Behavioural Realism as a metric of Presence

Similar documents
Manuscript no. 1317/111

Perceived depth is enhanced with parallax scanning

Quantitative evaluation of sensation of presence in viewing the "Super Hi-Vision" 4000-scanning-line wide-field video system

ANUMBER of electronic manufacturers have launched

Perception. What We Will Cover in This Section. Perception. How we interpret the information our senses receive. Overview Perception

Collaboration in Multimodal Virtual Environments

Perception of Self-motion and Presence in Auditory Virtual Environments

Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion

Psychophysics of night vision device halo

Cybersickness, Console Video Games, & Head Mounted Displays

Cyclopean Vision, Size Estimation and Presence in Orthostereoscopic Images. Bernard Harper and Richard Latto

Takeharu Seno 1,3,4, Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2, Stephen Palmisano 5 1

Perception in Immersive Virtual Reality Environments ROB ALLISON DEPT. OF ELECTRICAL ENGINEERING AND COMPUTER SCIENCE YORK UNIVERSITY, TORONTO

Thinking About Psychology: The Science of Mind and Behavior 2e. Charles T. Blair-Broeker Randal M. Ernst

Visual control of posture in real and virtual environments

Human Visual lperception relevant tto

Reinventing movies How do we tell stories in VR? Diego Gutierrez Graphics & Imaging Lab Universidad de Zaragoza

Postural instability precedes motion sickness

Vection in depth during consistent and inconsistent multisensory stimulation

Technologies. Philippe Fuchs Ecole des Mines, ParisTech, Paris, France. Virtual Reality: Concepts and. Guillaume Moreau.

Outline 2/21/2013. The Retina

Chapter 3. Adaptation to disparity but not to perceived depth

Perceiving self-motion in depth: the role of stereoscopic motion and changing-size cues

Chapter 9. Conclusions. 9.1 Summary Perceived distances derived from optic ow

Visual Effects of Light. Prof. Grega Bizjak, PhD Laboratory of Lighting and Photometry Faculty of Electrical Engineering University of Ljubljana

Accelerating self-motion displays produce more compelling vection in depth

ECOLOGICAL ACOUSTICS AND THE MULTI-MODAL PERCEPTION OF ROOMS: REAL AND UNREAL EXPERIENCES OF AUDITORY-VISUAL VIRTUAL ENVIRONMENTS

Discrimination of Virtual Haptic Textures Rendered with Different Update Rates

Visual Effects of. Light. Warmth. Light is life. Sun as a deity (god) If sun would turn off the life on earth would extinct

Application of 3D Terrain Representation System for Highway Landscape Design

Simple reaction time as a function of luminance for various wavelengths*

Self-motion perception from expanding and contracting optical flows overlapped with binocular disparity

State of the Science Symposium

MECHANICAL DESIGN LEARNING ENVIRONMENTS BASED ON VIRTUAL REALITY TECHNOLOGIES

Elements of a multi-level theory of presence: Phenomenology, mental processing and neural correlates

PERSONAL SPACE IN VIRTUAL REALITY

Paper Body Vibration Effects on Perceived Reality with Multi-modal Contents

A reduction of visual fields during changes in the background image such as while driving a car and looking in the rearview mirror

NEW, LIVELY, AND EXCITING OR JUST ARTIFICIAL, STRAINING, AND DISTRACTING A Sensory profiling approach to understand mobile 3D audiovisual quality

Defense Technical Information Center Compilation Part Notice

A Pilot Study: Introduction of Time-domain Segment to Intensity-based Perception Model of High-frequency Vibration

What a Decade of Experiments Reveals about Factors that Influence the Sense of Presence

A Three-Channel Model for Generating the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex in Each Eye

Motion Perception II Chapter 8

The effect of chicken blood and its components on wastewater characteristics and sewage surcharges

Virtual Reality Technology and Convergence. NBAY 6120 March 20, 2018 Donald P. Greenberg Lecture 7

ENHANCED HUMAN-AGENT INTERACTION: AUGMENTING INTERACTION MODELS WITH EMBODIED AGENTS BY SERAFIN BENTO. MASTER OF SCIENCE in INFORMATION SYSTEMS

Lecture 14. Jonathan Pillow Sensation & Perception (PSY 345 / NEU 325) Fall 2017

VIRTUAL REALITY Introduction. Emil M. Petriu SITE, University of Ottawa

Virtual Reality. Lecture #11 NBA 6120 Donald P. Greenberg September 30, 2015

CSC Stereography Course I. What is Stereoscopic Photography?... 3 A. Binocular Vision Depth perception due to stereopsis

Virtual Reality Technology and Convergence. NBA 6120 February 14, 2018 Donald P. Greenberg Lecture 7

A Three-Dimensional Evaluation of Body Representation Change of Human Upper Limb Focused on Sense of Ownership and Sense of Agency

Effects of Visual-Vestibular Interactions on Navigation Tasks in Virtual Environments

Chapter 73. Two-Stroke Apparent Motion. George Mather

Non-Provisional Patent Application #

Illusory displacement of equiluminous kinetic edges

Virtual Reality I. Visual Imaging in the Electronic Age. Donald P. Greenberg November 9, 2017 Lecture #21

P rcep e t p i t on n a s a s u n u c n ons n c s ious u s i nf n e f renc n e L ctur u e 4 : Recogni n t i io i n

Speed perception as an objective measure of presence in virtual environments

Prof. Riyadh Al_Azzawi F.R.C.Psych

Haptic control in a virtual environment

Gait & Posture 30 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Gait & Posture. journal homepage:

Effects of foveal retinal slip on visually induced motion sickness: a pilot study

the ecological approach to vision - evolution & development

The Human Visual System!

Depth seen with subjective

Cameras have finite depth of field or depth of focus

Spatial Judgments from Different Vantage Points: A Different Perspective

Chapter 6. Experiment 3. Motion sickness and vection with normal and blurred optokinetic stimuli

Analysis of Gaze on Optical Illusions

Object Perception. 23 August PSY Object & Scene 1

VR based HCI Techniques & Application. November 29, 2002

A Study on Evaluation of Visual Factor for Measuring Subjective Virtual Realization

HandsIn3D: Supporting Remote Guidance with Immersive Virtual Environments

The Concept of Presence and its Measurement

NAVIGATIONAL CONTROL EFFECT ON REPRESENTING VIRTUAL ENVIRONMENTS

A Vestibular Sensation: Probabilistic Approaches to Spatial Perception (II) Presented by Shunan Zhang

Human Vision. Human Vision - Perception

Virtual Reality. NBAY 6120 April 4, 2016 Donald P. Greenberg Lecture 9

Motion sickness issues in VR content

Sensation notices Various stimuli Of what is out there In reality

Simple Figures and Perceptions in Depth (2): Stereo Capture

EXPERIMENTAL BILATERAL CONTROL TELEMANIPULATION USING A VIRTUAL EXOSKELETON

Vision V Perceiving Movement

Vision V Perceiving Movement

Beyond Visual: Shape, Haptics and Actuation in 3D UI

Insights into High-level Visual Perception

A Multimodal Locomotion User Interface for Immersive Geospatial Information Systems

How various aspects of motion parallax influence distance judgments, even when we think we are standing still

The eye, displays and visual effects

PERCEPTUAL AND SOCIAL FIDELITY OF AVATARS AND AGENTS IN VIRTUAL REALITY. Benjamin R. Kunz, Ph.D. Department Of Psychology University Of Dayton

The Ecological View of Perception. Lecture 14

Perception of scene layout from optical contact, shadows, and motion

The peripheral drift illusion: A motion illusion in the visual periphery

First-order structure induces the 3-D curvature contrast effect

ANALYSIS AND EVALUATION OF IRREGULARITY IN PITCH VIBRATO FOR STRING-INSTRUMENT TONES

2/3/2016. How We Move... Ecological View. Ecological View. Ecological View. Ecological View. Ecological View. Sensory Processing.

the human chapter 1 Traffic lights the human User-centred Design Light Vision part 1 (modified extract for AISD 2005) Information i/o

Human Vision and Human-Computer Interaction. Much content from Jeff Johnson, UI Wizards, Inc.

Transcription:

Behavioural Realism as a metric of Presence (1) Jonathan Freeman jfreem@essex.ac.uk 01206 873786 01206 873590 (2) Department of Psychology, University of Essex, Wivenhoe Park, Colchester, Essex, CO4 3SQ, Dr. S.E Avons savons@essex.ac.uk 01206 873785 01206 873590 (3) Department of Psychology, University of Essex, Wivenhoe Park, Colchester, Essex, CO4 3SQ, Dr. David Harrison harrison@itc.co.uk 01962 848646 01962 886109 (4) Independent Television Commission Engineering Division, Kings Worthy Court, Kings Worthy, Winchester, Hampshire, SO23 7QA, UK Dr. Nick Lodge lodge@itc.co.uk 01962 848634 01962 886109 (5) Independent Television Commission Engineering Division, Kings Worthy Court, Kings Worthy, Winchester, Hampshire, SO23 7QA, UK Prof. Don dep@essex.ac.uk 01206 872865 01206 872900

Pearson Department of Electronic Systems Engineering, University of Essex, Wivenhoe Park, Colchester, Essex, CO4 3SQ, UK Summary Direct Subjective Assessment of Presence is potentially unstable - dependent on prior experience and task demands due to observers not having firm understanding of construct (Freeman, Avons, Pearson, & IJsselsteijn, 1998, submitted). Objective measures of Behavioural Realism provide a potential solution to this problem, as suggested by Slater and Wilbur (1997). Reflexive responses are likely to prove the most reliable of these objective measures. Postural Sway is one such measure. Following Lee and Lishman (1975) we have made video stimuli of a simulated swinging room and have manipulated display conditions (monoscopic and stereoscopic) and magnitude of camera swing. A preliminary experiment designed to evaluate the effects of viewing condition on postural sway is reported here Work is currently in progress to evaluate the effects of viewing condition on postural sway with a view to corroborating subjective scales of presence and constructs related to presence that are currently under development. Introduction We have recently demonstrated that prior experience and task demands can affect direct subjective ratings of presence (Freeman, Avons, Pearson and IJsselsteijn, 1998, submitted). This effect was in part attributed to observers having a limited comprehension of the presence construct. Objective measures of the behavioural realism elicited by a displayed environment offer a solution to this problem. The link we draw between behavioural measures, which may range from simple reflex responses to complex interactions between observers and the environments with which they are presented, and presence is based on the premise that when observers are presented with environments or displays that make them feel present, they will respond to stimuli within the environment in the same way as they would were they presented with the same stimuli in the real world. Our view is thus similar to that presented by Slater and Wilbur (1997) who point out that the utility of presence evoking systems for applications such as psychotherapy rely on this premise. To this we would add a variety of other applications, for example training, remote handling, collaborative working environments and broadcast entertainment. High level cognitive tasks might be susceptible to task demands. More useful and reliable measures of behavioural realism are likely to be those unavailable to consciousness, and thus immune to such problems. One promising measure for the assessment of behavioural realism is

the assessment of postural responses to displayed stimuli. Whilst some behavioural measures might be confounded by higher level cognitive processing and others might generate simple binary responses, for example an observer either responding or not, proprioception during normal stance does not normally involve conscious awareness and has the potential to produce differential levels of response. Postural Sway The importance of visual information for the control of stance has long been recognised, for example through observations that observers exhibit more postural sway in the dark. Lee and Lishman (1975) demonstrated that variations in the optic flow field at the eye are used for proprioception by placing observers within the confines of a polystyrene room. The room was moved sinusoidally through a distance of 6mm (peak to peak amplitude), with a period of 4 seconds, whilst observers postural responses to the room movement were measured. A significant amount of in-phase antero-posterior (AP) postural sway was measured, movement that Lee and Lishman s observers were unaware of. Lee and Lishman reported more body sway when observers were looking at an object 4.5m away than when they were looking at an object 0.45m away. One explanation for this is that the change in observers retinal images was greater for the near object. An alternative explanation rests on the increased efficacy of stereopsis at a near distance. Palmisano (1996) presented evidence that stereoscopically presented optic flow patterns produce improved forward linear vection (subjective sensation of motion). If subjective sensations of vection and postural adjustments are generated by the same mechanism then some of the reduction in body sway reported by Lee and Lishman at near distances may be attributable to the increased efficacy of the stereo cue. This idea is supported by the results of Ojima and Yano (1997) who showed that a smaller field of view (22.5deg) is required to generate relatively stable postural sway control using stereoscopic stimuli than the field of view required to generate similar sway using monoscopic stimuli (45deg). This research was done using computer generated shapes, not photo-realistic video, and it is thus possible that we will generate postural sway with a monoscopic stimulus with a FOV of 32deg. Effects of ocularity of stimulus presentation on postural sway This experiment was designed to test the prediction that stereoscopic stimulus presentation generates a larger postural response in observers than does monoscopic stimulus presentation. Method Two factors were manipulated: (i) viewing condition - two levels, monoscopic or stereoscopic, and (ii) amplitude of sinusoidal camera swing contained in each sequence - three levels, 0mm, 6mm or 12mm. The frequency of the camera swing was maintained at 0.21Hz for both amplitude levels that contained camera movement. Stimuli Sequences Video sequences were taken of a room with books and videos on a stool in the foreground. For each condition two 50 second videos were produced, one for the left and one for the right eye. For stereoscopic presentation of stimuli left and right eye video streams were played, for monoscopic the left eye video stream was presented to both eyes.

Observers Twelve observers (6 m, 6 f, average age 21 years) were paid a small fee to participate. All had normal or corrected to normal vision and a stereo-acuity of 30 sec-arc or better (as tested on the RANDOT random dot stereotest, Stereo Optical Co., Inc., Illinois). Apparatus Observers viewed the stimulus films on an AEA Technology 20" stereoscopic display consisting of two BARCO CPM 2053 colour monitors (50 Hz PAL) with polarised filters in front of each. Observers viewed the display wearing polarised spectacles. Two synchronised Panasonic M2 (A750-B) video players provided the video input for the display. A Flock of Birds (FOB) magnetic position tracker positioned on observers backs (Ascension Technology Corporation, Burlington, VT, USA), was used to collect observers x, y, z positions for each measured period. The FOB was connected to a standard PC running software that controlled both the video players and sampled x, y, z position data at 12.5Hz. Procedure An observer stood without shoes in front of the display, aligned with the centre of the screen and the FOB axis. Observers were instructed to stand still and comfortably and look into the centre of the display (see figure 1). Half of the observers saw the stimuli in mono, half in stereo. Figure 1 - laboratory set-up Results The time domain data of observers AP position was transferred to the frequency domain using the Discrete Fourier Transform (DFT). We summed the activity in the DFT data in frequency bands 0.20Hz and 0.22 Hz for each condition by observer to obtain a measure of the sway that may have been induced by the swinging camera stimuli. Postural Control in the Light and in the Dark: movement in light < movement in dark As is clearly visible in figure 2 there was significantly more sway at these frequencies in the dark than in the light, an order of magnitude of more than double (F (1,11) = 66.394, p<0.0001). This result serves to demonstrate the importance of visual proprioception in the control of stance. Postural Control from Swinging Video: movement in stereo > movement in mono

Figure 2 shows the results of the group mean sway amplitudes across the 3 stimuli conditions split be viewing condition with the group mean sway amplitudes for the dark and light conditions (averaged pre- and post-video) shown for comparison. Stereoscopic viewing was associated with higher magnitudes of postural sway at the frequency of the camera swing (0.21Hz) than was monoscopic viewing at all levels of camera swing; this result approached significance (F (1,11) = 4.709, p=0.053). There was no significant effect of camera sway level (F (2,22) = 1.4893, p=0.247), although there appeared to be a slight trend of higher magnitudes of camera sway being associated with higher postural responses at the frequency of interest. Finally there was also no significant interaction between viewing condition and camera sway level (F (2,22) <1, p=0.793). Discussion These results are encouraging in that postural sway has been shown to provide differential responses based on stimulus manipulations. It is important to point out that this methodology is at an early stage and the findings presented here require replication and validation. Furthermore, issues of observer fatigue and learning are currently under investigation. In addition, in order that postural sway be useful as a display evaluation metric several levels of response need be demonstrated. The results does suggest that stereoscopic presentation of photo-realistic visual stimuli generates a more realistic physical response in observers than does monoscopic presentation of identical stimuli. Presence has been shown to be enhanced by stereoscopic presentation of environments (Hendrix and Barfield, 1996) and photo-realistic television images (Freeman, Avons, Davidoff, & Pearson,1997; IJsselsteijn, de Ridder, Hamberg, Bouwhuis, & Freeman, 1997). The next stage of our research will be to correlate subjective ratings of presence and constructs related to presence with the objective physical measures we are currently developing.

References Freeman, J., Avons, S.E., Davidoff, J., & Pearson, D.E. (1997). Effects of Stereo and Motion manipulations on measured Presence in Stereoscopic Displays. Perception, 26 (suppl), 42. Presented at 20 th ECVP, Helsinki, 1997. Freeman, J., Avons, S.E., Pearson, D.E., & IJsselsteijn, W.A. (1998) Effects of sensory information and Prior Experience on Direct Subjective Presence Ratings. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments (submitted for publication) Hendrix, C., & Barfield, W. (1996). Presence within Virtual Environments as a Function of Visual Display Parameters. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 5, 274-289. IJsselsteijn, W., de Ridder, H., Hamberg, R., Bouwhuis, D., & Freeman, J. (1997). Perceived depth and the feeling of presence in 3DTV. Displays (submitted for publication). Lee, D.N. & Lishman, J.R. (1975) Visual Proprioceptive Control Of Stance. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 1, 87-95. Ojima, S., & Yano, S. (1996) Effect of Depth Sensation on Body Sway with Binocular Vision. Electronics and Communications in Japan, Part 3, 80(4), 83-91. Palmisano, S. (1996) Perceiving self-motion in depth: The role of stereoscopic motion and changing-size cues, Perception and Psychophysics, 58(8), 1168-1176. Slater, M., & Wilbur, S. (1997) A framework for Immersive Virtual Environments (FIVE): Speculations on the Role of Presence in Virtual environments, Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 6(6), 603-616.