fly ABSTRACT Low-frequency oscillations were observed near the H1 cell in the fly. Using coherence calculations, we found that this signal is correlated with the visual stimulus. Further supporting this correlation, controls such as blocking most of the visual stimulus decreased the coherence. The high correlation was short-lived, lasting only approximately 100 ms, although the specific duration of the correlation may depend on stimulus properties. INTRODUCTION Oscillations have been observed in many systems and have been theorized to be involved in multiple functions, ranging from attention (Fell 2003) to object recognition. Local field potentials in the fruit fly include a 20-30 Hz component that increases with the presentation of a novel stimulus (van Swinderen 2003). While examining signals near the H1 of the fly, we observed low-frequency oscillations. We wanted to determine whether this signal was dependent on the visual stimulus. In order to isolate the low-frequency component of the signal before addressing this question, we converted the signals into the frequency domain and calculated the coherence. The coherence can be thought of as the correlation between the two vectors at each frequency. The magnitude of the coherence values varies from zero to one, where one is the maximal correlation. We calculated the coherence while a full-field randomly varying stimulus was presented at two frequencies or when the stimulus was briefly but regularly turned on. We also calculated control coherences by shuffling the stimuli values or by blocking the majority of the stimulus from the fly. In the initial three conditions, we found high coherence, whereas it drastically decreased in each of the control conditions. We also examined whether the signal remained coherent across time. If the oscillations occurred only during novel stimuli, they might be transient. Also, studies have shown that characteristics such as firing rate and the information rate adapt extremely quickly within the H1 neuron (Fairhall 2001), so perhaps the oscillations occur only while adapting. The high coherence was found to be transient. METHODS

Recordings Calliphora or Sarcophaga flies were aged approximately two days to prepare for physiological recordings. Flies were first anesthetized by placing flies in freezing temperatures for approximately six minutes and were then fixed with wax at the abdomen to a small metal plate. The antennae, limbs, proboscis, and wings were amputated and small amounts of wax were placed on these locations to prevent further movement. The posterior part of the head was pushed waxed to the body, such that the back of the head was parallel to the metal plate. Small incisions were made to expose one hemisphere of the brain. One drop of saline was applied to this hemisphere. A small incision was also made in the posterior part of the abdomen for the ground electrode. The metal plate was then secured into a clamp that positioned the fly in the center of circle composed of LEDs (described in the Visual Stimuli section). Tungsten electrodes with 3 mega-ohm impedance were used. The recording electrode was positioned in the right third neuropil, in the region of H1, whereas the ground electrode was placed in the incision mentioned above. LABVIEW programs collected data from the electrodes at a rate of 30,000 Hz. Visual Stimuli Stimuli were presented on individually controlled light-emitting diodes (LEDs) that were located at multiple heights but an approximately constant radius from the fly. The LEDs extended across the majority of the circle, although a small angle behind the fly did not include any LEDs, as the hardware of the apparatus was positioned there. LABVIEW programs controlled the LEDs. This experiment was designed to use temporally varying full-field stimuli, as the width of the LEDs had been determined to be too small to simulate continuous moving vertical bars. The full-field stimuli s luminance values were selected from a random distribution. The luminance value was modulated either at 80 or 200 Hz. The delta experiment consisted of turning all of the LEDs to the maximum intensity for what would correspond to one-twentieth of a frame at either 80 or 200 Hz (.625 or.25 ms, respectively) and would then turn off for the remainder of the frames. During one experiment, the majority of the light was blocked, by placing cardboard in front of the LEDs, although some light did escape that was located on the LEDs most posterior to the fly s head. Data Analysis The voltage signal recorded from the electrodes was low-pass filtered using a Butterworth filter with a cutoff frequency of 500 Hz. The signal was then down sampled to 1000 Hz. Similarly the stimulus that was output was sampled at 1000 Hz. The coherence phase and magnitude were calculated between the first ten seconds of the voltage signal and the stimulus, unless otherwise noted. Error bars were calculated using the following equation:

where the p-value was set to equal.05. N is the number of samples that each time series interval was divided into, which for all analyses except that in figure 6 was equal to 39. µ was set to 1 as there was no overlap between the windows of the initial time sample. Error bars are only shown in figure 2, as the error is a constant error in all other graphs, except for analyses present in figure 6. (In the figure six analysis, a time segment was used that was only one-fifth as long as in other analyses, thereby decreasing the N value and increasing the error. However, it was necessary to use short segments, as we were analyzing the dynamics of the coherence measurements.) RESULTS & DISCUSSION Recordings from the optic lobe included low-frequency components Flies were placed in the center of a circle composed on light-emitting diodes (LEDs). The differential voltage was recorded between an electrode placed in the right optic lobe and a ground electrode located in the fly s abdomen. All LEDs were then changed to randomly selected luminance values that varied as a function of time. The luminance values were modulated either at 200 Hz or 80 Hz. A segment of the voltage trace obtained during the presentation of the 200-Hz stimulus is shown in Figure 1a. This trace seems to include spikes (probably from H1) that were not well isolated from the noise and low frequency oscillations (Fig. 1a). These characteristics were observed in both of the two flies tested and in both the 200- and 80-Hz stimulus conditions. In order to better analyze the low-frequency oscillations, the signal was filtered by a Butterworth, low-pass filter, with a cutoff frequency of 250 Hz (Fig. 1b). Finally, in order to save on computational time, the signal was down sampled from 30,000 to 1,000 Hz (Fig. 1c). The low-frequency signal s high coherence with the stimulus suggests that the signal is visual The low-frequency signal could be attributed to many sources, including noise in the apparatus. Coherence is a measure of how correlated each frequency component of the signal is with another signal. The coherence measurement varies between zero and one, with a coherence of one indicating maximal correlation. In this instance, we were interested in the coherence of the low-frequency band between the signal and the visual stimulus. We calculated the coherence during the first ten seconds of each of the two signals. Figure 2a shows that there is indeed high coherence within the low-frequency band (less than 100 Hz), regardless of whether the 200- or the 80-Hz stimulus was presented. As a control, the coherence was also calculated between the signal and a shuffled stimulus. The stimulus was shuffled in two manners. The first shuffling maintained the times at which the stimulus changed, but the luminance values to which the stimulus changed to were shuffled. For example, if the stimulus initially changed to the arbitrary intensity values of.5, 1, and.75 at the arbitrary times of 1, 6, and 11, then the shuffled stimulus would still change at times 1, 6, and 11, but the intensity values might be shuffled to.75,.5, and 1. This was termed the frame-shuffling control.

The second shuffling shuffled the stimulus values at every millisecond of the data (which is the sampling rate used in order to calculate the coherence). Extending the above example, this new shuffling would cause the stimulus to change every time value, from 1 to 15, with 1/3 of the frames being assigned to each of the three contrasts. This was termed the ms-shuffling control. Figures 2b and 2c show the results from the coherence calculated between the signals and the frame- and ms-shuffling controls, respectively, for both the 200- and the 80-Hz stimuli. (The non-shuffled coherence is plotted as a dotted line for comparison.) Both forms of shuffling seem to eliminate any significant coherence in the low-frequency band. In order to further explore whether the low-frequency oscillations were due to the fly s response to the visual stimulus (as opposed to external noise picked up on the electrode), the light was partially blocked using cardboard. Some of the LEDs, located in the extreme periphery escaped, and was probably seen by the fly, as their visual angle is so wide. This test was only performed using the 200-Hz stimuli. The coherence in the lowfrequency band decreased significantly (Fig. 3), although it still appears to be more coherent than baseline. This may be due to the light that was not blocked. The high coherence at the low frequencies suggests that the signal depends on the visual stimulus. The low coherence in the same frequency band with the shuffled stimulus further supports this fact. The ms-shuffling stimulus controls for the possibility that the signal would have caused this coherence with any given stimulus. The frame-shuffling stimulus controls for the possibility that the high coherence is due to the stimulus changing at particular times within the stimulus, rather than the actual luminance values within those frames. Therefore, the low-frequency component of the signal does seem to be controlled by the specific stimulus characteristics. The phase of the coherence is similar at frequencies with high coherence, suggesting that the high coherence was due to one single signal If there is a visually driven signal in the noted low-frequency range, then phase of the coherence with the visual stimulus across these frequencies should be similar. This is because the high coherence at that range of frequencies is likely due to a single signal with a single phase, rather than a collection of signals with similar frequencies but independent phases (and therefore likely to be independent signals). The coherence phases were found to be rather constant (especially compared to the large fluctuations in phase at high frequencies) in the low-frequency band during both the 80- and 200-Hz stimuli (Fig. 4). Fast delta signals still exhibit high low-frequency coherence, refuting the possibility that the signal was stimulus-dependent noise Perhaps the low-frequency coherence was due to noise that depended on the luminance of the LEDs. In order to test this possibility, the LEDs were briefly turned to the maximum luminance value for one-twentieth of the 40-Hz frame (1.25 ms). The LEDs turned off

for the remainder of the frame. Therefore, a stimulus change was still occurring at the same time as during previous stimuli, but if the signal directly depended on the stimulus luminance, then the frequency region of high coherence would be at a higher frequency than before. Figure 5 shows that when the delta stimulus was presented, the region of high coherence remains within the low-frequency region seen using previous stimuli, countering the idea that the signal is stimulus-dependent noise. The high coherence of the low-frequency signal is transient The coherence between the voltage signals and the stimuli were calculated using a smaller, 2-second sliding window that slid at 10-ms intervals. The low-frequency coherence decreased as a function of time. In order to quantify, a frequency range was chosen for each stimulus that initially had high coherence. (The ranges were 10-50 Hz and 30-70 Hz for the full-field stimuli modulated at 80- and 200-Hz, respectively.) The mean coherence was calculated within this frequency range for each window. The mean coherence could then be plotted as a function of the beginning time of the window. Similar averages were calculated from coherences calculated with the frame-shuffled, control stimuli. Figure 6 shows that at times later after the stimulus start, the coherence of the signal decreased, eventually becoming indistinguishable to that of the controls. From this data, we can roughly estimate that the coherence of the low-frequency signal becomes indistinguishable from baseline at approximately 100-ms after the stimulus onset for these stimuli. SUMMARY Signals recorded in the optic lobe of flies revealed a low-frequency signal. This signal was analyzed by examining the coherence of the signal with various visual stimuli. The signal s low-frequency band exhibited high coherence with three full-field stimuli, including random temporally varying 200- and 80-Hz stimuli and delta stimuli, consisting of brief, bright LED flashes. The controlled coherences, obtained either by introducing shuffled stimuli or blocking the stimuli decreased the coherence. Therefore, the signal seems to be a visual, neural signal not noise. The fact that the coherence of the low-frequency components of the signal exist for only 100 ms after the onset of the stimulus suggests that these oscillations are likely influenced by other factors. Perhaps the oscillations occur only while attending or perhaps the oscillations are quickly adapted away. Previous studies mentioned in the introduction conclude that oscillations within the fly depend on specific stimulus features. However, these oscillations do seem to contain information valuable in decoding a visual scene. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS We thank Evren Turner for his help in performing the experiments presented in this paper and writing all LABVIEW programs used in gathering data and presenting the stimulus.

REFERENCES 1. Fairhall AL, Lewen GD, Bialek W, de Ruyter Van Steveninck RR. Efficiency and ambiguity in an adaptive neural code. Nature. 2001 Aug; 412(6849): 787-92. 2. Fell J, Fernandez G, Klaver P, Elger CE, Fries P. Is synchronized neuronal gamma activity relevant for selective attention? Brain Res Rev. 2003 Jun; 42(3): 265-72. 3. van Swinderen B, Greenspan RJ. Salience modulates 20-30 Hz brain activity in Drosophila. Nat Neurosci. 2003 Jun; 6(6): 579-86. FIGURE 1. (a) Segment of the electrode s signal when the 200-Hz full-field stimulus was presented. The signal was low-pass filtered (b) and resampled (c), as described in the text.

FIGURE 2. (a) Coherence between a signal and the stimulus when a full-field temporally modulated stimulus, modulated either at 200 Hz (black line) or 80 Hz (red line), was presented. (b) Coherence calculated between the signal and a stimulus with luminance values that were shuffled over all frames (where a frame depends is 5 ms for the 200 Hz stimulus and 12.5 ms for the 80 Hz stimulus). The coherence values shown in (a) are plotted as the dotted lines. (c) Coherence calculated between the signal and a stimulus with luminance values shuffled over all millisecond bins. Error bars are calculated as described in the methods.

FIGURE 3. (a) Coherence between the signal and the stimulus when a full-field stimulus was modulated at 200 Hz when either all of the LEDs were visible to the fly (black line) or the majority of the LEDs were blocked (red line). (b) Similar coherence measurements using an 80-Hz stimulus.

FIGURE 4. The phase of the coherence calculated between the signal and a full-field stimulus modulated at either 200 Hz (black line) or 80 Hz (red line).

FIGURE 5. The coherence between the signal and a full-field stimulus that was flashed on and off every 25 ms.

FIGURE 6. The coherence was calculated as a function of time by calculating the coherence magnitude between the signal and the full-field temporally modulated stimulus within a two-second, sliding window. The sliding window moved in 10-ms increments. These values were calculated for both the 200- (black line) and 80-Hz (red line) stimuli. They were compared to the control coherence values calculated between the signals and a shuffled stimulus obtained by shuffling the luminance values over frames (dotted lines).