Introduction to Inferential Statistics

Similar documents
Description: PUP Math World Series Location: David Brearley High School Kenilworth, NJ Researcher: Professor Carolyn Maher

Probability. March 06, J. Boulton MDM 4U1. P(A) = n(a) n(s) Introductory Probability

Randomness Exercises

Targets - Year 3. By the end of this year most children should be able to

Lesson 8: The Difference Between Theoretical Probabilities and Estimated Probabilities

Waiting Times. Lesson1. Unit UNIT 7 PATTERNS IN CHANCE

Multiplication and Probability

Heads Up! A c t i v i t y 5. The Problem. Name Date

saying the 5 times, 10 times or 2 times table Time your child doing various tasks, e.g.

Name Class Date. Introducing Probability Distributions

November 6, Chapter 8: Probability: The Mathematics of Chance

ECOSYSTEM MODELS. Spatial. Tony Starfield recorded: 2005

SAMPLING DISTRIBUTION MODELS TODAY YOU WILL NEED: PENCIL SCRATCH PAPER A PARTNER (YOUR CHOICE) ONE THUMBTACK PER GROUP Z-SCORE CHART

Probability Paradoxes

Unit 6: What Do You Expect? Investigation 2: Experimental and Theoretical Probability

EECS 203 Spring 2016 Lecture 15 Page 1 of 6

Algebra II Journal. Module 4: Inferences. Predicting the Future

MITOCW mit_jpal_ses06_en_300k_512kb-mp4

Lesson 3: Chance Experiments with Equally Likely Outcomes

Probability - Introduction Chapter 3, part 1

Probabilities and Probability Distributions

Geometric Distribution

Junior Circle Meeting 5 Probability. May 2, ii. In an actual experiment, can one get a different number of heads when flipping a coin 100 times?

CIS 2033 Lecture 6, Spring 2017

Whenever possible, ask your child to tell you the time to the nearest 5 minutes. Use a clock with hands as well as a digital watch or clock.

Random Variables. A Random Variable is a rule that assigns a number to each outcome of an experiment.

Random Variables. Outcome X (1, 1) 2 (2, 1) 3 (3, 1) 4 (4, 1) 5. (6, 1) (6, 2) (6, 3) (6, 4) (6, 5) (6, 6) }

Outcome X (1, 1) 2 (2, 1) 3 (3, 1) 4 (4, 1) 5 {(1, 1) (1, 2) (1, 3) (1, 4) (1, 5) (1, 6) (6, 1) (6, 2) (6, 3) (6, 4) (6, 5) (6, 6)}

Finding The Recipe For Success How failure helped me find the recipe for success in small business.

7.1 Chance Surprises, 7.2 Predicting the Future in an Uncertain World, 7.4 Down for the Count

First Tutorial Orange Group

The Coin Toss Experiment

Variations on the Two Envelopes Problem

Austin and Sara s Game

goals, objectives and the future Keeping track of your goals

COMPOUND EVENTS. Judo Math Inc.

1. How to identify the sample space of a probability experiment and how to identify simple events

JIGSAW ACTIVITY, TASK # Make sure your answer in written in the correct order. Highest powers of x should come first, down to the lowest powers.

Assignment 4: Permutations and Combinations

Find the probability of an event by using the definition of probability

Such a description is the basis for a probability model. Here is the basic vocabulary we use.

Unit 11 Probability. Round 1 Round 2 Round 3 Round 4

We all know what it means for something to be random. Or do

MEI Conference Short Open-Ended Investigations for KS3

1. More on Binomial Distributions

Mini-Unit. Data & Statistics. Investigation 1: Correlations and Probability in Data

Dominant and Dominated Strategies

1. Theoretical probability is what should happen (based on math), while probability is what actually happens.

PHYSICS 220 LAB #1: ONE-DIMENSIONAL MOTION

Simulations. 1 The Concept

Probability A = {(1,4), (2,3), (3,2), (4,1)},

BuildER s Guide. Overlap technique

log

Math 14 Lecture Notes Ch. 3.3

If a fair coin is tossed 10 times, what will we see? 24.61% 20.51% 20.51% 11.72% 11.72% 4.39% 4.39% 0.98% 0.98% 0.098% 0.098%

Common Core Math Tutorial and Practice

5- STEP BEACHBODY CHALLENGE GROUP INVITATION PROCESS

EE 307 Project #1 Whac-A-Mole

Sample Student Reflections on Persuasive Piece. Writing

When a number cube is rolled once, the possible numbers that could show face up are

Lesson 15.5: Independent and Dependent Events

STAT Chapter 14 From Randomness to Probability

Basic Probability Ideas. Experiment - a situation involving chance or probability that leads to results called outcomes.

How to get more quality clients to your law firm

Hypothesis Tests. w/ proportions. AP Statistics - Chapter 20

Probability is the likelihood that an event will occur.

Probability Models. Section 6.2

3.6 Theoretical and Experimental Coin Tosses

Math 7 Notes - Unit 11 Probability

Probability. 13 February Math 210G 13 February /21

The Fear Eliminator. Special Report prepared by ThoughtElevators.com

Session 12. MAKING DECISIONS Giving informed consent

Test 2 SOLUTIONS (Chapters 5 7)

Finite Mathematics MAT 141: Chapter 8 Notes

Rosen, Discrete Mathematics and Its Applications, 6th edition Extra Examples

Section 6.1 #16. Question: What is the probability that a five-card poker hand contains a flush, that is, five cards of the same suit?

Review Questions on Ch4 and Ch5

Lecture 6 Probability

Physics 131 Lab 1: ONE-DIMENSIONAL MOTION

Probability and Randomness. Day 1

CALCULATING SQUARE ROOTS BY HAND By James D. Nickel

10-4 Theoretical Probability

Probability. A Mathematical Model of Randomness

A C E. Answers Investigation 3. Applications. 12, or or 1 4 c. Choose Spinner B, because the probability for hot dogs on Spinner A is

Lesson 10: Using Simulation to Estimate a Probability

Everything You Wanted to Know About Contracts (But Were Afraid to Ask) Professor Monestier

Math 1313 Section 6.2 Definition of Probability

Section 7.3 and 7.4 Probability of Independent Events

Problem Set 9: The Big Wheel... OF FISH!

Rubber Hand. Joyce Ma. July 2006

or More Events Activities D2.1 Open and Shut Case D2.2 Fruit Machines D2.3 Birthdays Notes for Solutions (1 page)

Chapter 4: Probability and Counting Rules

Stat 20: Intro to Probability and Statistics

PhD Vlog Week 4 Eddie Procter. Location: Exeter

FREA BUCKLER: ON FOCUS, LIGHT, POSITIVITY AND POSSIBILITY

Math Exam 2 Review. NOTE: For reviews of the other sections on Exam 2, refer to the first page of WIR #4 and #5.

Math Exam 2 Review. NOTE: For reviews of the other sections on Exam 2, refer to the first page of WIR #4 and #5.

What is the Law of Attraction?

Grade 8 Math Assignment: Probability

STAT 155 Introductory Statistics. Lecture 11: Randomness and Probability Model

Transcription:

Introduction to Inferential Statistics Can Dolphins Communicate? (Source: Tintle et al.; 2012, John Wiley and Sons) A famous study from the 1960 s explored whether two dolphins (Doris and Buzz) could communicate abstract ideas. Researchers believed dolphins could communicate simple feelings like Watch out! or I m happy, but Dr. Jarvis Bastian wanted to explore whether they could also communicate in a more abstract way, much like humans do. To investigate this, Dr. Bastian spent many years training Doris and Buzz and exploring the limits of their communicative ability. During a training period lasting many months, Dr. Bastian placed buttons underwater on each end of a large pool two buttons for Doris and two buttons for Buzz. He then used an old automobile headlight as his signal. When he turned on the headlight and let it shine steadily, he intended for this signal to mean push the button on the right. When he let the headlight blink on and off, this was meant as a signal to push the button on the left. Every time the dolphins pushed the correct button, Dr. Bastian gave the dolphins a reward of some fish. Over time Doris and Buzz caught on and could earn their fish reward every time. Then Dr. Bastian made things a bit harder. Now, Buzz had to push his button before Doris. If they didn t push the buttons in the correct order no fish. After a bit more training, the dolphins caught on again and could get earn their fish reward every time. The dolphins were now ready to participate in the real study to examine whether they could communicate with each other. Dr. Bastian placed a large canvas curtain in the middle of the pool. (See Figure 1.1.) Doris was on one side of the curtain and could see the headlight, whereas Buzz was on the other side of the curtain and could not see the headlight. Dr. Bastian turned on the headlight and let it shine steadily. He then watched to see what Doris would do. After looking at the light, Doris swam near the curtain and began to whistle loudly. Shortly after that, Buzz whistled back and then pressed the button on the right he got it correct and so both dolphins got a fish. But this single attempt was not enough to convince Dr. Bastian that Doris had communicated with Buzz through her whistling. Dr. Bastian repeated the process several times, sometimes having the light blink (so Doris needed to let Buzz know to push the left button) and other times having it glow steadily (so Doris needed to let Buzz know to push the right button). He kept track of how often Buzz pushed the correct button. Figure 1.1: Depending whether or not the light was blinking or shown steadily, Doris had to communicate to Buzz as to which button to push. In one phase of the study, Dr. Bastian had Buzz attempt to push the correct button a total of 16 different times. In this phase of the study, Buzz pushed the correct button 15 out of 16 times. This means that the proportion of the time that Buzz was correct was

Think about it: Based on these data, do you think Buzz somehow knew which button to push? Is 15 out of 16 correct pushes convincing to you? Or do you think that Buzz could have just been guessing? How might you justify your answer? What are the two possible explanations for Buzz pushing the correct button most of the time? (1) (2) These are the two possible explanations to be evaluated. It s certainly possible that Buzz was just guessing and got lucky! But does this seem like a reasonable explanation to you? How would you argue against someone who thought this was the case? So how are we going to decide between these two possible explanations? One approach is to model the random process (repeated attempts to push the correct button) and then see whether the observed data is consistent with guessing. That is, is it the kind of result that we might expect to see just by chance if Buzz was just guessing? Suppose Buzz was simply guessing. What proportion of the time out of 16 tries would we expect him to be correct? To investigate, we can model guessing behavior using a coin flip to represent (or simulate) Buzz s choice of which button to push assuming he is just guessing. To generate this artificial data, we can let heads represent and let tails represent that. This gives Buzz a 50% chance of pushing the correct button. This can be used to represent the Buzz was just guessing or the random chance alone explanation. How do we simulate coin flipping with 16 flips of a coin? (1) Make up a sequence of 16 flips of a coin (pretend you re flipping a coin and write down H or T for each flip). Write your made-up sequence here: How many heads did you get in your made-up, random sequence? Is this result truly random? Explain.

(2) Use the Random Number Table in the textbook appendix (use Row #21) to get a sequence of 16 random numbers. Write those numbers down below. Then make the even numbers heads and the odd numbers tails and write that sequence underneath the number sequence. Is this sequence truly random? How many heads did you get in this sequence? What is the proportion of heads? (3) Now flip a coin 16 times and write down the result. How many heads did you get in this sequence? Is this result random? Find the proportion of heads in 16 flips. (4) Now simulate the 16 flips using the Rossman/Chance applet (see result below)

Using and evaluating the coin flip guessing model Because coin flipping is a random process, we know that we won t obtain the same number of heads with every set of 16 flips. But are some numbers of heads more likely than others? Does the distribution of the number of heads that result in 16 flips have a predictable long-run pattern? In particular, how much variability is there in our simulated statistics between repetitions (sets of 16 flips) just by random chance? In order to investigate these questions, we need to continue to flip our coin to get many, many sets of 16 flips (or many repetitions of the 16 choices where we are modeling Buzz is simply guessing each time). We did this, and Figure 1.4 shows what we found when we graphed the number of heads from each set of 16 coin flips. Here, the process of flipping a coin 16 times was repeated 1,000 times this number was chosen for convenience, but also appears to be large enough to give us a fairly accurate sense of the long-run behavior for the number of heads in 16 tosses. Figure 1.4: A dotplot showing 1000 repetitions of flipping a coin 16 times and counting the number of heads Each dot represents one set of 16 attempts by Buzz Let s think carefully about what the graph in Figure 1.4 shows. For this graph, one dot represents one set of 16 coin tosses and the variable, what we measure about each set of 16 coin tosses, is the number of heads. What outcomes (number of heads in 16 flips) are pretty common? What outcomes are more unusual? What outcomes are exceedingly rare? So what does this have to do with the dolphin communication study? Well, we said that we would flip a coin to simulate what could happen if Buzz was really just guessing each time he pushed the button in 16 attempts. We saw that getting results like 15 heads out of 16 never happened in our 1000 repetitions showing that 15 is a exceedingly rare outcome, far in the tail of the distribution of the simulated statistics, if Buzz is simply guessing. In short, even though we expect some variability in the results for different sets of 16 tosses, the pattern shown in this distribution indicates that an outcome of 15 heads is outside the typical chance variability we would expect to see when Buzz is simply guessing. In the actual study, Buzz really did push the correct button 15 times out of 16, an outcome that would rarely occur if Buzz was just guessing. So, our coin flip guessing model tells us that we have very strong evidence that Buzz was not just guessing to make his choices. Therefore, we don t believe the by chance alone explanation is a good one for Buzz. We instead believe the study result provides strong enough evidence to be statistically significant, not a result that happened by chance alone.

One of the most important aspects of research is showing that a result can be reproduced or an experiment can be replicated. Reproducibility is the ability of an entire analysis of an experiment or study to be duplicated, either by the same researcher or by someone else working independently, whereas reproducing an experiment is called replicating it. Another Doris and Buzz study One goal of statistical significance is to rule out random chance as a plausible explanation for what we have observed. We still need to worry about how well the study was conducted. For example, are we absolutely sure Buzz couldn t see the headlight around the curtain? Are we sure there was no pattern to which headlight setting was displayed that he might have detected? And of course we haven t completely ruled out random chance, he may have had an incredibly lucky day. But the chance of him being that lucky is so small, that we conclude that other explanations are more plausible or credible. One option that Dr. Bastian pursued was to re-do the study except now he replaced the curtain with a wooden barrier between the two sides of the tank, to ensure a more complete separation between the dolphins to see whether that would diminish the effectiveness of their communication. In this case, Buzz pushed the correct button only 16 out of 28 times. The variable is the same (whether or not Buzz pushed the correct button), but the number of trials has changed to 28 (the number of attempts). Think about it: For this phase of the study, do you think that Doris could tell Buzz which button to push, even under these conditions? Or is it believable that Buzz could have just been guessing? Quiz 2: Do the following with a partner. 1. What proportion of the time did Buzz choose correctly in this second study? Use the correct notation. 2. Is it plausible (believable) that Buzz was simply guessing in this set of attempts? What is your initial impression? 3. Use the Rossman/Chance One Proportion Applet to carry out a simulation like the one described for the first study and create a dotplot that shows the distribution of number of correct guesses. Make a rough sketch of the dotplot here: Sketch of dotplot: Sketch a line that shows the cutoff for 16 heads. 4. Based on the simulated results, is there enough evidence to say that Buzz is not just guessing? Explain your reasoning.