Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones.

Similar documents
Visual optics, rods and cones and retinal processing

Fundamental Optics of the Eye and Rod and Cone vision

Color Outline. Color appearance. Color opponency. Brightness or value. Wavelength encoding (trichromacy) Color appearance

The best retinal location"

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I

Color Perception. Color, What is It Good For? G Perception October 5, 2009 Maloney. perceptual organization. perceptual organization

Spectral colors. What is colour? 11/23/17. Colour Vision 1 - receptoral. Colour Vision I: The receptoral basis of colour vision

Visibility, Performance and Perception. Cooper Lighting

We have already discussed retinal structure and organization, as well as the photochemical and electrophysiological basis for vision.

Retina. Convergence. Early visual processing: retina & LGN. Visual Photoreptors: rods and cones. Visual Photoreptors: rods and cones.

Spatial Vision: Primary Visual Cortex (Chapter 3, part 1)

Image and video processing (EBU723U) Colour Images. Dr. Yi-Zhe Song

The Human Visual System. Lecture 1. The Human Visual System. The Human Eye. The Human Retina. cones. rods. horizontal. bipolar. amacrine.

What is Color. Color is a fundamental attribute of human visual perception.

The Photoreceptor Mosaic

Question From Last Class

Spatial Vision: Primary Visual Cortex (Chapter 3, part 1)

Digital Image Processing

Early Visual Processing: Receptive Fields & Retinal Processing (Chapter 2, part 2)

Color Perception. This lecture is (mostly) thanks to Penny Rheingans at the University of Maryland, Baltimore County

Color and perception Christian Miller CS Fall 2011

Slide 1. Slide 2. Slide 3. Light and Colour. Sir Isaac Newton The Founder of Colour Science

Further reading. 1. Visual perception. Restricting the light. Forming an image. Angel, section 1.4

Visual Perception of Images

CGT 511 Perception. Facts. Facts. Facts. When perceiving visual information

COLOR. and the human response to light

The human visual system

COLOR and the human response to light

The eye* The eye is a slightly asymmetrical globe, about an inch in diameter. The front part of the eye (the part you see in the mirror) includes:

CS 534: Computer Vision

Visual Perception. Jeff Avery

CS 544 Human Abilities

The eye, displays and visual effects

Human Visual System. Prof. George Wolberg Dept. of Computer Science City College of New York

approximately 10-3 cd/m² to 10 cd/m², i.e. from the scotopic light

Psych 333, Winter 2008, Instructor Boynton, Exam 1

Structure of the eye and retina

History of Computer Vision and Human Vision System

Why is blue tinted backlight better?

1 Human Color Vision

Retina. last updated: 23 rd Jan, c Michael Langer

The Visual System. Computing and the Brain. Visual Illusions. Give us clues as to how the visual system works

Additive. Subtractive

Sensation, Part 4 Gleitman et al. (2011), Chapter 4

Color. Bilkent University. CS554 Computer Vision Pinar Duygulu

Color and Perception

Digital Image Processing

III: Vision. Objectives:

Vision. The eye. Image formation. Eye defects & corrective lenses. Visual acuity. Colour vision. Lecture 3.5

Vision Science I Exam 1 23 September ) The plot to the right shows the spectrum of a light source. Which of the following sources is this

Mahdi Amiri. March Sharif University of Technology

iris pupil cornea ciliary muscles accommodation Retina Fovea blind spot

Colour. Why/How do we perceive colours? Electromagnetic Spectrum (1: visible is very small part 2: not all colours are present in the rainbow!

DIGITAL IMAGE PROCESSING (COM-3371) Week 2 - January 14, 2002

Reading. 1. Visual perception. Outline. Forming an image. Optional: Glassner, Principles of Digital Image Synthesis, sections

This question addresses OPTICAL factors in image formation, not issues involving retinal or other brain structures.

Multimedia Systems Color Space Mahdi Amiri March 2012 Sharif University of Technology

Colour. Electromagnetic Spectrum (1: visible is very small part 2: not all colours are present in the rainbow!) Colour Lecture!

Visual Perception. human perception display devices. CS Visual Perception

A World of Color. Session 4 Color Spaces. OLLI at Illinois Spring D. H. Tracy

Chapter Six Chapter Six

Color Perception and Applications. Penny Rheingans University of Maryland Baltimore County. Overview

Reflectance curves of some common foods. Spectral colors. What is colour? 11/4/11

Physiology of Vision The Eye as a Sense Organ. Rodolfo T. Rafael,M.D. Topics

Image Processing. Michael Kazhdan ( /657) HB Ch FvDFH Ch. 13.1

Simple reaction time as a function of luminance for various wavelengths*

Visual Perception. Overview. The Eye. Information Processing by Human Observer

A piece of white paper can be 1,000,000,000 times brighter in outdoor sunlight than in a moonless night.

Lecture 4. Opponent Colors. Hue Cancellation Experiment HUV Color Space

Visual Effects of Light. Prof. Grega Bizjak, PhD Laboratory of Lighting and Photometry Faculty of Electrical Engineering University of Ljubljana

AP PSYCH Unit 4.2 Vision 1. How does the eye transform light energy into neural messages? 2. How does the brain process visual information? 3.

Vision. PSYCHOLOGY (8th Edition, in Modules) David Myers. Module 13. Vision. Vision

Colour. Cunliffe & Elliott, Chapter 8 Chapman & Chapman, Digital Multimedia, Chapter 5. Autumn 2016 University of Stirling

Victor Ostromoukhov Université de Montréal. Victor Ostromoukhov - Université de Montréal

Visual System I Eye and Retina

excite the cones in the same way.

Reading. Lenses, cont d. Lenses. Vision and color. d d f. Good resources: Glassner, Principles of Digital Image Synthesis, pp

The Eye. Morphology of the eye (continued) Morphology of the eye. Sensation & Perception PSYC Thomas E. Van Cantfort, Ph.D

2 The First Steps in Vision

Structure and Measurement of the brain lecture notes

The Science Seeing of process Digital Media. The Science of Digital Media Introduction

Color & Graphics. Color & Vision. The complete display system is: We'll talk about: Model Frame Buffer Screen Eye Brain

Visual Effects of. Light. Warmth. Light is life. Sun as a deity (god) If sun would turn off the life on earth would extinct

Radiometry vs. Photometry. Radiometric and photometric units

The Special Senses: Vision

any kind, you have two receptive fields, one the small center region, the other the surround region.

Color. Color. Colorfull world IFT3350. Victor Ostromoukhov Université de Montréal. Victor Ostromoukhov - Université de Montréal

Contours, Saliency & Tone Mapping. Donald P. Greenberg Visual Imaging in the Electronic Age Lecture 21 November 3, 2016

Capturing Light in man and machine

Chapter 2: The Beginnings of Perception

12/02/2017. From light to colour spaces. Electromagnetic spectrum. Colour. Correlated colour temperature. Black body radiation.

Is the fovea vision only photopic?

CMPSCI 670: Computer Vision! Color. University of Massachusetts, Amherst September 15, 2014 Instructor: Subhransu Maji

Vision and Color. Reading. Optics, cont d. Lenses. d d f. Brian Curless CSE 557 Autumn Good resources:

Vision and Color. Brian Curless CSE 557 Autumn 2015

Color Science. What light is. Measuring light. CS 4620 Lecture 15. Salient property is the spectral power distribution (SPD)

Capturing Light in man and machine

Lecture 5. The Visual Cortex. Cortical Visual Processing

Color Assimilation and Contrast near Absolute Threshold

Capturing Light in man and machine

Transcription:

Achromatic and chromatic vision, rods and cones. Andrew Stockman NEUR3045 Visual Neuroscience

Outline Introduction Rod and cone vision Rod vision is achromatic How do we see colour with cone vision? Vision and visual pathways Achromatic and chromatic cone vision (colour and luminance)

Light 400-700 nm is important for vision

ROD AND CONE VISION

An inverted image is formed on the retina The retina is carpeted with lightsensitive rods and cones

Rods and cones Webvision

Human photoreceptors Cones Daytime, achromatic and chromatic vision 3 types Long-wavelengthsensitive (L) or red cone Middle-wavelengthsensitive (M) or green cone Short-wavelengthsensitive (S) or blue cone

Human photoreceptors Rods Achromatic night vision 1 type Rod Cones Daytime, achromatic and chromatic vision 3 types Long-wavelengthsensitive (L) or red cone Middle-wavelengthsensitive (M) or green cone Short-wavelengthsensitive (S) or blue cone

Why do we have rods and cones?

Our vision has to operate over an enormous range of 10 12 (1,000,000,000,000) levels Moonlight Sunlight Typical ambient light levels Starlight Indoor lighting Visual function Absolute rod threshold Cone threshold Rod saturation begins Damaging levels To cover that range we have two different types of photoreceptor...

Rods that are optimized for low light levels Cones that are optimized for higher light levels Moonlight Sunlight Typical ambient light levels Starlight Indoor lighting Visual function Absolute rod threshold Cone threshold Rod saturation begins Damaging levels Sensitive ROD SYSTEM Lower range Less sensitive CONE SYSTEM Upper range

Two systems Moonlight Sunlight Typical ambient light levels Starlight Indoor lighting Photopic retinal illuminance (log phot td) -4.3-2.4-0.5 1.1 2.7 4.5 6.5 8.5 Scotopic retinal illuminance (log scot td) -3.9-2.0-0.1 1.5 3.1 4.9 6.9 8.9 Visual function SCOTOPIC MESOPIC PHOTOPIC Absolute rod threshold Cone threshold Rod saturation begins Damage possible Scotopic levels (below cone threshold) where rod vision functions alone. A range of c. 10 3.5 Mesopic levels where rod and cone vision function together. A range of c. 10 3 Photopic levels (above rod saturation) where cone vision functions alone. A range of > 10 6

Rod vision Achromatic High sensitivity Poor detail and no colour Cone vision Achromatic and chromatic Lower sensitivity Detail and good colour

Facts and figures There are about 120 million rods. They are absent in the central 0.3 mm diameter area of the fovea, known as the fovea centralis. There are only about 6 to 7 million cones. They are much more concentrated in the fovea.

Rod and cone distribution 0.3 mm of eccentricity is about 1 deg of visual angle

At night, you have to look away from things to see them in more detail Rod density peaks at about 20 deg eccentricity

During the day, you have to look at things directly to see them in detail Cones peak at the centre of vision at 0 deg

Cone distribution and photoreceptor mosaics

Original photograph The human visual system is a foveating system Simulation of what we see when we fixate with cone vision Credit: Stuart Anstis, UCSD

Visual acuity gets much poorer with eccentricity Credit: Stuart Anstis, UCSD

The foveal region is magnified in the cortical (brain) representation

ROD AND CONE DIFFERENCES

Rod and cone differences can be demonstrated using several techniques, including visual psychophysics.

What is visual psychophysics? Psychophysicists study human vision by measuring an observer s performance on carefully chosen perceptual tasks. INPUT PROCESSING OUTPUT?? STIMULUS VISUAL SYSTEM PERCEPTION The idea is to work out what is going on inside the visual system from the relationship between the stimulus at the input and the response of the observer.

Rod-cone threshold sensitivity differences How might we measure them?

Rod and cone threshold versus intensity curves Rod-cone break

Rods are about one thousand times more sensitive than cones. They can be triggered by individual photons.

Spectral sensitivity differences

Threshold versus target wavelength measurements Incremental flash 10-deg eccentric fixation Intensity Space (x)

Threshold versus target wavelength measurements Incremental flash 10-deg eccentric fixation Intensity Space (x)

Threshold versus target wavelength measurements Incremental flash 10-deg eccentric fixation Intensity Space (x)

Threshold versus target wavelength measurements Incremental flash 10-deg eccentric fixation Intensity Space (x)

Rod and cone spectral sensitivity curves Plotted as thresholds versus wavelength curves

-1 Plotted as the more conventional spectral sensitivity curve Relative sensitivity (energy) -2-3 -4-5 Sensitivity = 1/threshold or log (sensitivity) = -log(threshold)

Approximate darkadapted photoreceptor sensitivities. 4 3 Rods Log 10 quantal sensitivity 2 1 0-1 -2 S L M -3 400 500 600 700 Wavelength (nm)

The Purkinje Shift A change in the relative brightness of colours as the light level changes because of the difference in spectral sensitivity between rod and cone vision (e.g., reds and oranges become darker as rods take over) Simulated: Dick Lyon & Lewis Collard at Wikimedia

Rod-cone dark adaptation curves Rod-cone break

Rod-cone dark adaptation curves Cone plateau Rods take much longer to recover after a bleach than cones From Hecht, Haig & Chase (1937)

Temporal differences

Suction electrode recording

Photocurrent responses Greater temporal integration improves rod sensitivity (but reduces temporal acuity)

Highest flicker rates that can just be seen (c.f.f.) FLICKER INVISIBLE Cones Rods FLICKER VISIBLE Photopically (cone) equated scale

Spatial differences (visual acuity)

Rod and cone visual acuities Visual acuity The acuity here is defined as the reciprocal value of the size of the gap (measured in arc minutes) that can be reliably identified. Visual acuity (mins of arc) 0.56 0.63 0.71 0.83 1.00 1.25 1.67 2.50 5.00 8 König (1897) Rods Rods

Rod and cone visual acuities 0.56 0.63 König (1897) Greater spatial integration improves rod sensitivity but reduces acuity The loss must be postreceptoral because the rods are smaller than cones in the periphery) Visual acuity (mins of arc) 0.71 0.83 1.00 1.25 1.67 2.50 5.00 Rods Rods 8

Rod vision is achromatic Why?

Vision at the photoreceptor stage is relatively simple because the output of each photoreceptor is: UNIVARIANT What does univariant mean?

UNIVARIANCE Crucially, the effect of any absorbed photon is independent of its wavelength. Rod Once absorbed a photon produces the same change in photoreceptor output whatever its wavelength.

UNIVARIANCE Crucially, the effect of any absorbed photon is independent of its wavelength. Rod So, if you monitor the rod output, you can t tell which colour of photon has been absorbed.

UNIVARIANCE Crucially, the effect of any absorbed photon is independent of its wavelength. Rod All the photoreceptor effectively does is to count photons.

UNIVARIANCE What does vary with wavelength is the probability that a photon will be absorbed. This is reflected in what is called a spectral sensitivity function.

Rod spectral sensitivity function (also known as the scotopic luminosity curve, CIE V λ ) Log relative sensitivity (energy units) 0-1 -2-3 -4-5 -6-7 CIE V' λ 400 500 600 700 800 Wavelength (nm) More sensitive Less sensitive

Rod spectral sensitivity function (V λ ) Logarithmic sensitivity plot Linear sensitivity plot Log relative sensitivity (energy units) 0-1 -2-3 -4-5 -6-7 CIE V' λ Much more detail at lower sensitivities 10 V log(v ) Relative sensitivity (energy units) 1.0 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0.0 CIE V' λ 400 500 600 700 800 400 500 600 700 800 Wavelength (nm) Wavelength (nm)

Rod spectral sensitivity function (V λ ) Log relative sensitivity (energy units) 0-1 -2-3 -4-5 -6-7 CIE V' λ 400 500 600 700 800 Wavelength (nm) In order of rod sensitivity: > > > > > > > >

Log relative sensitivity (energy units) 0-1 -2-3 -4-5 -6-7 CIE V' λ 400 500 600 700 800 Wavelength (nm) So, imagine you have four lights of the same intensity (indicated here by the height) The green will look brightest, then blue, then yellow and lastly the red will be the dimmest

Log relative sensitivity (energy units) 0-1 -2-3 -4-5 -6-7 CIE V' λ 400 500 600 700 800 Wavelength (nm) We can adjust the intensities to compensate for the sensitivity differences. When this has been done, the four lights will look completely identical.

Rod Changes in light intensity are confounded with changes in colour (wavelength)

UNIVARIANCE A change in photoreceptor output can be caused by a change in intensity or by a change in colour. There is no way of telling which. Colour or intensity change?? Each photoreceptor is therefore colour blind, and is unable to distinguish between changes in colour and changes in intensity.

A consequence of univariance is that we are colourblind when only one photoreceptor operates Examples: SCOTOPIC VISION, cone monochromacy

With three cone photoreceptors, our colour vision is trichromatic

Cone spectral sensitivities 0 Log 10 quantal sensitivity -1-2 S M L -3 400 450 500 550 600 650 700 Wavelength (nm)

So, if each photoreceptor is colourblind, how do we see colour? Or to put it another way: How is colour encoded?

TRICHROMACY A change in colour from green to red causes a relative increase in the L-cone output but causes a decrease in the M-cone output. M L A change in colour from red to green causes a relative increase in the M-cone output but causes a decrease in the L-cone output. M L Thus, colour can be encoded by comparing the outputs of different cone types

At the photoreceptors, colour is encoded by the relative cone outputs Blue light 0 Log 10 quantal sensitivity -1-2 S M L -3 400 450 500 550 600 650 700 Wavelength (nm)

Colour is encoded by the relative cone outputs Blue light 0 Log 10 quantal sensitivity -1-2 S M L Green light -3 400 450 500 550 600 650 700 Wavelength (nm)

Colour is encoded by the relative cone outputs Blue light 0 Log 10 quantal sensitivity -1-2 S M L Green light -3 Red light 400 450 500 550 600 650 700 Wavelength (nm)

Colour is encoded by the relative cone outputs Blue light Red light Green light Purple light Yellow light White light

POSTRECEPTORAL COLOUR VISION

But what happens next (i.e., how is colour encoded after the photoreceptors)?

Colour phenomenology Can provide clues about how colours are processed after the photoreceptors Which pairs of colours coexist in a single, uniform patch of colour? Which pairs never coexist? WHY?

Reddish-yellows?

Reddish-blues?

Reddish-greens?

Bluish-yellow?

The colour opponent theory of Hering Reds can get bluer or yellower but not greener

The colour opponent theory of Hering Yellows can get greener or redder but not bluer

The colour opponent theory of Hering Greens can get bluer or yellower but not redder

The colour opponent theory of Hering Blues can get greener or redder but not yellower

The colour opponent theory of Hering is opposed to R-G is opposed to Y-B How might this be related to visual processing after the cones?

Some ganglion cells are colour opponent Imagine that this is the region of space that the cell sees in the external world

Some ganglion cells are colour opponent A red light falling on the central area excites the cell (makes it fire faster)

Some ganglion cells are colour opponent A green light falling on the surround area inhibits the cell (makes it fire slower)

Some ganglion cells are colour opponent RED On-centre GREEN Off-surround

Some ganglion cells are colour opponent GREEN On-centre RED Off-surround

Red-green colour opponency Four variants Parvocellular pathway

Blue/yellow pathway Source: David Heeger Koniocellular pathway

Parvocellular pathways Koniocellular pathway

LGN cell responses LESS COMMON

Summary Trichromatic stage Colour opponent stage

So that s colour (chromatic) vision, but what about luminance (achromatic) vision?

Colour

ACHROMATIC COMPONENTS Split the image into... CHROMATIC COMPONENTS

CHROMATIC COMPONENTS By itself chromatic information provides relatively limited information

ACHROMATIC COMPONENTS Achromatic information important for fine detail

Achromatic and chromatic cone vision (colour and luminance)

In addition to neural pathways that signal colour there are also pathways that signal intensity or luminance:

Luminance is encoded by summing the L- and M-cone signals: L+M Blue light + Red light + L+M Green light + Purple light + L+M Yellow light + White light +

Colour is in many ways secondary to luminance

+

+

+

+

+

+

Rob van Lier, Mark Vergeer & Stuart Anstis

Boynton illusion

Boynton illusion

Boynton illusion

Boynton illusion

Watercolour effect

Neon Spreading www.blelb.com

Interesting artistic effects occur when vision depends only on colour (and not on luminance)

Detail from 'Plus Reversed', Richard Anuszkiewicz, 1960 from Viperlib

What are the postreceptoral neural substrates of the chromatic and luminance pathways?

Red-green chromatic pathways have been linked to the parvocellular retinal stream for L-M.

Parvocellular midget bipolars midget ganglion cell From Rodieck (1998)

midget ganglion cell L M These cells are chromatically opponent simply by virtue of the fact that they have single cone inputs to the centre of their receptive fields!

Blue-yellow chromatic pathways have been linked to the koniocellular stream

Koniocellular diffuse bipolar S-cone bipolar diffuse bipolar blue-yellow bistratified ganglion cell From Rodieck (1998)

Koniocellular diffuse bipolar S-cone bipolar diffuse bipolar blue-yellow bistratified ganglion cell From Rodieck (1998)

Luminance pathways, which produce achromatic percepts, have been linked to the magnocellular stream.

Magnocellular diffuse bipolar parasol ganglion cells From Rodieck (1998)

But the luminance pathways must be more than just the magnocellular stream. Why? Consider spatial acuity

Austin Roorda, 2004 CONE ARRAY ON RETINA To be able to resolve this E, the image must be sampled at enough points. The parvocellular pathway, with its one-to-one cone to bipolar connections, provides enough samples. The magnocellular pathway, with diffuse bipolar cells and many-to-one cone to bipolar connections, does not.

The parvocellular pathway must be double-duty supporting finely detailed luminance vision as well as more coarsley colour vision. Austin Roorda, 2004

Colour and luminance information are multiplexed in the parvocellular pathway

Chromatic pathways, which produce chromatic percepts, have been linked to the parvocellular retinal stream. Luminance pathways, which produce achromatic percepts, have been linked to the magnocellular stream, but also depend on the parvocellular stream.

Parvocellular pathway Koniocellular pathway Magnocellular pathway

Parvocellular pathway: High spatial frequencies (spatial detail) Low temporal frequencies Chromatic Lower contrast sensitivity Magnocellular pathway: High temporal frequencies (motion/flicker) Low spatial frequencies Achromatic Higher contrast sensitivity From Rodieck (1998)