Poles for Increasing the Sensibility of Vertical Gradient. in a Downhill Road

Similar documents
Magnification rate of objects in a perspective image to fit to our perception

CAN WE BELIEVE OUR OWN EYES?

Experimental Study of Infrastructure Radar Modulation for. Vehicle and Pedestrian Detection

Psychophysics of night vision device halo

Evaluation of High Intensity Discharge Automotive Forward Lighting

The Persistence of Vision in Spatio-Temporal Illusory Contours formed by Dynamically-Changing LED Arrays

FEATURE. Adaptive Temporal Aperture Control for Improving Motion Image Quality of OLED Display

The Perception of Optical Flow in Driving Simulators

A Vehicle Speed Measurement System for Nighttime with Camera

Module 2. Lecture-1. Understanding basic principles of perception including depth and its representation.

SURVEYING 1 CE 215 CHAPTER -3-

Development of Gaze Detection Technology toward Driver's State Estimation

The shape of luminance increments at the intersection alters the magnitude of the scintillating grid illusion

Combined effects of low frequency vertical vibration and noise on whole-body vibration sensation

Limitations of the Oriented Difference of Gaussian Filter in Special Cases of Brightness Perception Illusions

DEVELOPMENT OF A MICROSCOPIC TRAFFIC SIMULATION MODEL FOR INTERACTIVE TRAFFIC ENVIRONMENT

Illusory size-speed bias: Could this help explain motorist collisions with railway trains and other large vehicles?

Eye catchers in comics: Controlling eye movements in reading pictorial and textual media.

The application of Work Domain Analysis (WDA) for the development of vehicle control display

Field experiment on ground-to-ground sound propagation from a directional source

DEVELOPMENT OF AN ALGORITHM OF AUTOMATICALLY SETTING CRITICAL SPEEDS ON URBAN EXPRESSWAYS

Work Domain Analysis (WDA) for Ecological Interface Design (EID) of Vehicle Control Display

Discrimination of Virtual Haptic Textures Rendered with Different Update Rates

Spatial Judgments from Different Vantage Points: A Different Perspective

SOUND: A Traffic Simulation Model for Oversaturated Traffic Flow on Urban Expressways

Three elemental illusions determine the Zöllner illusion

TRAFFIC SIGN DETECTION AND IDENTIFICATION.

DIFFERENCE BETWEEN A PHYSICAL MODEL AND A VIRTUAL ENVIRONMENT AS REGARDS PERCEPTION OF SCALE

Evaluation based on drivers' needs analysis

Evaluating a Signal Control System Using a Real-time Traffic Simulator Connected to a Traffic Signal Controller

THE SCHOOL BUS. Figure 1

Image Characteristics and Their Effect on Driving Simulator Validity

Patterns and Graphing Year 10

Review of Probability

THE EXPANSION OF DRIVING SAFETY SUPPORT SYSTEMS BY UTILIZING THE RADIO WAVES

Evaluation of Actuated Right Turn Signal Control Using the ITS Radio Communication System

Characteristics of the Visual Perception under the Dark Adaptation Processing (The Lighting Systems for Signboards)

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

Guide to Basic Composition

A new position detection method using leaky coaxial cable

Chapter 2: PRESENTING DATA GRAPHICALLY

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN FILL-DEPTHS BASED ON GIS ESTIMATION, EARTHQUAKE DAMAGE AND THE MICRO-TREMOR PROPERTY OF A DEVELOPED HILL RESIDENTIAL AREA

Chapter 8: Perceiving Motion

Visibility based on eye movement analysis to cardinal direction

A reduction of visual fields during changes in the background image such as while driving a car and looking in the rearview mirror

A Road Traffic Noise Evaluation System Considering A Stereoscopic Sound Field UsingVirtual Reality Technology

VIRTUAL FIGURE PRESENTATION USING PRESSURE- SLIPPAGE-GENERATION TACTILE MOUSE

Maps and map interpretation An introduction for geoscientists

The Influence of the Noise on Localizaton by Image Matching

The Shape-Weight Illusion

The Mona Lisa Effect: Perception of Gaze Direction in Real and Pictured Faces

Close-Range Photogrammetry for Accident Reconstruction Measurements

D) visual capture. E) perceptual adaptation.

Discriminating direction of motion trajectories from angular speed and background information

The Hudson River Illusion: How the Optical Illusions behind Anti-Gravity Hills Affects the River that Flows Two Ways

This article reprinted from: Linsenmeier, R. A. and R. W. Ellington Visual sensory physiology.

The Effect of Opponent Noise on Image Quality

Assessments of Grade Crossing Warning and Signalization Devices Driving Simulator Study

COPYRIGHTED MATERIAL. Overview

First-order structure induces the 3-D curvature contrast effect

Grade Pellissippi State Middle School Mathematics Competition Funded by ORAU 1. Pellissippi State. Middle School Mathematics Competition

COPYRIGHTED MATERIAL OVERVIEW 1

Chapter 1 Virtual World Fundamentals

Optimization of an Acoustic Waveguide for Professional Audio Applications

Egocentric reference frame bias in the palmar haptic perception of surface orientation. Allison Coleman and Frank H. Durgin. Swarthmore College

Gestalt Principles of Visual Perception

THE EFFECTS OF PC-BASED TRAINING ON NOVICE DRIVERS RISK AWARENESS IN A DRIVING SIMULATOR

Directional Bias in the Perception of Cast Shadows

76-GHz High-Resolution Radar for Autonomous Driving Support

Development of Informal Communication Environment Using Interactive Tiled Display Wall Tetsuro Ogi 1,a, Yu Sakuma 1,b

Dynamic Visual Performance of LED with Different Color Temperature

Argos Ingegneria S.p.A. October 2009

EYE MOVEMENT STRATEGIES IN NAVIGATIONAL TASKS Austin Ducworth, Melissa Falzetta, Lindsay Hyma, Katie Kimble & James Michalak Group 1

The influence of changing haptic refresh-rate on subjective user experiences - lessons for effective touchbased applications.

Putting The Hole Thing Together

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Surface Contents Author Index

Pull Down Menu View Toolbar Design Toolbar

Häkkinen, Jukka; Gröhn, Lauri Turning water into rock

Perceived depth is enhanced with parallax scanning

Development of A Finger Mounted Type Haptic Device Using A Plane Approximated to Tangent Plane

GROUPING BASED ON PHENOMENAL PROXIMITY

A Research of The attractiveness of visual Information in Railway Station

Simultaneous presentation of tactile and auditory motion on the abdomen to realize the experience of being cut by a sword

Electromagnetic Stimulation of Transport in Water for Geoenvironmental Applications

CMOS Image Sensor for High Speed and Low Latency Eye Tracking

A WARNING SYSTEM FOR OVERSPEED AT THE CORNER USING VISIBLE LIGHT BASED ROAD-TO-VEHICLE COMMUNICATION

Size Illusion on an Asymmetrically Divided Circle

Digital inertial algorithm for recording track geometry on commercial shinkansen trains

Discussion and Application of 3D and 2D Aperture Problems

Self-motion perception from expanding and contracting optical flows overlapped with binocular disparity

HAND-SHAPED INTERFACE FOR INTUITIVE HUMAN- ROBOT COMMUNICATION THROUGH HAPTIC MEDIA

Development of a Finger Mounted Type Haptic Device Using a Plane Approximated to Tangent Plane

Thinking About Psychology: The Science of Mind and Behavior 2e. Charles T. Blair-Broeker Randal M. Ernst

Fig.2 the simulation system model framework

Evaluation of Millimeter wave Radar using Stepped Multiple Frequency Complementary Phase Code modulation

Technical Notes Volume 1, Number 25. Using HLA 4895 modules in arrays: system controller guidelines

Takeharu Seno 1,3,4, Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2, Stephen Palmisano 5 1

/15/$ IEEE

Perceptual Organization. Unit 3 RG 4e

Transcription:

Poles for Increasing the Sensibility of Vertical Gradient 1 Graduate School of Science and Engineering, Yamaguchi University 2-16-1 Tokiwadai,Ube 755-8611, Japan r007vm@yamaguchiu.ac.jp in a Downhill Road Shunsuke Kakumori 1, Atsushi Osa 2, Ken Matsuda 3,Hidetoshi Miike 4 2 Graduate School of 3 Graduate School of Science and Engineering, Science and Engineering, Yamaguchi University Yamaguchi University 2-16-1 Tokiwadai,Ube 2-16-1 Tokiwadai,Ube 755-755-8611, Japan 8611, Japan osaa@yamaguchi-u.ac.jp matsuken@yamaguchiu.ac.jp 4 Graduate School of Science and Engineering, Yamaguchi University 2-16-1 Tokiwadai,Ube 755-8611, Japan miike@yamaguchiu.ac.jp ABSTRACT Excessive speed is one of major causes of traffic accidents, and it leads to traffic fatality with high probability. In a previous study, it has reported that drivers tend to perceive that the load they travel was horizontal, although they travel on a downhill road. If drivers perceive true vertical gradient more sensitively, we expect that the drivers reduce their car speed on the downhill road. The study also said that road poles on both sides of the road were useful to perceive vertical gradient of the road. The purpose of this study is to find the best way of the pole s form and formation. In experiments, we used a video simulator that can be rendered in real-time computer graphics (CG). Through three experiments, the pole tilted 60 deg to frontward direction, 1.7 or 2.0 m length, was the most effective for perception of vertical gradient of the downhill road. Keywords Road pole, Visual illusion, Excessive speed, Driving simulator INTRODUCTION Current Car Accidents Have you experienced to misperceive a gradient in downhill roads? Have you experienced to drive a car faster speed than that you recognized on the downhill road? According to a report about traffic accidents by the metropolitan police department in Japan, the number of traffic accidents in 2009 was 736,688, the number of the injured was 915,092, the number of the deaths was 4,914 [1]. When cars are driven by high speed, the probability of fatal accident becomes very high. The rates of deaths in car accidents at vehicle speeds under 50 km/h, over 50km/h, over 80km/h were 0.4%, 5.1%, and 28.3% [2], respectively. The rate of deaths when cars were driven over 80 km/h speed becomes 49.7 times of the cases when cars were driven under 80 km/h speed. Reducing car speed leads to decrease fatal car accidents. The Obake-zaka Drivers tend to perceive that the load they travel was horizontal, although they travel on a downhill road. This phenomenon is well known as Obake-zaka in Japan. This is a kind of visual illusion. For example, a car is going a downward road like Figure 1. The gradient of road changes slightly at a location on the road. In this situation, drivers tend to see an illusionary upslope like Figure 2 [3]. If drivers perceive true vertical gradient more sensitively, we expect that the drivers reduce their car speed on the road. It is important that drivers can judge the gradient correctly. Figure1. Real gradient change in a road Figure2. Illusory perceived gradient change in a load Previous Study and Our Purpose In a previous research [4], our research group reported that road poles on both sides of the road were useful to perceive vertical gradient of the road. In the experiments,

we created a driving simulator that represented the Obake-zaka with road poles. We tried to install the poles in the following 4 patterns. 1. Nothing. (no-p.) 2. Poles with the same height. (same-p.) 3. Poles with gradually increasing height from near to far. (inc.-p) 4. Poles with gradually increasing height which has 2 cycles in the downhill road, which seems a staircase. (staircase-p) 14 participants observed the simulator, and judged whether the road is a horizontal road or a downhill. The gradient of the road was selected from 0 to 5%. The participants could judge downward gradient with high probability when the downward was high gradient. In the case of no-p, the 50% level of the probability was reported at 2.5% gradient. In the cases of same-p, inc.-p, and staircase-p, the 50% levels of the probability were 2.1, 2.0, and 1.5 % gradients, respectively. The previous study is not enough to research the effect of road poles. The purpose of this study is to find the best way of the pole s form and formation. speed of the simulator was adjusted to 80 km/h which was assumed an excessive speed. The total distance of the road was 290m. The simulator started a horizontal road for 35 m distance, and the road became a gentle downward gradient (7% gradient) to 80 m distance (see Figure 5). Then, the road from 115 m to 175 m was selected form 6 types of gradient (0~5%). We call this section as the test section. Two white lines were drawn at the start point and the end point of the test section. From 245 m to the finish, the road was horizontal. Therefore, the road from 80 m to 115 m, and the road from 175 m to 245 m were generated automatically in order to connect the gradient of the roads. Because the previous study showed that the probability to detect downward was almost 100% in 4~6% gradient downward, we used 0~5% as gradient variation. The sight around the road was a kind of hill in order to avoid information of road gradient. Road poles were installed in the test section, from 85 m to 175 m distance. 10 poles lined up on each side of the road at intervals of 6 m. The pole diameter was 7cm, and the color was vivid yellow. EXPERIMENT 1 Purpose of the Experiment We conducted Experiment 1 for confirmation of the previous study [4], and for revealing effects of the order of observing stimuli. Experimental Apparatus and Environment An image sequence of driving simulator generated by a personal computer was projected on a screen. Figure 3 shows the experimental environment. The height of participant s eyes was adjusted to 120 cm which matches the center of the projected image. The participants were directed to gaze the center of the image. The image aspect ratio was 9:16. The projected size was 63! 112 cm 2. Fighre3. Experimental environment Setting the Video Simulator The simulation image was rendered by using open-gl (see Figure 4). The width of one road lane was 3.25m. The regulation speed was 60 km/h [5], however, the car Figure4. Snapshot of the driving simulator Patterns of Pole Installation The patterns of pole installation were the following 6 types, of which 4 types were the same as the previous study [4], and two types were added in Experiment 1. 1. Nothing. (no-p.) (Figure 6) 2. Poles with the same height, 2 m. (same-p.) (Figure 7) 3. Poles with gradually increasing height from near to far. The shortest was 20 cm height; the longest was 2 m height. (inc.-p) (Figure 8) 4. Poles with gradually increasing height which has 2 cycles in the test section, which seems a staircase. The shortest was 20 cm height; the longest was 2 m height. (staircase-p) (Figure 9) 5. Poles with gradually decreasing height from near to far. The longest was 2 m height; the shortest was 20 cm height. (dec.-p) (Figure 10) 6. Poles tilted 60 degrees to frontward direction. The height was 2 m constant. (tilt-p) (Figure 11)

Figure5. Road alignment in the simulator Participants 18 undergraduate and graduate students took part in this experiment as observers (aged 20-24 years). All participants used to drive a car, and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Procedures Poles installation conditions were 6 patterns as described before (Figure 6~11). Downward gradient conditions of the test section were 6 types, 0% (horizontal condition), 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 %(gradient condition). We created 36 (6 x 6) video images totally for the driving simulator. One stimulus was shown for 13 seconds. 9 participants observed in the sequence of no-p, same-p, inc.-p, dec.-p, staircase-p, and tilt-p (Order 1). Remaining 9 participants observed in the sequence of tilt-p, staircase- P, dec.-p, inc.-p, same-p, no-p (Order 2). In each poles condition, participants observed a downhill road randomly selected from 6 types of gradient, and judged whether the road in the test section was downward or not. They answered the question verbally just after going through the second white line marked at the end of the test section (175 m distance). Breaks of the experiment were taken on demand of participants. Figure7. same-p pattern of pole installation Figure8. inc.-p pattern of pole installation Figure6. no-p pattern of pole installation Figure9. staircase-p pattern of pole installation

Figure10. dec.-p pattern of pole installation Figure12. Probability of perceiving downward gradient (Order 1 group) Figure11. tilt-p pattern of pole installation Experimental Results and Discussion Figures 12 and 13 show the results of the observing Orders 1 and 2, respectively. The vertical axis is probability of perceiving downward gradient, and the horizontal axis is actual gradient of the test section. The participant could judge downward gradient with high precision when the downward of the test section was high gradient. This result is the same as in the previous study, but different results are recognized by comparing between Figure 12 and Figure 13. The observing order effects to the perception of downward gradient. Figure 14 shows the integrated result of Orders 1 and 2. In the case of no-p, the 50% chance level of the probability was about 4.5% gradient. In this simulation we also confirmed the phenomena of the Obake-zaka. In the case of tilt-p, the 50% chance level of the probability was about 2% gradient, which was the lowest gradient in all pole patterns. In the other cases, the differences were not clear. Because we found an effect of observing order, we have to take counter balance in the order of conditions among the participants. Figure13. Probability of perceiving downward gradient (Order 2 group) Figure14. Integrated result collected from the data in Order 1 group and in Order 2group.

EXPERIMENT 2 Purpose of the Experiment In this experiment, the observing order of pole patterns was randomized in order to have counter balance among the participants. Then, we added variations of tilt pole pattern, because the tilt-p pattern brought the best sensitivity for downward gradient in Experiment 1. The purpose of this experiment was to find good pole pattern to perceive the true road gradient. Patterns of Pole Installation and Experimental Purposes The experimental environment and basic configuration of the simulator were the same as Experiment 1. Since the results of Experiment 1 show that the effect of poles in same-p and staircase-p patterns were not remarkable, we excluded those patterns. And we added new two patterns that were poles tilted 75 and 45 degrees to frontward direction. Thus, the patterns for the installation in Experiment 2 were the following six patterns. 1. Nothing. (no-p.) 2. Poles with gradually increasing height from near to far. The shortest was 20 cm height; the longest was 2 m height. (inc.-p) 3. Poles with gradually decreasing height from near to far. The longest was 2 m height; the shortest was 20 cm height. (dec.-p) 4. Poles tilted 75 degrees to frontward direction. The height was 2 m constant. (tilt75-p) (Figure 15) 5. Poles tilted 60 degrees to frontward direction. The height was 2 m constant. (tilt60-p) (Figure 16) 6. Poles tilted 45 degrees to frontward direction. The height was 2 m constant. (tilt45-p) (Figure 17) Figure16. tilt60-p pattern of pole installation Figure17. tile45-p pattern of pole installation Experiment Participants 20 undergraduate and graduate students took part in this experiment as observers (aged 19-25 years). All participants used to drive a car, and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Experimental Results and Discussion The results are shown in Figures 18, 19. As the gradient in the test section increased, the probability of perceiving the gradient in each pattern increased. We calculated each 50% chance level as a threshold to judge a downward gradient by using profit analysis. Table 1 shows the threshold in each pattern. Installing poles improved the sensitivity to perceive vertical gradient of the road. In the pole patterns, tilt poles brought good sensitivity, and the 60 deg tilt brought the best sensitivity. We confirmed the effectiveness of tilted poles for increasing the sensitivity. Figure15. tilt75-p pattern of pole installation

Fighure 20. 1.0m tilt pole pattern of pole installation Figure18. Probability of perceiving downward gradient in Experiment 2. Pole pattern Threshold (gradient) no-p 4.4% inc.-p 3.6% dec.-p 3.9% tilt75-p 2.1% tilt60-p 1.5% tilt45-p 2.8% Table1. Threshold to perccive the gradient in each pole pattern. EXPERIMENT 3 Purpose of the Experiment In this experiment, we tried to find the best length of tilt pole for increasing the sensitivity. Patterns of Pole Installation and Experimental Purposes The experimental environment and basic configuration of the simulator were the same as Experiment 1. Because the results of Experiment 2 show that the effect of poles in tilt60-p pattern was the best for increasing of the sensitivity, we added new three patterns as follows; 1. Poles (1.0 m height) tilted 60 degrees to frontward direction. (Figure 20) 2. Poles (1.4 m height) tilted 60 degrees to frontward direction. (Figure 21) 3. Poles (1.7 m height) tilted 60 degrees to frontward direction. (Figure 22) 4. Poles (2.0 m height) tilted 60 degrees to frontward direction. (Figure 23) Figure 21. 1.4m tilt pole pattern of pole installation Figure 22. 1.7m tilt pole pattern of pole installation Figure 23. 2.0m tilt pole pattern of pole installation Experiment Participants 11 undergraduate and graduate students took part in this experiment as observers (aged 21-23 years). All participants used to drive a car, and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision.

Experimental Results and Discussion The results are shown in Figures 24, 25. We calculated each 50% chance level as a threshold to judge a downward slope by using profit analysis. Table 2 shows the threshold in each pattern. As a result, the thresholds of 2.0 m, 1.7 m, and 1.4 m tilt pole were similar. 1.0m pole brought the sensitivity decreasing. This result indicates that tilt pole needs enough height for increasing the sensitivity. Pole pattern Threshold (gradient) 1.0m tilt pole 3.2% 1.4m tilt pole 2.3% 1.7m tilt pole 1.8% 2.0m tilt pole 1.9% Table2. Threshold to perccive the gradient in each pole pattern. CONCLUSION In this study, we found the following results through three experiments; 1. Installing poles on the both sides of a downhill road, Obake-zaka, brought increasing sensitivity of vertical gradient of the road. 2. Poles tilting to frontward direction were more effective for increasing the sensitivity than the other installations. 3. The best angle of the tilt poles was 60 degree, and the best height of the tilt pole was 1.7 m. Figure24. Probability of perceiving downward gradient in Experiment 3. REFERENCES 1. The Traffic Bureau, the National Police Agency. (2009). heisei21nentyuu no koutuusiboujikono tokutyou oyobi dourokoutuuhoutorisimariihan nituite (About the feature of the traffic fatality accidents and violation of the road traffic law in 2009). Tokyo, Japan (in Japanese) 2. The Traffic Bureau, the National Police Agency. (2009). heisei21nentyuu no koutuujiko no hasseijyoukyou (The generation status of the traffic accidents in 2009). Tokyo, Japan (in Japanese) 3. Ibukiyama T., Ibukiyama, S. (1966). douro no ningenkougaku (The ergonomics in roads), Gijyutsusyoin, Tokyo, Japan (in Japanese) 4. Gotou, S., Osa, A., Kinoshita, T., Ichikawa, M., Miike, H. (2002). Study on the arrangement of accident prevention device and slant perception, Proceedings of Symposium on ITS, pp. 235-240. (in Japanese) 5. Okura I. (1993). The traffic engineering. Corona Publishing, Yokohama, Japan (in Japanese)