Size Illusion on an Asymmetrically Divided Circle

Similar documents
Algebraic functions describing the Zöllner illusion

On the intensity maximum of the Oppel-Kundt illusion

The Mirrored Triangles Illusion. On the perceived distance between triangles in mirror image arrangement. W.A. Kreiner

Zoomed Paintings. W.A. Kreiner University of Ulm / Faculty of Natural Sciences

Orientation Illusions and Crosstalk. University of Ulm Faculty of Natural Sciences

Ingoing versus outgoing wings. The Müller-Lyer and the mirrored triangle illusion

The vertical-horizontal illusion: Assessing the contributions of anisotropy, abutting, and crossing to the misperception of simple line stimuli

Depth adjacency and the rod-and-frame illusion

THE POGGENDORFF ILLUSION: THE PRESENCE OF ANOMALOUS FIGURE IN GENERATING THE EFFECT. Department of General Psychology, University of Padua, Italy

GROUPING BASED ON PHENOMENAL PROXIMITY

Human Vision and Human-Computer Interaction. Much content from Jeff Johnson, UI Wizards, Inc.

Vision. The eye. Image formation. Eye defects & corrective lenses. Visual acuity. Colour vision. Lecture 3.5

Effect of Stimulus Duration on the Perception of Red-Green and Yellow-Blue Mixtures*

UNIT 5a STANDARD ORTHOGRAPHIC VIEW DRAWINGS

THE POGGENDORFF ILLUSION WITH ANOMALOUS SURFACES: MANAGING PAC-MANS, PARALLELS LENGTH AND TYPE OF TRANSVERSAL.

AD-A lji llllllllllii l

Evidence for a significant contribution of interactions between oriented line segments in the Tolansky version of the Poggendorff illusion

Psychophysics of night vision device halo

B.A. II Psychology Paper A MOVEMENT PERCEPTION. Dr. Neelam Rathee Department of Psychology G.C.G.-11, Chandigarh

Introduction to Psychology Prof. Braj Bhushan Department of Humanities and Social Sciences Indian Institute of Technology, Kanpur

Experiments on the locus of induced motion

IOC, Vector sum, and squaring: three different motion effects or one?

Spatial Judgments from Different Vantage Points: A Different Perspective

Interference in stimuli employed to assess masking by substitution. Bernt Christian Skottun. Ullevaalsalleen 4C Oslo. Norway

the dimensionality of the world Travelling through Space and Time Learning Outcomes Johannes M. Zanker

The horizon line, linear perspective, interposition, and background brightness as determinants of the magnitude of the pictorial moon illusion

Haptic control in a virtual environment

Perception. The process of organizing and interpreting information, enabling us to recognize meaningful objects and events.

No symmetry advantage when object matching involves accidental viewpoints

Shape Constancy and Polar Perspective

The Persistence of Vision in Spatio-Temporal Illusory Contours formed by Dynamically-Changing LED Arrays

IV: Visual Organization and Interpretation

Perception. What We Will Cover in This Section. Perception. How we interpret the information our senses receive. Overview Perception

Background stripes affect apparent speed of rotation

The regression to right angles tendency, lateral inhibition, and the transversals in the Zollner and Poggendorff illusions

The roles of axes of symmetry in orientation illusions

Cognition and Perception

objects, we never lose a chance of perceiving an object as solid. Especially vivid suggestions of solidity come from the so-called stereoscopic

Self-motion perception from expanding and contracting optical flows overlapped with binocular disparity

Module 2. Lecture-1. Understanding basic principles of perception including depth and its representation.

COPYRIGHTED MATERIAL. Overview

by intersection with static contours

METHOD Apparatus. A wooden screen, 80 cm. high and 90 cm. wide, covered by a white cardboard whose brightness was 9.0/ in Munsell value,

Shape constancy measured by a canonical-shape method

COPYRIGHTED MATERIAL OVERVIEW 1

Civil Engineering Drawing

Object Perception. 23 August PSY Object & Scene 1

ADAPTATION, AFTER-EFFECT, AND CONTRAST IN THE PERCEPTION OF TILTED LINES. II. SIMULTANEOUS CONTRAST AND THE AREAL RESTRICTION OF THE AFTER-EFFECT

Chapter 1 Overview of an Engineering Drawing

The shape of luminance increments at the intersection alters the magnitude of the scintillating grid illusion

REPORT ITU-R BO Multiple-feed BSS receiving antennas

A new illusion of height and width: taller people are perceived as thinner

Size-illusion. P.J. Grant Accurate judgment of the size of a bird is apparently even more difficult. continued...

Takeharu Seno 1,3,4, Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2, Stephen Palmisano 5 1

The ground dominance effect in the perception of 3-D layout

The occlusion illusion: Partial modal completion or apparent distance?

Computers and Imaging

The constancy of the orientation of the visual field

Visual perception of motion in depth: Application ofa vector model to three-dot motion patterns*

Three stimuli for visual motion perception compared

Perceiving Motion and Events

The Grand Illusion and Petit Illusions

7.1 INTRODUCTION TO PERIODIC FUNCTIONS

Chapter 1. Trigonometry Week 6 pp

Limitations of the Oriented Difference of Gaussian Filter in Special Cases of Brightness Perception Illusions

Learned Stimulation in Space and Motion Perception

Camera Resolution and Distortion: Advanced Edge Fitting

The curse of three dimensions: Why your brain is lying to you

MOTION PARALLAX AND ABSOLUTE DISTANCE. Steven H. Ferris NAVAL SUBMARINE MEDICAL RESEARCH LABORATORY NAVAL SUBMARINE MEDICAL CENTER REPORT NUMBER 673

Visual Rules. Why are they necessary?

Simple Figures and Perceptions in Depth (2): Stereo Capture

The Haptic Perception of Spatial Orientations studied with an Haptic Display

11/12/2015 CHAPTER 7. Axonometric Drawings (cont.) Axonometric Drawings (cont.) Isometric Projections (cont.) 1) Axonometric Drawings

Copyrighted Material. Copyrighted Material. Copyrighted. Copyrighted. Material

Student Name: Teacher: Date: District: Rowan. Assessment: 9_12 T and I IC61 - Drafting I Test 1. Description: Unit C - Sketching - Test 2.

Vision Research 48 (2008) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage:

Light Refraction by Water as a Rationale for the Poggendorff Illusion

Unit 8 Trigonometry. Math III Mrs. Valentine

CHAPTER 33 ABERRATION CURVES IN LENS DESIGN

A Tactile Display using Ultrasound Linear Phased Array

Design III CRAFTS SUPPLEMENT

Chapter 5 DESIGN AND IMPLEMENTATION OF SWASTIKA-SHAPED FREQUENCY RECONFIGURABLE ANTENNA ON FR4 SUBSTRATE

CHAPTER 01 PRESENTATION OF TECHNICAL DRAWING. Prepared by: Sio Sreymean

Multi-View Drawing Review

Human Vision. Human Vision - Perception

Visual computation of surface lightness: Local contrast vs. frames of reference

3B SCIENTIFIC PHYSICS

PSY 310: Sensory and Perceptual Processes 1

Articulation: brightness, apparent illumination, and contrast ratios

The Shape-Weight Illusion

First-order structure induces the 3-D curvature contrast effect

This page intentionally left blank

ORTHOGRAPHIC PROJECTIONS. Ms. Sicola

3B SCIENTIFIC PHYSICS

Isometric Drawing Chapter 26

DISTORTION OF VLF RADIO WAVE FIELD VERTICAL METAL POLES.

Analysis of Gaze on Optical Illusions

Perceived depth is enhanced with parallax scanning

Thinking About Psychology: The Science of Mind and Behavior 2e. Charles T. Blair-Broeker Randal M. Ernst

Chapter 3, Part 4: Intro to the Trigonometric Functions

Transcription:

Size Illusion on an Asymmetrically Divided Circle W.A. Kreiner Faculty of Natural Sciences University of Ulm

2 1. Introduction In the Poggendorff (18) illusion a line, inclined by about 45 0 to the horizontal, is occluded by a vertical bar. The thought extensions of the two protruding ends seem to miss each other. The systematic extrapolation error is such as if, unconsciously, one had turned the visible segments of the line slightly towards the horizontal. Taking into account a possible interaction between the edges of the bar and the line, Hotopf (1966) puts it as if... the illusion manifested a tendency for the angles of intersection of the distorting with the distorted parts to appear nearer to right angles than they are. The intensity of the illusion is considerably reduced for the crossing line being oriented either horizontally or vertically. However, what is important in the context of the Poggendorff illusion is not so much the perceived orientation of the protruding line segments but the orientation of their perceived extensions. This was investigated by several authors. Bouma and Andriessen (1968) report that the perceived extension of a horizontal or vertical line segment agrees with its geometric extension. A similar observation is made on a line segment inclined by α=45 0 with respect to the horizontal, except the error is considerably larger. However, for slightly larger or smaller angles α, the perceived direction appears to be tilted towards the nearest horizontal or vertical. Deviations up to 10 0 have been observed. Its dependence on the slant α can be approximated by a function sin(4 α). In the presence of an abutting vertical line the perceived extension of a slanted line (45 0 ) deviates substantially from its true extension. This was shown by Wenderoth, Beh, and White (1978). Deviations up to 7 0 have been found. Spivey-Knowlton and Bridgeman (1993) showed that context elements exert an influence on the illusion, too. Greene and Fisher (1994) investigated the angular induction process by decomposing the induction segment into an array of scattered points. In the divided circle illusion (Ehrenstein, 1954; Lingelbach, 1984), a bar partly occludes a circular line such that segments of unequal size are protruding on either side. Similar to the Poggendorff illusion, the thought extensions of the circular arcs of the two segments appear to miss each other. As a consequence, the protruding segments give the impression as if they were not part of the identical circle, ie, the smaller segment appears too small, and vice versa. The goal of the first experiment was to measure the perceived size of both segments, with the bar upright and, then, tilted by 45 0. The size of the larger segment was varied, serving as the independent parameter x. In a second experiment, where both segments were parts of the identical circle, the apparent size of both segments was measured as a function of the orientation of the occluding bar, its tilt angle being varied in nine steps, from vertical to horizontal orientation.

3 2. Experiments 2.1 Experiment 1 2.1.1 Subjects. Eight healthy volunteers took part, mainly elderly people. Sight was corrected to normal. All of them were naive. 2.1.2 Stimuli. The stimuli consisted of black circular lines, printed on white glossy paper (DIN A3, 297x420 mm), partly occluded by an empty rectangle, as shown in Fig. 1. The small segment was always of the same size, the diameter of the corresponding complete circle being d=68mm, while the diameter D of the large segment s circular line was varied between D=63mm and 75mm, in steps of 3 mm. Both circles were concentric. The occluding bar was 23mm wide, its height being 115mm, and its maximum distance from the arc of the small segment was 12mm. Line width of the circles and the bar was 1.8mm. The bar was oriented either vertically or inclined by 45 0. Five empty black circles below the target served as a reference, decreasing in size from left to right (outer diameter 79, 74.5,, 65.5, 61mm). In total 20 transparencies were shown. 1 2 3 4 5 51 48 45 42 39 1 2 3 4 5 51 48 45 42 39 Fig. 1. Examples of stimuli, one with the occluding bar upright and one with the bar tilted at 45 0. In separate runs, the subjects chose one of the five circles shown underneath to indicate the perceived size of the right or the left segment, respectively. 2.1.3. Procedure. The stimuli were printed on DIN A3 glossy paper and fixed to a flip chart board. The 10 transparencies were presented in random order. They were shown for eight seconds, followed by a blank of five seconds. In the first run, the size of the small segment had to be estimated, in the second run (different order of the transparencies) the larger one. Mean observation distance was about 3m. 2.1.4 Results. Figs. 2a, b give the perceived size of the segments in relation to their true size, as a function of the true size of the large segment, with the bar oriented vertically. On the average, the perceived size of the smaller segment is 91% of its true size (Fig. 2a), while the

Apparent vs. true size of left segment / % Apparent vs. true size of right segment / % 4 larger segment, on the average, is estimated nearly correctly (Fig. 2b). In addition, with increasing true size of the larger segment, a steady decrease of the apparent size of both segments is observed. This is ascribed to a size constancy effect. 110 Fig. 2a. Perceive size of the smaller segment. It is underestimated. In addition, its relative apparent size decreases with increasing true size of the large segment, x. This is interpreted as due to a size constancy effect. Average value is.6(24) %. 62 64 66 68 72 74 76 True size of left segment / mm 110 Fig. 2b. Perceived size of the large segment (left) in relation to its true size. On the average, it is estimated correctly. However, the relative apparent size decreases slightly with increasing true size, too. This, again, is interpreted as a size constancy effect. Average value:.3(22) %. 62 64 66 68 72 74 76 True size of left segment This illusion is less powerful when the bar is rotated counterclockwise by 45 0 (Fig.3a). On the average, the small segment is perceived at 95% of its true size, while the size of the large segment is perceived almost correctly again (Fig. 3b).

Apparent vs. true size of left segment / % Apparent vs. true size of right segment / % 5 110 Fig. 3a. With the bar tilted, the size of the smaller segment is underestimated, but not to the same degree as in case of a vertical bar. In addition, its relative apparent size decreases with increasing true size of the large segment. Average value : 95.4(24) %. 62 64 66 68 72 74 76 True size of left segment 110 Fig. 3b. With the bar tilted, the size of the larger segment, on the average, is estimated correctly. Again, there is a slight decrease of the relative apparent size with increasing true size. Average value:.1(1.5) % 62 64 66 68 72 74 76 True size of left segment / mm Fig. 4 gives the ratio of the illusions, ie, the apparent size of the small segment divided by the perceived size of the large segment for vertical orientation of the occluding bar. The apparent reduction in size is always more pronounced on the smaller segment than on the larger one. Fig. 5 shows the situation for the bar rotated counterclockwise by 45 0. In both diagrams the results are plotted as a function of the true diameter of the larger segment. Size constancy effect. So far, the intensity of the illusion has been plotted versus the true diameter of the larger segment. The perceived size of both of the segments decreases with

Apparent size right vs. left / % Apparent size right vs. left / % 6 Fig. 4. Bar upright. Ratio of the illusions. Perceived size of the small segment (right) divided by the apparent size of the left segment, in percent. On the average, the ratio is.4(34) %. 62 64 66 68 72 74 76 True size of left segment / mm Fig. 5. Bar tilted by 45 0. Ratio of the illusion. Perceived size of the small segment (above the bar) divided by the apparent size of the large segment, in percent. On the average, the ratio is 95.4(28) %. 62 64 66 68 72 74 76 True size of left segment / mm increasing diameter of the larger one. This is ascribed to a size constancy effect. It means that objects of different size are not perceived in strict proportion to their true size, ie, to the size of their retinal images (Lühr, 1898; Cornish, 1937; Schur, 1925; Gilinsky, 1955; Kaufman and Rock, 1962). Concentrating on one particular stimulus, the effect of size constancy occurs when the retinal image varies in size, either because the distance of observation increases or, at constant distance, the size of the object decreases. To give an example, in case the diameter of the retinal image of an object shrinks by a factor of 4, the perceived size of the object will not shrink to one quarter of the original value, but will appear somewhat larger. The degree of size constancy can be quantified by the size constancy parameter, n (Kreiner, 2004). The parameter n can take any value between zero and one. Zero means no size constancy, ie, the apparent size changes in proportion to the size of the retinal image. n=1 refers to the case where the object is always perceived at the same size, independent of the variation of the retinal image. 0 < n <1 means that the apparent size will be larger than one would expect it from the laws of geometrical optics. In general, the effect is triggered by an increase in perceived structural density.

7 In this particular case, the large segment will be increasingly overestimated as soon it gets smaller. This effect transfers to the perceived size of the small segment, so it will appear larger, too. On the other hand, the two segments have also in common that their perceived size reduces steadily with increasing true size of the larger segment. In order to determine the size constancy parameter n, the function y(x) = F x^(-n) has been employed for the fitting procedure. y means the apparent size of a segment relative to its true size, as a function of the true size x of the larger segment (Figs. 2 and 3). F is a scaling parameter. Data are collected in Table 1. It may be worth mentioning that the size constancy parameters found in this experiment (between n=0.28 and 0.37) are comparable to values obtained from experiments performed in order to investigate the moon illusion: From data reported by Schur (1925) one can derive values of n=0.32 and n=0.58, for vertical and horizontal direction of observation, respectively. From experiments performed by Gilinsky (1955) n-values between 0.36 and 0.42 are obtained. Table 1. Results of Experiment 1. Function fitted: y(x) = F x^(-n) Left segment Average perceived size / % Size constancy parameter n Scaling parameter F Bar vertical.3(22) 0.284(49) 334(69) Bar tilted by 45 0.1(15) 0.285(94) 297(106) Right segment Average perceived size / % Size constancy parameter n Scaling parameter F Bar vertical.6(24) 0.322(73) 354(109) Bar tilted by 45 0 95.4(24) 0.37(11) 464(215) Bar vertical.4(34) Bar tilted by 45 0 95.4(28) Ratio of apparent size (right vs. left) / %

8 2.2 Experiment 2 2.2.1 Subjects. In total 11 healthy volunteers took part (among them the author), mainly elderly people. Nine were present in the first run, when the size of the small segment was estimated. Sight was corrected to normal. 10 participants were naive. 2.2.2 Stimuli. 12 Transparencies were employed. Both segments were part of the identical circle. When printed on a DIN A4 paper, the diameter of the circle was 48mm. The orientations of the occluding bar were 0 0 (vertical), 8 0, 23 0, 37 0, 45 0, 53 0, 67 0, 82 0, and 0 (horizontal). In addition, three more transparencies were shown (0 0, 45 0, and 0 ), where the circle s diameter was 45.7 mm (smaller by 5%). Dots were placed symmetrically right on the edges of the bar in order to enhance the effect. Examples are given in Figs. 6a and b. For comparison, five reference circles were presented (52, 49, 46, 43, and 40 mm in diameter). On the transparencies with the smaller circle the same standards were employed. Fig. 6a. Occluding bar inclined by 82 0 with respect to the vertical. Dots are positioned symmetrically on the edge of the bar. In the classic Poggendorff illusion dots enhance the perceived offset: 1 2 3 4 5 8/48/48/45/ref 52 13 Fig. 6b. Stimulus with dots, bar inclined by 45 0. For comparison, in the Poggendorff illusion the effect is reduced in case the crossing line is horizontal. 1 2 3 4 5

Apparent size of the large segment Apparent size of the small segment 9 2.2.3. Procedure. The transparencies were presented with a beamer in random order. First, the stimulus was shown for four seconds, then, the five reference circles were added for another six seconds. Viewing distance was 3m and 4.5m, respectively. Depending on the viewing distance, the target circle (48 mm) subtended an angle of 66 10-3 rad and 44 10-3 rad, respectively. For the 45.7 mm circle the angles are smaller by 5%. In a first run, the small segment was estimated while the perceived size of the large segment was determined in a separate run, presenting the transparencies in different order. 2.2.4 Results are given in Fig. 7. (The results obtained from the smaller circle have been adjusted proportionally.) The large segment was estimated nearly correctly for all orientations of the occluder except for the extreme positions, where a slight enhancement of the apparent size has been found. The small segment appears to be reduced compared to its true size, except for angles around 45 0. Maximum illusion is observed for tilt angles of the bar slightly off the horizontal and vertical orientation (82 0 and 8 0, respectively). A sine function has been fitted. 52 52 50 50 48 48 46 46 44 44 42 42 40 40 38 0 20 40 60 Orientation of the occluding bar/ degrees arc 38 0 20 40 60 Orientation of the occluding bar/ degrees arc Fig. 7: Left: Apparent size of the large segment at nine different angles of inclination of the occluder. At 0 0 (vertical) and 0 (horizontal) the segment appears to be slightly enlarged. Fourth order polynomial fit was performed. For the small segment, the apparent and the true size coincide only for a tilt angle around 45 0. For the other angles between zero and ninety degrees the segment appears too small. A sine function was fitted. The dotted line gives the true diameter of the circle. 3. Comparison with the Poggendorff illusion The size illusion is explained in anlogy to the Poggendorff phenomenon (Fig. 6a). There, the visual system extends each of the protruding ends of the crossing line beyond the occluding bar, just with a tendency to take a slightly shorter distance. For a vertical bar, one rotates these thought extensions unconsciously towards the horizontal, so they miss each other.

10 In the split circle illusion, the extensions of the circular arcs seem to miss each other, too. As a consequence, a size illusion is induced on both of the segments. The size of the large segment is recognized almost correctly, most probably because the major part of the circle can be seen. However, the apparent size of the small segment seems to be more susceptible to the influence of the context. There is experimental evidence that the intensity of the illusion depends considerably on the orientation of the bar. In order to judge the diameter of the corresponding circle one has to extrapolate the circular arc. For simplicity, in Fig. 8 these thought extensions are approximated by the tangents (dotted lines) to the arc at the merging points. In Fig. 8, left, the bar is in vertical position (0 degrees). In analogy to the Poggendorff illusion, the tangents to the small segment will appear to be rotated towards the horizontal. As a result, one concludes the segment to be part of a smaller circle, so the circular lines don t seem to match. However, in case the occluding bar is tilted by 45 0, the tangents to the segments are oriented already nearly horizontally or vertically, respectively. Hardly any Fig. 8. Left. In analogy to the classic Poggendorff illusion, the imaginary extension of the circular line at the merging point (tangent; dashed black line) is rotated towards the horizontal. As a consequence, the small segment appears to be part of a smaller circle than the large one. Right. In case of a horizontal or vertical tangent, hardly any rotation of the thought extension will occur. The size of the small segment is perceived nearly correctly. rotation will occur (Fig. 8, right), again in analogy to classic Poggendorff illusion. For tilt angles of 8 0 and 82 0, there seems to be a strong tendency to rotate the thought continuation of the tangent off its true orientation. For simplicity, the dots positioned symmetrically at the edges of the bar (enhancing the illusion) have been omitted in the last figure. 4. Conclusion The divided circle illusion has been investigated. The perceived size of both protruding segments have been determined experimentally. While, on the average, the size of the large segment is estimated almost correctly, the size of the small segment is mostly underestimated. In a second experiment, the occluding bar has been rotated in steps between vertical and horizontal orientation. The illusion on the small segment nearly

11 disappears when the bar is oriented at 45 degrees. In addition, the perceived size of both segments decreases with increasing diameter of the large segment. This is ascribed to a size constancy effect. Citations Bouma, H, and Andriessen, J J (1968). Perceived orientation of isolated line segments. Vision Research, 8, I, 493-S07. Cornish, V (1937). Apparent Enlargement of the Sun at the time of Rising and Setting. Nature, 140, 1082-1083. Ehrenstein, W (1954). Probleme der ganzheitspsychologischen Wahrnehmungslehre. Fig., p. 154. Leipzig: Johann Ambrosius Barth /Verlag. Gilinsky, AS (1955). The Effect of Attitude upon the perception of size. The American Journal of Psychology, 68, 173-192. Greene, E, and Fisher, J (1994). Classical illusion effects with nonclassic stimuli: Angular induction from decomposing lines into point arrays. Perception & Psychophysics, 56, 575-589. Hotopf, WAN (1966). The size constancy theory of visual illusions. Br. J. Psychol. 57, 307-318. Kaufman, L, and Rock I (1962). The Moon Illusion. Scientific American, July, 120-130. Kreiner, WA (2004). Size Illusions as a Phenomenon of Limited Information Capacity. Z. Phys. Chem., 218, 1041-1061. Lingelbach, B (1984). Der Einfluss der Kontrastempfindlichkeit auf geometrisch optische Täuschungen. Deutsche Optiker Zeitung, 39, 46 53. Lingelbach, B. Die Poggendorff-Täuschung. http://www.leinroden.de/index.php?do=showtext&item=27. Lühr, K (1898). Die scheinbare Vergrößerung der Gestirne in der Nähe des Horizonts. Mitth. d. Ver. v. Freunden d. Astron. und kosm. Physik, 8, 31-35. Poggendorff, JC (18). Annalen der Physik, Ser. 2, 139, 513-546. Schur, E (1925). Mondtäuschung und Sehgrößenkonstanz. Psychologisch Forschung, 7, 44-. Spivey-Knowlton, M J and Bridgeman, B (1993). Spatial context affects the Poggendorff illusion, Perception & Psychophysics, 53, 467-474. Wenderoth, P, Beh, H, and White D (1978). Perceptual distortion of an oblique line in the presence of an abuttung vertical line. Vision Research, 18, 923-930.