Envelopment and Small Room Acoustics

Similar documents
Surround: The Current Technological Situation. David Griesinger Lexicon 3 Oak Park Bedford, MA

Speaker placement, externalization, and envelopment in home listening rooms

General overview of spatial impression, envelopment, localization, and externalization

Auditory Localization

RECENT EXPERIENCES WITH ELECTRONIC ACOUSTIC ENHANCEMENT IN CONCERT HALLS AND OPERA HOUSES

The psychoacoustics of reverberation

Multichannel Audio In Cars (Tim Nind)

Acoustics II: Kurt Heutschi recording technique. stereo recording. microphone positioning. surround sound recordings.

Sound source localization and its use in multimedia applications

Listening with Headphones

Binaural Hearing. Reading: Yost Ch. 12

Accurate sound reproduction from two loudspeakers in a living room

SIA Software Company, Inc.

Computational Perception. Sound localization 2

Room Acoustics. March 27th 2015

Sound Radiation Characteristic of a Shakuhachi with different Playing Techniques

Audio Engineering Society. Convention Paper. Presented at the 124th Convention 2008 May Amsterdam, The Netherlands

Copyright 2009 Pearson Education, Inc.

Psychoacoustic Cues in Room Size Perception

Introduction. 1.1 Surround sound

Multichannel Audio Technologies. More on Surround Sound Microphone Techniques:

Effects of Reverberation on Pitch, Onset/Offset, and Binaural Cues

URBANA-CHAMPAIGN. CS 498PS Audio Computing Lab. 3D and Virtual Sound. Paris Smaragdis. paris.cs.illinois.

APPLICATIONS OF A DIGITAL AUDIO-SIGNAL PROCESSOR IN T.V. SETS

Added sounds for quiet vehicles

CHAPTER ONE SOUND BASICS. Nitec in Digital Audio & Video Production Institute of Technical Education, College West

CONTENTS. Preface...vii. Acknowledgments...ix. Chapter 1: Behavior of Sound...1. Chapter 2: The Ear and Hearing...11

Validation of lateral fraction results in room acoustic measurements

The relation between perceived apparent source width and interaural cross-correlation in sound reproduction spaces with low reverberation

PERCEIVED ROOM SIZE AND SOURCE DISTANCE IN FIVE SIMULATED CONCERT AUDITORIA

IMPLEMENTATION AND APPLICATION OF A BINAURAL HEARING MODEL TO THE OBJECTIVE EVALUATION OF SPATIAL IMPRESSION

INVESTIGATING BINAURAL LOCALISATION ABILITIES FOR PROPOSING A STANDARDISED TESTING ENVIRONMENT FOR BINAURAL SYSTEMS

DC-1 Theory and Design

Evaluation of a new stereophonic reproduction method with moving sweet spot using a binaural localization model

MUS 302 ENGINEERING SECTION

DESIGN OF ROOMS FOR MULTICHANNEL AUDIO MONITORING

Physics I Notes: Chapter 13 Sound

Assessing the contribution of binaural cues for apparent source width perception via a functional model

Spatialisation accuracy of a Virtual Performance System

From time to time it is useful even for an expert to give a thought to the basics of sound reproduction. For instance, what the stereo is all about?

Audio Engineering Society. Convention Paper. Presented at the 129th Convention 2010 November 4 7 San Francisco, CA, USA. Why Ambisonics Does Work

Final Exam Study Guide: Introduction to Computer Music Course Staff April 24, 2015

Chapter 12. Preview. Objectives The Production of Sound Waves Frequency of Sound Waves The Doppler Effect. Section 1 Sound Waves

Exploiting envelope fluctuations to achieve robust extraction and intelligent integration of binaural cues

New acoustical techniques for measuring spatial properties in concert halls

Chapter 14, Sound. 1. When a sine wave is used to represent a sound wave, the crest corresponds to:

Computational Perception /785

A3D Contiguous time-frequency energized sound-field: reflection-free listening space supports integration in audiology

Improving 5.1 and Stereophonic Mastering/Monitoring by Using Ambiophonic Techniques

Improving room acoustics at low frequencies with multiple loudspeakers and time based room correction

Impulse Response Measurements Using All-Pass Deconvolution David Griesinger

Spatial Audio Reproduction: Towards Individualized Binaural Sound

Loudspeaker Distortion Measurement and Perception Part 2: Irregular distortion caused by defects

Lecture PowerPoints. Chapter 12 Physics: Principles with Applications, 6 th edition Giancoli

Chapter 3. Meeting 3, Psychoacoustics, Hearing, and Reflections

SPATIAL AUDITORY DISPLAY USING MULTIPLE SUBWOOFERS IN TWO DIFFERENT REVERBERANT REPRODUCTION ENVIRONMENTS

Perception of low frequencies in small rooms

DECORRELATION TECHNIQUES FOR THE RENDERING OF APPARENT SOUND SOURCE WIDTH IN 3D AUDIO DISPLAYS. Guillaume Potard, Ian Burnett

CONTROL OF PERCEIVED ROOM SIZE USING SIMPLE BINAURAL TECHNOLOGY. Densil Cabrera

Monaural and binaural processing of fluctuating sounds in the auditory system

A COMPARISION OF ACTIVE ACOUSTIC SYSTEMS FOR ARCHITECTURE

SECTION A Waves and Sound

The Human Auditory System

Reproduction of Surround Sound in Headphones

Sound, acoustics Slides based on: Rossing, The science of sound, 1990.

SECTION A Waves and Sound

Master MVA Analyse des signaux Audiofréquences Audio Signal Analysis, Indexing and Transformation

Chapter 6: Room Acoustics and 3D Sound Processing

Suppose you re going to mike a singer, a sax, or a guitar. Which mic should you choose? Where should you place it?

Lecture PowerPoints. Chapter 12 Physics: Principles with Applications, 7 th edition Giancoli

EQ s & Frequency Processing

No Brain Too Small PHYSICS

Sound. Production of Sound

ROOM ACOUSTICS MEASUREMENT AND ASSESSMENT METHODS. 1. Signal to Noise OBJECTIVE METHODS ASSESSMENT OF ROOM ACOUSTICS CAN BE DIVIDED INTO:

CHAPTER 12 SOUND ass/sound/soundtoc. html. Characteristics of Sound

Introduction to Dynamic Loudspeaker Design

Convention Paper Presented at the 125th Convention 2008 October 2 5 San Francisco, CA, USA

Monitor Setup Guide The right monitors. The correct setup. Proper sound.

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007 VIRTUAL AUDIO REPRODUCED IN A HEADREST

SOUND 1 -- ACOUSTICS 1

Advanced techniques for the determination of sound spatialization in Italian Opera Theatres

IS SII BETTER THAN STI AT RECOGNISING THE EFFECTS OF POOR TONAL BALANCE ON INTELLIGIBILITY?

29th TONMEISTERTAGUNG VDT INTERNATIONAL CONVENTION, November 2016

MULTICHANNEL REPRODUCTION OF LOW FREQUENCIES. Toni Hirvonen, Miikka Tikander, and Ville Pulkki

The analysis of multi-channel sound reproduction algorithms using HRTF data

Validation of a Virtual Sound Environment System for Testing Hearing Aids

STUDIO ACUSTICUM A CONCERT HALL WITH VARIABLE VOLUME

Aalborg Universitet Usage of measured reverberation tail in a binaural room impulse response synthesis General rights Take down policy

Measuring procedures for the environmental parameters: Acoustic comfort

Principles of Musical Acoustics

PHYSICS 102N Spring Week 6 Oscillations, Waves, Sound and Music

An overview of multichannel level alignment

Preview. Sound Section 1. Section 1 Sound Waves. Section 2 Sound Intensity and Resonance. Section 3 Harmonics

AUDL GS08/GAV1 Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Loudness & Temporal resolution

NCERT solution for Sound

Audio Spotlighting. Premkumar N Role Department of Electrical and Electronics, Belagavi, Karnataka, India.

Towards a generalized theory of low-frequency sound source localization

Sound Source Localization using HRTF database

7.8 The Interference of Sound Waves. Practice SUMMARY. Diffraction and Refraction of Sound Waves. Section 7.7 Questions

SGN Audio and Speech Processing

Transcription:

Envelopment and Small Room Acoustics David Griesinger Lexicon 3 Oak Park Bedford, MA 01730 Copyright 9/21/00 by David Griesinger

Preview of results Loudness isn t everything! At least two additional perceptions: Envelopment Externalization Both require multiple drivers at LF Both are improved if the drivers are at the side of the listener Externalization better with a 90 degree shift

Loudness isn t everything We all think we can perceive loudness. (we know how to measure it.) BUT: rooms with identical loudness can sound quite different. Small rooms have audible spatial properties. These properties interact with the recording technique and loudspeaker placement.

Measurement and Modeling You don t understand anything unless you can make a machine that measures what you perceive. A sound level meter measures loudness - (more or less) We need a machine that can measure our spatial perception of enclosed spaces.

OK - so build a machine! The method is clear - true envelopment is created by apparent motion of the reverberation from a syllabic source, created by flucutations in the ITD and the IID We have to detect the ITD and the IID the way the ear does Details of how the ear detects localization are not well known and details are where it is at...

It s all in your head To understand perception we must: understand the physics of the sound detector understand how the brain processes the detected stimulus build a model that includes both. To understand rooms we must couple the room properties to the perception. Frequencies below 200Hz behave quite differently than higher frequencies

Interaural Time Delay (ITD) Interaural Cross Correlation a traditional measure IACC cannot be easily calculated from the basilar membrane data IACC combines Interaural Intensity Differences (IID) and ITD Perceptual experiments show IID and ITD are separately perceived.

ITD and single reflections spatial properties of single lateral reflections depend on the delay the delay dependency is different for cues based on ITD, IID, and IACC measured data show that below 200Hz ITD is the primary cue for spatial properties. Below 200Hz ITD is also the primary cue for localization.

Spatial perception and ITD A constant ITD (or ITD during a fast rise) is perceived as source azimuth. A rapidly varying or randomly varying ITD is perceived as a stationary source in the presence of envelopment. An absence of variation in the ITD in the presence of head motion results in in-thehead localization.

TWO spatial perceptions Envelopment the perception that room sound - particularly reverberation - surrounds the listener. most small rooms provide no envelopment of their own. Envelopment must come from the recording Externalization low frequencies are perceived as inside the head in many playback rooms.

How does the ear detect the ITD? ITD of sine waves seems easy to detect but these are only weakly localized!! There are inherent ambiguities in the ITD of monochromatic signals beyond a certain ITD the lead or lag of phase becomes ambiguous Steady tones are weakly localized Phase of steady signals is not detected above 500Hz.

Example - Sine waves with earphones We expect to hear a signal moving left and right at a one Hz rate actual perception is of a poorly localized sound moving ~+-30 degrees

Localization has several states 1. Sharply localized - (syllabic inputs with fast rise-times) 2. Poorly localized but moving (spacious) 3. Unlocalized - (surrounding but not enveloping) it is possible that the perception I have called continuous spatial impression is related to the Unlocalized localization state.

And the localization depends strongly on the source plucked string bass produces strong localization and high envelopment Bowed (arco) string bass does not High source dependence makes the measurement of envelopment directly from an impulse response unlikely to be successful.

Human hearing detects the IDT during signal rise-times Most musical signals are NOISEY level and phase fluctuate rapidly The ear is always looking for ITD differences during the rising edge of signals IDTs during dips in the level (of either ear) are inhibited IDTs during steady tones are also inhibited

Some ideas for further experiments 1. How quickly does a signal have to rise to be strongly localized? 2. What is the difference between an unlocalized sound and a (syllabic) sound that is enveloping by virtue of high fluctuation in the ITD?

Example - decay in Boston 63 Hz stopped tone

Envelopment envelopment is the Holy Grail of concert hall design when reproducing sound in small spaces envelopment is frequently absent sound mixing rooms with low reverberation times are often particularly poor In rooms where envelopment can be heard the strength of the perception depends on the recording technique.

How do we measure envelopment? ITD Fluctuation in the range of 2-20Hz is perceived as envelopment Fluctuations during the reverberant component of the signal stream are particularly important. Reflected sound causes ITD fluctuation The amount of fluctuation depends on the properties of the source music.

Reflected sound causes ITD fluctuation Large spaces can produce fluctuations even with narrow band signals.

The impulse response of a small room is short 12 x15 x9 room, RT ~0.2sec, TC ~ 30ms If the music signal varies slowly the room will always be steady-state

Small spaces - listening rooms Small spaces produce fluctuations in the 2-20Hz range ONLY if the sound source is broadband. For narrow band signals a fluctuating ITD can still be produced IF the recording has fluctuating phase AND there are multiple drivers.

Anechoic spaces envelopment can be created by reproducing sound from two decorrelated loudspeakers envelopment at LF is maximum when the loudspeakers are at the side a single loudspeaker gives no envelopment

Anechoic space - std stereo Standard stereo gives little envelopment because the speakers are not lateral - even with decorrelated material.

Reflective spaces can create envelopment directly ONLY if the reverberation time constant is larger than the inverse bandwidth of the stimulus or if there are multiple drivers reproducing material with fluctuating phase.

Recorded reverb has narrow bandwidth and slow variation A small room cannot produce a fluctuating ITD from a single driver.

Bandwidth of sound decay Decay of a held sine tone in Boston Symphony Note the bandwidth is 3Hz or less

A measure for Envelopment Must measure zero in an anechoic space Must measure low values when a single driver is used DG has not found a clever way of doing this directly from the impulse response or its Fourier transform!!

Brute force works for DFT To find the Diffuse Field Transfer Function (DFT) we model: (or measure) the room, to find the binaural impulse response from multiple drivers the musical signal - to convolve with the impulse response the head-pinnae system, to calculate the ITD calculate the fluctuation in the ITD The average magnitude of the fluctuations is our measure

Image Model for Rooms Is valid at LF if all surfaces have identical absorption. This is almost never the case in listening rooms. The model works well enough anyway.

The Musical Signal We can use measured decays of musical notes in large spaces as test sources must use an uncorrelated decay for each driver Narrow band noise (~3Hz bandwidth) appears to adequately model music Critical band noise models envelopment from broadband signals

The Head-Pinnae system We will model as two omnidirectional receivers separated by ~25cm Model is valid only below about 150Hz Such a model allows an enormous simplification of the problem without losing qualitative accuracy

Calibration of the DFT The optimal angle for two uncorrelated loudspeakers can be tested limited listening tests reveal that below ~120Hz envelopment is optimal when the speakers are at the sides We can use this DFT value as a reference

Tests of the DFT Drivers at the front Drivers at the side Anechoic Space

Anechoic DFT along the center line = drivers at side; - - - - = drivers in front

Octave Band Noise Sources in reflective space Single driver in corner Two drivers in front, uncorrelated 12 x15 x9 room, wall reflectivity 0.8

Octave Band DFT as a function of reflectivity DFT along the center line; = Two drivers, 0.8, = Two drivers, 0.6, - - - - = One driver, 0.6

DFT with Music - 3Hz Bandwidth, reflectivity.8 = Two drivers, at side, - - - = Two drivers, at front = Single driver, at the front left

Conclusions on Envelopment at low frequencies Two + drivers are essential for music A single LF driver in the front does NOT create envelopment in a room with lateral reflectivity < 0.6 LF drivers are better at the side. Recorded reverberation must be decorrelated

Demonstration of Low Frequency Envelopment we can design a beat frequency signal

And use it to test rooms envelopment is clearly audible whenever the listener is near a velocity maximum of a lateral mode envelopment is nearly inaudible when the listener is near a pressure maximum

Example A listener at a velocity maximum will hear high envelopment

Envelopment at High Frequencies Above 200Hz most music is no longer monochromatic Many (at least the best) playback rooms can be well damped Loudspeakers tend to be more directional Thus the reverberation radius can be larger than the source to listener distance

Above 200Hz room modes become less important Although a live room could produce substantial envelopment, rooms in common use do not. Above 1000Hz front/back differences begin to be noticeable. At 1500Hz just the front speakers can produce envelopment Between 200 and 500Hz the loudspeaker arrangement and the method of driving these loudspeakers become critical.

Measurement requires higher bandwidth 1/3 octave noise bands are useful

Success is elusive with a fixed listening position many experiments with a fixed measuring head did not yield results that agreed with subjective impressions. It is necessary to measure both lateral and front/back envelopment

2-5 and 2-7 Matrices Matrix systems are capable of greatly increasing both subjective and measured envelopment in most rooms However most matrix systems were developed to enlarge the sweet spot for dialog and sound effects, not to increase envelopment

A successful matrix increases envelopment by reproducing reverberation from the sides of the listener with maximum decorrelation reproducing low frequencies from the sides of the listener wherever possible reproducing enveloping sound effects - such as crowd noise or applause - with full separation to the sides and the rear of the listeners.

Not all matrix systems are the same Several 2-5 matrix systems are currently on the market These systems differ markedly in their subjective and measured envelopment in general, image width and envelopment from the front speakers are reduced compared to two channel stereo rear channels are not optimally decorrelated These differences are particularly noticeable in cars

Conclusions 1 spatial properties of small rooms are determined by the interaction between lateral and medial room modes the bandwidth and syllabic properties of the source the orientation of the listener

Conclusions 2 small rooms develop their own sense of space if the room time constant is greater than the inverse bandwidth of the source the listener is not near a lateral velocity minimum for the source frequency there are at least two drivers on opposite sides of the listener the source material contains decorrelated reverberation

Conclusions 3 most if not all the low frequency spatial properties of small rooms are measurable with a swept wobble tone, a binaural microphone, and a detector for interaural fluctuations A measurement system for higher frequency room properties is under development. (Keep checking the author s web page for updates)