Tone-in-noise detection: Observed discrepancies in spectral integration. Nicolas Le Goff a) Technische Universiteit Eindhoven, P.O.

Similar documents
A CLOSER LOOK AT THE REPRESENTATION OF INTERAURAL DIFFERENCES IN A BINAURAL MODEL

I. INTRODUCTION. NL-5656 AA Eindhoven, The Netherlands. Electronic mail:

The role of intrinsic masker fluctuations on the spectral spread of masking

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007

Interaction of Object Binding Cues in Binaural Masking Pattern Experiments

Document Version Publisher s PDF, also known as Version of Record (includes final page, issue and volume numbers)

Spectral and temporal processing in the human auditory system

The role of distortion products in masking by single bands of noise Heijden, van der, M.L.; Kohlrausch, A.G.

Psycho-acoustics (Sound characteristics, Masking, and Loudness)

2920 J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 102 (5), Pt. 1, November /97/102(5)/2920/5/$ Acoustical Society of America 2920

Modeling auditory processing of amplitude modulation II. Spectral and temporal integration Dau, T.; Kollmeier, B.; Kohlrausch, A.G.

A cat's cocktail party: Psychophysical, neurophysiological, and computational studies of spatial release from masking

The effect of noise fluctuation and spectral bandwidth on gap detection

III. Publication III. c 2005 Toni Hirvonen.

Influence of fine structure and envelope variability on gap-duration discrimination thresholds Münkner, S.; Kohlrausch, A.G.; Püschel, D.

Estimating critical bandwidths of temporal sensitivity to low-frequency amplitude modulation

Perception of pitch. Definitions. Why is pitch important? BSc Audiology/MSc SHS Psychoacoustics wk 4: 7 Feb A. Faulkner.

Intensity Discrimination and Binaural Interaction

INTRODUCTION. Address and author to whom correspondence should be addressed. Electronic mail:

Effect of fast-acting compression on modulation detection interference for normal hearing and hearing impaired listeners

Results of Egan and Hake using a single sinusoidal masker [reprinted with permission from J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 22, 622 (1950)].

Technical University of Denmark

Detection of Tones in Reproducible Noises: Prediction of Listeners Performance in Diotic and Dichotic Conditions

You know about adding up waves, e.g. from two loudspeakers. AUDL 4007 Auditory Perception. Week 2½. Mathematical prelude: Adding up levels

Modeling auditory processing of amplitude modulation I. Detection and masking with narrow-band carriers Dau, T.; Kollmeier, B.; Kohlrausch, A.G.

The relation between perceived apparent source width and interaural cross-correlation in sound reproduction spaces with low reverberation

Binaural Hearing. Reading: Yost Ch. 12

THE MATLAB IMPLEMENTATION OF BINAURAL PROCESSING MODEL SIMULATING LATERAL POSITION OF TONES WITH INTERAURAL TIME DIFFERENCES

Perception of pitch. Definitions. Why is pitch important? BSc Audiology/MSc SHS Psychoacoustics wk 5: 12 Feb A. Faulkner.

Monaural and binaural processing of fluctuating sounds in the auditory system

Measurement of the binaural auditory filter using a detection task

Perception of pitch. Importance of pitch: 2. mother hemp horse. scold. Definitions. Why is pitch important? AUDL4007: 11 Feb A. Faulkner.

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007

Assessing the contribution of binaural cues for apparent source width perception via a functional model

Modeling binaural signal detection

A binaural auditory model and applications to spatial sound evaluation

Computational Perception. Sound localization 2

Auditory modelling for speech processing in the perceptual domain

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007 AUDITORY EVOKED MAGNETIC FIELDS AND LOUDNESS IN RELATION TO BANDPASS NOISES

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

Effect of Harmonicity on the Detection of a Signal in a Complex Masker and on Spatial Release from Masking

Imperfect pitch: Gabor s uncertainty principle and the pitch of extremely brief sounds

Auditory filters at low frequencies: ERB and filter shape

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Rapid estimation of high-parameter auditory-filter shapes

Hearing and Deafness 2. Ear as a frequency analyzer. Chris Darwin

Predictions of diotic tone-in-noise detection based on a nonlinear optimal combination of energy, envelope, and fine-structure cues

AN AUDITORILY MOTIVATED ANALYSIS METHOD FOR ROOM IMPULSE RESPONSES

Enhancing and unmasking the harmonics of a complex tone

EC209 - Improving Signal-To-Noise Ratio (SNR) for Optimizing Repeatable Auditory Brainstem Responses

Complex Sounds. Reading: Yost Ch. 4

COM325 Computer Speech and Hearing

A Pole Zero Filter Cascade Provides Good Fits to Human Masking Data and to Basilar Membrane and Neural Data

Additive Versus Multiplicative Combination of Differences of Interaural Time and Intensity

A psychoacoustic-masking model to predict the perception of speech-like stimuli in noise q

I R UNDERGRADUATE REPORT. Stereausis: A Binaural Processing Model. by Samuel Jiawei Ng Advisor: P.S. Krishnaprasad UG

Acoustics, signals & systems for audiology. Week 9. Basic Psychoacoustic Phenomena: Temporal resolution

Document Version Publisher s PDF, also known as Version of Record (includes final page, issue and volume numbers)

The psychoacoustics of reverberation

Across frequency processing with time varying spectra

Distortion products and the perceived pitch of harmonic complex tones

NSMRL Report JULY 2001

Common principles of across-channel processing in monaural versus binaural hearing

Citation for published version (APA): Lijzenga, J. (1997). Discrimination of simplified vowel spectra Groningen: s.n.

Perceptual Sensitivity to High-Frequency Interaural Time Differences Created by Rustling Sounds

Spectro-Temporal Methods in Primary Auditory Cortex David Klein Didier Depireux Jonathan Simon Shihab Shamma

An auditory model that can account for frequency selectivity and phase effects on masking

A102 Signals and Systems for Hearing and Speech: Final exam answers

Pre- and Post Ringing Of Impulse Response

Research Note MODULATION TRANSFER FUNCTIONS: A COMPARISON OF THE RESULTS OF THREE METHODS

Exploiting envelope fluctuations to achieve robust extraction and intelligent integration of binaural cues

HCS 7367 Speech Perception

An evaluation of discomfort reduction based on auditory masking for railway brake sounds

AUDL GS08/GAV1 Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Loudness & Temporal resolution

Perception of low frequencies in small rooms

Computational Perception /785

Temporal Modulation Transfer Functions for Tonal Stimuli: Gated versus Continuous Conditions

DETERMINATION OF EQUAL-LOUDNESS RELATIONS AT HIGH FREQUENCIES

6.551j/HST.714j Acoustics of Speech and Hearing: Exam 2

A triangulation method for determining the perceptual center of the head for auditory stimuli

Temporal resolution AUDL Domain of temporal resolution. Fine structure and envelope. Modulating a sinusoid. Fine structure and envelope

Preface A detailed knowledge of the processes involved in hearing is an essential prerequisite for numerous medical and technical applications, such a

Experiments in two-tone interference

INVESTIGATING BINAURAL LOCALISATION ABILITIES FOR PROPOSING A STANDARDISED TESTING ENVIRONMENT FOR BINAURAL SYSTEMS

FFT 1 /n octave analysis wavelet

Signals & Systems for Speech & Hearing. Week 6. Practical spectral analysis. Bandpass filters & filterbanks. Try this out on an old friend

Jason Schickler Boston University Hearing Research Center, Department of Biomedical Engineering, Boston University, Boston, Massachusetts 02215

AUDITORY ILLUSIONS & LAB REPORT FORM

THE PERCEPTION OF ALL-PASS COMPONENTS IN TRANSFER FUNCTIONS

Auditory Based Feature Vectors for Speech Recognition Systems

The Human Auditory System

E ects of masker frequency and duration in forward masking: further evidence for the in uence of peripheral nonlinearity

Magnetic Tape Recorder Spectral Purity

EFFECT OF STIMULUS SPEED ERROR ON MEASURED ROOM ACOUSTIC PARAMETERS

ADC Based Measurements: a Common Basis for the Uncertainty Estimation. Ciro Spataro

PERFORMANCE COMPARISON BETWEEN STEREAUSIS AND INCOHERENT WIDEBAND MUSIC FOR LOCALIZATION OF GROUND VEHICLES ABSTRACT

Phase and Feedback in the Nonlinear Brain. Malcolm Slaney (IBM and Stanford) Hiroko Shiraiwa-Terasawa (Stanford) Regaip Sen (Stanford)

Introduction to cochlear implants Philipos C. Loizou Figure Captions

HRTF adaptation and pattern learning

Signals, Sound, and Sensation

Transcription:

Tone-in-noise detection: Observed discrepancies in spectral integration Nicolas Le Goff a) Technische Universiteit Eindhoven, P.O. Box 513, NL-5600 MB Eindhoven, The Netherlands Armin Kohlrausch b) and Steven van de Par c) Philips Research Laboratories, High Tech Campus 36, NL-5656 AE Eindhoven, The Netherlands d) Jeroen Breebaart e) Philips Research Laboratories, High Tech Campus 34, NL-5656 AE Eindhoven, The Netherlands (Dated: January 29, 2009) 1

Abstract Spectral integration for several tone-in-noise conditions is discussed and an experiment is conducted for both monaural (NoSo) and binaural conditions (NoSπ). Monaural detection thresholds for running-noise maskers increased for masker bandwidths up to the critical bandwidth and remained constant for larger bandwidths. Binaural conditions and monaural conditions with a frozen-noise masker revealed different spectral integration patterns that are monotonic for masker bandwidth below and beyond the critical bandwidth, an effect known as the apparently wider binaural critical band. Finally we show a different type of spectral integration obtained for binaural conditions with a reduced masker correlation (NρSπ) or for NoSπ with an overall interaural level difference. In these cases the integration patterns are non-monotonic with a maximum for masker bandwidths around the critical bandwidth. PACS numbers: 2

I. INTRODUCTION In the context of tone-in-noise detection, spectral integration refers to the dependency of the detection thresholds on the bandwidth of the noise masker. In order to simplify the description we consider situations where the power spectral density (spectral level) of the masker is kept constant for all bandwidths. Spectral integration was first formalized by the concept of the critical band proposed by Fletcher (1940). In this concept, thresholds for tones spectrally centered in a noise masker were increasing with increasing masker bandwidth up to a specific (the critical) bandwidth and remained constant for larger masker bandwidths. In subcritical situations the thresholds were increasing as the total energy of the masker. In supracritical situations thresholds remained constant because the filtering occurring on the basilar membrane removed all masker components outside the critical band. This view was later modified by Bos and de Boer (1966). They proposed a refinement for subcritical situations where it was found that besides energy principles that lead to integration rates of about 3 db/octave, the external masker variability could also be the limiting factor for running-noise maskers resulting in integration rates of about 1.5 db/octave. This approach was then extended to account for a phenomenon that was primarily observed for binaural conditions where the auditory filter appeared to be wider than that measured in monaural conditions (Sever and Small, 1979). In these conditions a monotonic increase of the thresholds is observed for bandwidths below and beyond the critical band. The phenomenon was explained by taking into account the contribution of information contained in off-frequency channels for conditions where the limiting factor for the detection process are the internal errors of the auditory system (van de a) Electronic address: n.legoff@tue.nl b) Electronic address: armin.kohlrausch@philips.com c) Electronic address: steven.van.de.par@philips.com d) Also at Technische Universiteit Eindhoven, P.O. Box 513, NL-5600 MB Eindhoven, The Netherlands e) Electronic address: jeroen.breebaart@philips.com 3

Par and Kohlrausch, 1999; Breebaart et al., 2000). In addition to those known spectral integration patterns, we will present experimental data that reveal a third pattern. In this type of spectral integration, thresholds are increasing with increasing masker bandwidths for subcritical situations and are decreasing for further increases of the masker bandwidths. This type of non-monotonic spectral integration was observed in binaural conditions where the binaural stimuli were presented with an overall interaural level difference (ILD) and in conditions with a reduced interaural correlation of the noise masker. II. EXPERIMENT Spectral integration was measured for several conditions of tone-in-noise detection. The signal had a frequency of 500 Hz and the noise masker was centered on this frequency. It had a bandwidth between 10 and 1000 Hz and was presented as either a running or frozen noise. The experiment included monaural and binaural conditions. In addition to these common conditions we also included binaural conditions with an overall ILD of 30 db and with a reduced interaural masker correlation. A. Method and stimuli A three-interval forced-choice procedure with adaptive signal-level adjustment was used to determine the thresholds. The three intervals of 300-ms duration were separated by pauses of 200 ms. A 200-ms sinusoid was added to the temporal center of one of the masker intervals. Feedback was provided to the subjects after each trial. The signal level was adjusted according to a two-down one-up rule tracking the 70.7% correct response level (Levitt, 1971). The initial step size for adjusting the level was 8 db. After each second reversal of the level track, the step size was halved until a step size of 1 db was reached. The run was then continued for another eight reversals. The median of the levels at these last eight reversals was calculated and used as a thresholds value. At least three threshold 4

values were obtained and averaged for each parameter value and subject. Three subjects participated in this experiment, among them two of the authors. All subjects had normal hearing. The noise masker was, unless stated otherwise, presented diotically (No) and the signal (500 Hz) was either presented diotically (So) or out-of-phase between the two ears (Sπ). The masker bandwidth was either 10, 20, 50, 100, 200, 500 or 1000 Hz. Bandwidths of 20, 50, 200 and 500 Hz were not measured for all conditions. For each masker bandwidth, the masker level was set to an overall sound pressure level of 65 db. For running-noise conditions, the noise samples for each interval were obtained by randomly selecting 300-ms segments from a two-channel, 2000-ms bandpass-noise buffer. The 2000-ms noise buffer was created as a white Gaussian noise in the time domain that was filtered to the desired bandwidth in the frequency domain. For frozen-noise conditions, only one fixed 300-ms noise sample was used in all three intervals of an entire run. These bandlimited noise samples were generated in the same way as the noise buffers for random noise conditions followed by a normalization of their rms value. To exclude the possibility that the frozen-noise thresholds would depend on the specific waveform token, a different frozen-noise sample was used for each run. Not all conditions were measured with frozen-noise maskers. The partially interaurally correlated (ρ=0.93) noise maskers were generated by combining two independent noise samples. In order to avoid spectral splatter, the signals and the maskers were gated with Hanning windows that had 50-ms onset and offset ramps. B. Results The graphical pattern of spectral integration or its absence depends on the experimental conditions and how the detection thresholds are represented. The present experiment was conducted with noise maskers that had a constant overall power regardless of their bandwidth. Consequently, a variation of the masker bandwidth is in fact a redistribution of the energy of the noise masker in the frequency domain, which will therefore lead to variation in 5

20 Threshold S/No [db] 15 10 5 0 5 10 10 100 1000 Bandwidth [Hz] FIG. 1. Monaural and binaural masked thresholds expressed as signal to spectral density ratios are shown as a function of the masker bandwidth. Filled symbols represent thresholds obtained for frozen-noise maskers and open symbols represent thresholds obtained with running-noise maskers. Upward triangles represent NoSo thresholds. Downward triangles represent NoSπ thresholds. Left pointing triangles represent thresholds obtained for NoSπ conditions with an ILD of 30 db. Diamonds represent NρSπ thresholds with an interaural masker correlation of 0.93. the spectral density of the noise. In order to study solely the effect of spectral expansion of the masker and not its level variation, thresholds are represented in terms of signal to noise spectral density ratios, which will give the same integration pattern as a representation of the thresholds in db SPL for an experiment conducted with a constant spectral level of the noise masker. Average thresholds for three subjects are shown in Fig. 1 as a function of the masker bandwidth. Running-noise conditions are represented by the open symbols and frozen-noise conditions by the filled symbols. The error bars denote the standard deviation across the complete data set for each condition. For masker bandwidths smaller than 1 ERB, about 78 Hz at 500 Hz (Glasberg and Moore, 1990), thresholds in all conditions are increasing with increasing masker bandwidth. On the contrary, for masker bandwidths wider than 1 ERB three different behaviors are observed; detection at constant signal to noise spectral den- 6

sity ratio, thresholds still increasing with increasing masker bandwidths and an uncommon behavior of thresholds decreasing with increasing masker bandwidths. Regarding the monaural conditions (upward triangles) we observe a thresholds change between masker bandwidths of 10 and 100 Hz of 1.3 db/octave and 3.2 db/octave for the running-noise (open symbols) and the frozen-noise masker (filled symbols) respectively. In the case of the frozen-noise masker the spectral integration corresponds to the variation of energy of the noise masker within the auditory filter, where a doubling of the bandwidth results in a 3 db increase of the detection thresholds for bandwidths up to 1 ERB. In the case of the running-noise masker the integration rate is close to 1.5 db/octave which fits with the assumption that detection is limited in this case by the variability of the noise masker on a per sample basis (Bos and de Boer, 1966). For masker bandwidths wider than 1 ERB, we observe no further spectral integration which is in line with the energy masking principles and the concept of the auditory filter. Some minor spectral integration can arguably be seen for the frozen-noise masker thresholds. This phenomenon has been previously reported (Breebaart et al., 2000) and related to the apparently wider auditory filter known from binaural conditions (van de Par and Kohlrausch, 1999). The phenomenon of an apparently wider critical band is particularly visible for binaural conditions (NoSπ, downward triangles) where one can clearly see that for both running- and frozen-noise maskers the increase in thresholds is monotonic below and beyond the auditory filter bandwidth. The thresholds for these binaural conditions are very similar for both types of noise masker. The spectral integration appears to be stronger for bandwidths smaller than 1 ERB, about 2.5 db/octave, and weaker for bandwidths larger than 1 ERB and amounts to about 0.7 db/octave. The subcritical value is in good agreement with the hypothesis that detection requires a constant change in the stimulus correlation. This happens at a constant signal-to-noise ratio and would ideally give a gain of 3 db/octave. Such a behavior is expected if detection is not limited by external variability but internal noise in the auditory system. The monotonic increase of the thresholds beyond the critical bandwidth or in other words, the apparent wider critical bandwidth has been explained by assuming the 7

contribution of off-frequency auditory filters to the detection for conditions in which detection is limited by internal errors (van de Par and Kohlrausch, 1999; Breebaart et al., 2000). When the masker is narrower than 1 ERB the phenomenon is not particularly visible in the spectral integration pattern, but as the masker bandwidth slowly increases beyond 1 ERB, the information in the off-frequency auditory filters becomes gradually unusable (covered by the masker), resulting in minor spectral integration beyond 1 ERB. The phenomenon also occurs to some extent for monaural conditions with a frozen-noise masker but not with a running-noise masker. Effectively, as seen previously, in monaural conditions with runningnoise the detection process is limited by the external variability of the masker waveform, while in conditions with frozen-noise the process is, similarly to binaural conditions (NoSπ), limited by internal limitations. Detection thresholds for binaural conditions with additional overall ILD (left-pointing triangles) or a reduced masker correlation (open diamonds) lie between those obtained for monaural conditions (NoSo) and the binaural condition (NoSπ). Likewise they elicit spectral integration that also lies between patterns given by a detection process limited by external variability (increase of 1.5 db/octave) and internal errors (increase of 3 db/octave) for bandwidths smaller than 1 ERB. The thresholds obtained for the NoSπ condition with an overall ILD of 30 db are clearly higher than those obtained for the plain NoSπ condition and are below the thresholds obtained for the monaural conditions. The thresholds are similar for both types of noise maskers. The integration rate is about 2.9 db/octave. The spectral integration obtained for the NoSπ condition with a reduced masker correlation is about 1.7 db/octave for masker bandwidths smaller than 1 ERB. In both cases an extension of the masker bandwidth beyond 1 ERB leads to a decrease in thresholds. Conditions with an overall ILD show a small decrease of about 1.6 db for an increase of the bandwidth from 100 Hz to 1000 Hz. For the same bandwidths the decrease is about 4.3 db for the thresholds obtained for the NoSπ condition with a reduced masker correlation. A comparison of our data with other running-noise measurements adapted from the literature is shown in Fig. 2. Regarding the NoSπ conditions (downward triangles), our data 8

15 Threshold S/No [db] 10 5 0 5 10 10 100 1000 Bandwidth [Hz] FIG. 2. Binaural masked thresholds expressed as signal to spectral density ratios are shown as a function of the masker bandwidth for running-noise maskers. Data are represented with the same convention of symbol types as in Fig. 1. The continuous lines show our data, the dashed lines represent data from Breebaart and Kohlrausch (2001); data adapted from van der Heijden and Trahiotis (1998) are shown as dot-dashed lines. (continuous lines), the data from Breebaart and Kohlrausch (2001) (dashed lines) and the data from van der Heijden and Trahiotis (1998) (dot-dashed lines) all show a monotonic increase in thresholds, including masker bandwidths beyond 1 ERB. Regarding the NoSπ conditions with a reduced masker correlation (all diamonds) our data and those from Breebaart and Kohlrausch (2001) are obtained for similar experimental conditions and the same masker correlation of 0.93, those from van der Heijden and Trahiotis (1998) are taken from a data set as the best fit to our own data set which was found for a masker correlation of 0.87. For this condition, we also see a common behavior across the three data sets: thresholds are increasing with increasing masker bandwidth up to about 1 ERB and decrease for further extension of the bandwidth. 9

15 Threshold S/No [db] 10 5 0 5 10 10 100 1000 Bandwidth [Hz] FIG. 3. Binaural masked thresholds expressed as signal to spectral noise density ratios are shown as a function of the masker bandwidth for running-noise maskers. Symbol types are used with the same convention as for Fig. 1. Open symbols represent experimental data, and filled symbols represent the equivalent simulated thresholds III. DISCUSSION While the various spectral integration patterns that we observed for the monaural (NoSo) and the binaural conditions (NoSπ) have been reported and modeled previously, that of the binaural condition with either an overall ILD or a reduced masker correlation still needs to be fitted in a model approach. Figure 3 shows a comparison of the behavior of the listeners and the model proposed by Breebaart et al. (2001a). The open symbols represent the experimental data and the filled symbols represent the equivalent simulated thresholds. One can see that the model accurately predicts the binaural thresholds (NoSπ), shown by the downward triangles. Especially the wider apparent auditory filter is predicted due the capacity of the model to integrate information from both on-frequency and off-frequency auditory filters. However the model is not able to predict the unusual spectral integration that is observed for the binaural condition with overall ILD (left-pointing triangles) or reduced masker correlation (diamonds). The prediction for these conditions are however correct for the widest masker bandwidth. Effectively for a masker bandwidth of 1000 Hz, 10

the model prediction for both cases rises by an amount from the base NoSπ condition that is in line with the experimental data. In the model this dependence results from an increase of model activity in the internal representation of the reference intervals that is due to the overall ILD or the masker decorrelation that prevent a total cancellation of activity in the binaural processor. A reduction of the masker bandwidth to 1 ERB and smaller values makes the model following the same behavior as for the base NoSπ condition (wider apparent critical band) and therefore predicts the same type of spectral integration. Consequently, it is unlikely that the non-monotonic spectral integration that is observed for the binaural condition with either an overall ILD or a reduction in the masker correlation could be explained in terms of off-frequency listening or across-frequency integration. This sort of non-monotonic spectral integration has been reported for several other experimental conditions. To stay in the field of tone-in-noise detection, three studies on the monaural detection of a short, high-frequency tone in a noise masker reported the same type of non-monotonic spectral integration. Figure 3 in Oxenham (1998) shows (a) an increase of detection thresholds of a 6-kHz tone for masker bandwidths increasing from 70 Hz up to 1200 Hz and (b) a decrease of thresholds for wider masker bandwidths. Thresholds are reported for 3 signal durations (2, 20, and 300 ms, masker duration 500 ms) and the effect is stronger for the shortest durations. The author comments that it is not clear what mechanism should underlie this result (Oxenham, 1998, pg. 1037). For similar experimental conditions (4-kHz signal, 10-ms duration), Bacon and Smith (1991) also reported that detection thresholds decrease by about 2.5 db for masker bandwidths wider than 1 ERB. Likewise Wright (1997) reported that detection thresholds for a 20-ms (noise) signal centered around 2500 Hz decreased by about 2 db as the masker bandwidth increased from 1000 to 8000 Hz. Another case of non-understood spectral integration for supracritical situations was reported by Gabriel and Colburn (1981). They conducted, among others, an experiment to measure interaural correlation detection as a function of the stimulus bandwidth at a reference correlation of 1. One can see in their Figure 4 that thresholds increase with increasing 11

bandwidth beyond the critical band. They comment that this phenomenon is not consistent with the concept of optimal processing. This remark is in line with simulations reported in Breebaart et al. (2001b) of these conditions. The model has a central processor which is an optimal detector and, as can be seen in their fig. 3, it predicts a monotonic improvement of the performance with increasing bandwidth but certainly not a decrease of performance. The fact that both these conditions by Gabriel and Colburn (1981) and our NoSπ conditions with a reduced masker correlation involves a major role of correlation in the discrimination process could suggest that the unexpected dependence of the thresholds beyond 1 ERB is somehow related to the influence of interaural decorrelation. IV. CONCLUSIONS To conclude this overview on spectral integration, we observe that for subcritical situations, spectral integration for all conditions varies between what one would predict based on a pure energy integration (frozen-noise NoSo and running- and frozen-noise NoSπ, gain of 3 db/octave) and what one would predict on a more statistical approach (running-noise NoSo, gain of 1.5 db/octave). For supracritical conditions we observe several behaviors for thresholds as a function of masker bandwidth: (a) constant thresholds (running-noise NoSo), (b) minor spectral integration, reflecting the effect of the apparent wider auditory filter (frozen- and running-noise NoSπ and frozen-noise NoSo), (c) the non-monotonic case of spectral integration where thresholds decrease again for bandwidths beyond 1 ERB (NoSπ with overall ILD or reduced masker correlation). This third situation can not be predicted with a model based on optimal detection and can also not be explained in terms of offfrequency listening. One possibility could be, as it has been suggested previously in a different approach by van der Heijden and Trahiotis (1998), to replace the internal noise that is currently defined in the model by Breebaart et al. (2001a) as independent in each individual auditory channel by a bandwidth-dependent internal noise. 12

References Bacon, S. P. and Smith, M. A. (1991). Spectral, intensive, and temporal factors influencing overshoot, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 43A, 373 400. Bos, C. E. and de Boer, E. (1966). Masking and discrimination, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 39, 708 715. Breebaart, J. and Kohlrausch, A. (2001). The influence of interaural stimulus uncertainty on binaural signal detection, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 109, 331 345. Breebaart, J., van de Par, S., and Kohlrausch, A. (2000). An explanation for the apparently wider critical bandwidth in binaural experiments, in Proceeding of the 12th International Symposium on Hearing. Breebaart, J., van de Par, S., and Kohlrausch, A. (2001a). Binaural processing model based on contralateral inhibition I. Model structure, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 110, 1074 1088. Breebaart, J., van de Par, S., and Kohlrausch, A. (2001b). Binaural processing model based on contralateral inhibition III. Dependence on temporal parameters, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 110, 1105 1117. Fletcher, H. (1940). Auditory patterns, Reviews of Modern Physics 12, 47 65. Gabriel, K. J. and Colburn, H. (1981). Interaural correlation discrimination: I. bandwidth and level dependence, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 69, 1394 1401. Glasberg, B. R. and Moore, B. C. J. (1990). Derivation of auditory filter shapes from notched noise data, Hearing Research 47, 103 138. Levitt, H. (1971). Transformed up-down methods in psychoacoustics, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 49, 467 477. Oxenham, A. J. (1998). Temporal integration at 6 khz as a function of masker bandwidth, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 103, 1033 1042. Sever, J. and Small, A. (1979). Binaural critical masking bands, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 66, 1343 1350. 13

van de Par, S. and Kohlrausch, A. (1999). Dependence of the binaural masking level differences on center frequency, masker bandwidth, and interaural parameters, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 106, 1940 1947. van der Heijden, M. and Trahiotis, C. (1998). Binaural detection as a function of interaural correlation and bandwidth of masking noise: Implications for estimates of spectral resolution, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 103, 1609 1614. Wright, B. A. (1997). Detectability of simultaneously masked signals as a function of masker bandwidth and configuration for different signal delays, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 101, 420 429. 14

List of Figures FIG. 1 Monaural and binaural masked thresholds expressed as signal to spectral density ratios are shown as a function of the masker bandwidth. Filled symbols represent thresholds obtained for frozen-noise maskers and open symbols represent thresholds obtained with running-noise maskers. Upward triangles represent NoSo thresholds. Downward triangles represent NoSπ thresholds. Left pointing triangles represent thresholds obtained for NoSπ conditions with an ILD of 30 db. Diamonds represent NρSπ thresholds with an interaural masker correlation of 0.93......................... 6 FIG. 2 Binaural masked thresholds expressed as signal to spectral density ratios are shown as a function of the masker bandwidth for running-noise maskers. Data are represented with the same convention of symbol types as in Fig. 1. The continuous lines show our data, the dashed lines represent data from Breebaart and Kohlrausch (2001); data adapted from van der Heijden and Trahiotis (1998) are shown as dot-dashed lines................. 9 FIG. 3 Binaural masked thresholds expressed as signal to spectral noise density ratios are shown as a function of the masker bandwidth for running-noise maskers. Symbol types are used with the same convention as for Fig. 1. Open symbols represent experimental data, and filled symbols represent the equivalent simulated thresholds............................... 10 15