Visitor s Drawings of Small. Front-End Evaluation. Joyce Ma

Similar documents
Spiral Zoom on a Human Hand

Visitors Interpretations of Images of the Nanoscale. Front-End Evaluation. Joyce Ma

Three Drops. Formative Evaluation. Joyce Ma

Key Concept: Nanoparticles are extremely tiny particles that have one dimension that is less than 100 nm in size.

Rubber Hand. Joyce Ma. July 2006

Zebrafish with Magnification Comparison of two Physical Layouts. Joyce Ma and Jackie Wong. October 2003

XYZ Control with Interactive Media for Sea Urchin Embryos/Larvae. Joyce Ma and Jackie Wong. June 2003

School Field Trip Framework

Title: Thinking with the Eyes Author(s): Elizabeth Haggerty Hutton Date Created: 8/5/2011 Subject: Biology Grade Level: 9 th Grade Honors Standards:

Learning Progression for Narrative Writing

How Small Can Robots Be?

Sample pages. Skip Counting. Until we know the pattern of numbers, we can count on from the last answer. Skip count and write the numbers as you go.

STEM Gaming in Museums Making the Right Moves

Saxon Publishers PLACEMENT INVENTORY. Math K 3

VLSI Physical Design Prof. Indranil Sengupta Department of Computer Science and Engineering Indian Institute of Technology, Kharagpur

Distorted Mirrors: Hopscotch

Lev Manovich Excerpts from The Anti-Sublime Ideal in Data Art Visualization and Mapping

Problem Solving with Length, Money, and Data

Tables and Figures. Germination rates were significantly higher after 24 h in running water than in controls (Fig. 4).

Dollar Board $1.00. Copyright 2011 by KP Mathematics

Negotiating representations of scientific phenomena during the. development of games for learning. Cornelia Brunner

You must have: Ruler graduated in centimetres and millimetres, pen, HB pencil, eraser. Tracing paper may be used.

Thinking Kids. First Grade. NCTM Strands Covered: Number and Operations. Algebra. Geometry. Measurement. Data Analysis and Probability.

11.5 The Senses Tuesday January 7, Wednesday, 8 January, 14

Ex 1: Introduction to the microscope

ABC Quilts. ABC Quilts. 1 of 22. Copyright, Exemplars. All rights reserved.

Education and Outreach: Nanotechnology Activity Guides

IBPS PO (PH-II) MOCK TEST (SOLUTION)

NNIN Nanotechnology Education

AWM 11 UNIT 1 WORKING WITH GRAPHS

1995 Video Lottery Survey - Results by Player Type

NNIN Nanotechnology Education

ABC Quilts. ABC Quilts. 1 of 24. Copyright, Exemplars. All rights reserved.

IN PROFILE. Brenda Hoddinott. clumping them all into only three categories is very challenging.

Mirror Anamorphosis Large

The Next Generation Science Standards Grades 6-8

Abstract shape: a shape that is derived from a visual source, but is so transformed that it bears little visual resemblance to that source.

3rd Grade Science. Grade 3 : Inquiry

FICTION: Understanding the Text

FRONTAL VIEW OF. Brenda Hoddinott

Elements of Art Name Design Project

Visitors interest, questions, and observations about GFP Zebrafish under the Axiovert200M. Joyce Ma and Jackie Wong. February 2004

Smaller than the eye can see: How your computer is made June 19, 2006

Chemical Wonders, L1, Activity 1:The Nano-Scale

LESSON INTRODUCTION. Reading Comprehension Modules Page 1. Joanne Durham, Interviewer (I); Apryl Whitman, Teacher (T)

K.1 Structure and Function: The natural world includes living and non-living things.

Rational Number Project

Generic noise criterion curves for sensitive equipment

STEM School Chattanooga

Drawing Portfolio. Advanced Placement Studio Art. Drawing embodies a genuine and independent way of thinking. Phillip Rawson

A u s t r a l i a n Ma t h e m a t i c s Co m p e t i t i o n

Reach Out and Touch Someone

Many-particle Systems, 3

7 - Science Science Inquiry

Numbers. Counting. Key Point. Key Point. Understand what a number is Count from 0 20 in numbers and words Count to 100

Conceptual Metaphors for Explaining Search Engines

Refraction of Light. Refraction of Light

Numeracy Warm Up. Introduction

TURNING IDEAS INTO REALITY: ENGINEERING A BETTER WORLD. Marble Ramp

Oaktree School Assessment MATHS: NUMBER P4

How to Quit NAIL-BITING Once and for All

PBL Challenge: DNA Microarray Fabrication Boston University Photonics Center

Iowa Research Online. University of Iowa. Robert E. Llaneras Virginia Tech Transportation Institute, Blacksburg. Jul 11th, 12:00 AM

PBL Challenge: Of Mice and Penn McKay Orthopaedic Research Laboratory University of Pennsylvania

Beyond Words. Lam Loc. Technical Drafting. Imagine using only words to describe your design for a bridge to

An Introduction to ScratchJr

Infographics at CDC for a nonscientific audience

EYE MOVEMENT STRATEGIES IN NAVIGATIONAL TASKS Austin Ducworth, Melissa Falzetta, Lindsay Hyma, Katie Kimble & James Michalak Group 1

The Grade 1 Common Core State Standards for Geometry specify that children should

DREAM INTERVIEWING A contemporary method of dream interpretation

Focus Group Participants Understanding of Advance Warning Arrow Displays used in Short-Term and Moving Work Zones

Little Youngsters Stencil Package Game Ideas

History of microscopy

Table of Contents SCIENTIFIC INQUIRY AND PROCESS UNDERSTANDING HOW TO MANAGE LEARNING ACTIVITIES TO ENSURE THE SAFETY OF ALL STUDENTS...

Object Perception. 23 August PSY Object & Scene 1

Chapter 1: About Science

Date. Probability. Chapter

NOT QUITE NUMBER THEORY

The Australian Curriculum Science

21 st Century Skills. Describe how satellite data is transmitted from space to Earth,

The Hole Story. Fun with the Exploratorium business card Written by Stephanie Chasteen, and credited to the person who sent me the activity

Arches are the simplest type of fingerprints that are formed by ridges that enter on one of the print and exit on the. No are present.

Techniques. Introduction to Drawing Final Exam Study Guide

DIFFERENCE BETWEEN A PHYSICAL MODEL AND A VIRTUAL ENVIRONMENT AS REGARDS PERCEPTION OF SCALE

First Practice Test 2 Levels 3-5 Calculator allowed

Shapes. Practice. Family Note. Unit. show 3-sided, 4-sided, 5-sided, and 6-sided shapes. Ask an adult for permission first. Add.

Use the table above to fill in this simpler table. Buttons. Sample pages. Large. Small. For the next month record the weather like this.

INTRODUCTION. 5. Electromagnetic Waves

Elements of Visual Representation Prof. Shatarupa Thakurta Roy Department of Humanities and Social Sciences Indian Institute of Technology, Kanpur

AC phase. Resources and methods for learning about these subjects (list a few here, in preparation for your research):

TGR EDU: EXPLORE HIGH SCHOOL DIGITAL TRANSMISSION

High Touch High Tech Science Experiences That Come To You

Geometry. Module 2. Representations. Grades 4 to 7. Teacher Document

What Type Of Light Microscope Is Used To View Large Objects Such As Flowers

Topic 1 - What is Light? 1. Radiation is the type of energy transfer which does not require... A matter B heat C waves D light

Go to the following link: to answer the questions and help you solve the case.

Making Middle School Math Come Alive with Games and Activities

Picks. Pick your inspiration. Addison Leong Joanne Jang Katherine Liu SunMi Lee Development Team manager Design User testing

THE NORTH LONDON INDEPENDENT GIRLS SCHOOLS CONSORTIUM MATHEMATICS

Sense Organs (Eye) The eye is the sense organ of sight. The eye is shaped like a ball and is located in bony

Transcription:

Visitor s Drawings of Small Front-End Evaluation Joyce Ma October 2007

Acknowledgements The author would like to thank Karen Chang for her thoughtful comments and for checking the coding scheme devised in this study. This report was based on work supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant No. ESI-0532536. Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this report are those of the author and do not necessarily reflect the views of the Foundation. Joyce Ma Exploratorium 3601 Lyon Street San Francisco joycem at exploratorium dot edu 415 674-2874 NISE Network Research and Evaluation - 2 - www.nisenet.org

Table of Contents Purpose... 4 Summary Of Key Findings... 4 Method... 5 Data... 5 Results... 6 Visitors Representation Types... 6 Object-to-Object Comparisons... 12 Actual Size... 14 Analogy... 14 Notation... 16 Zoom... 16 Technology... 18 A Look Across the Different Representations... 18 Concluding Thoughts and Next Steps...19 References... 20 NISE Network Research and Evaluation - 3 - www.nisenet.org

Purpose This report documents a study conducted at the Exploratorium to characterize how visitors depict the concept of small. Working on the premise that people, both experts and novices, start with a rich pool of representational competence (disessa, 2004), this study looks at visitors drawings in order to inform the design of diagrams, animations and other visualizations that help the public visualize and learn about the nanoscale in informal learning environments. This work was conducted as part of the Visualization Laboratory, under the larger NISE Net effort. Summary Of Key Findings Table 1 summarizes the key findings from this study and their possible implications for the design of visualizations. Table 1. Key findings and their implications Finding About a quarter of the drawings included a small pencil mark, or dot, that was used to convey small, either in a comparison or simply to note that the small is invisible About 15% of the visitors tried to depict the actual size of the smallest object they could imagine. Visitors used macroscale objects in their analogies to convey the magnitude of the size differences between familiar objects and the very small. About 10% of the visitors used numbers somewhere in their drawings. Less than 10% of the drawings included the human body. The human was mainly used in zooms, as the largest object in a sequence of smaller and smaller objects. Implication Objects that are on the border between the visible and invisible could be useful in introducing the world of the very small. Try using objects that are barely visible as size references in visualizations for the nanoscale. Consider including actual size objects in visualizations to establish size and scale. Experiment with analogies that use familiar macroscale objects to help visitors visualize the magnitude of the size difference between the nanoscale and other size scales. Support visitors in interpreting numbers in diagrams. The human is too big of an object to serve as the main size reference to convey how very small something is. Consider using smaller, but familiar, objects as references. Additional findings and more detailed explications can be found in the Results Section of this document, organized according to the different types of representations visitors drew to convey small. NISE Network Research and Evaluation - 4 - www.nisenet.org

Method The data for this study were collected as part of a pre-test administered in a series of evaluation studies on the scale ladder diagram. The pre-test tasks and questions were the same in all these studies as was our recruitment method. That is, we systematically recruited visitors inside the Exploratorium, asking every 3 rd visitor who crossed an imaginary line for an interview. We did not approach visitors under the age of 10, as identified by sight. If it were not clear if the visitor who crossed our line was 10 or older, the evaluator would ask the child s age before the interview. We conducted interviews with individual visitors, although other members of their visiting group were welcomed to listen in if they chose. At the start of the interview, we asked visitors to name the smallest thing they could think of and to describe both verbally and with a drawing how they would communicate how small that object is. Earlier pilot testing indicated that some visitors would focus on drawing the object itself instead of trying to depict its size. When the evaluator felt that this was happening, she asked the visitor how his/her drawing would show someone, who did not know anything about that object, how small the object is. This was enough to redirect the visitor. In addition, the evaluator noted any explanations visitors gave for their drawing. Data We recruited and interviewed visitors on the following days: 6/24/2007 Sunday 7/1/2007 Sunday 7/8/2007 Sunday 7/10/2007 Tuesday 7/12/2007 Thursday 7/13/2007 Friday 7/15/2007 Sunday 7/17/2007 Tuesday 7/20/2007 Friday 7/22/2007 Sunday In total, 121 visitors completed the drawing task (N = 121). Their demographic information are summarized in Table 2 and Table 3. NISE Network Research and Evaluation - 5 - www.nisenet.org

Table 2. Gender of visitors Gender Count Female 65 (54%) Male 56 (46%) All 121 (100%) Table 3. Age group of visitors Age Group Count Child (10-12) 10 (8%) Teen (13-17) 27 (22%) Adult (18-65) 77 (64%) Senior (65+) 7 (6%) All 121 (100%) Results Visitors Representation Types Small is inherently a relational term that indicates a size comparison between one object and another. There are, however, different definitions of small depending on the frame of reference and the nature of the comparison. 1 To characterize visitors descriptions of small in our drawing task, we iteratively developed a coding scheme by looking through visitors drawings and descriptions, paying particular attention to the types of comparisons depicted. The following defines the categories we used and gives examples to better illustrate each category. 1 For example, in their work with very young children, Ebeling and Gelman (1994) defined three meanings: 1) normative, where the object is compared to the typical size of its kind; 2) perceptual, where the object is compared to another object that is physically present; and 3) functional, where the object is assessed according to its intended use. NISE Network Research and Evaluation - 6 - www.nisenet.org

Object-to-object comparisons. These drawings compare two or more object types to express relative size. Oftentimes, words rather than any graphical elements are used to indicate that one object is smaller than the other. Sometimes one of the objects, typically the smaller object, is described through annotations but not drawn. Figure 1 gives an example of a drawing that we placed in this category. Figure 1. Visitor drawing (V42) depicting how small a white blood cell is by comparing it to a fly. Actual Size A few visitors tried to draw the actual size of the smallest object they could think of. When the visitors identified something in the macroscale as their smallest object, they drew that object. Figure 2 shows an example. Figure 2. Visitor drawing (V51) depicting how small a flea is. The visitor described small by drawing the actual size of the smallest object they could think of. However, when they identified something invisible as the smallest thing they could think of, these visitors did not draw anything, instead explaining that it would be impossible to draw anything to communicate how small that object is. For example, Visitor24: Can t see, can t draw it. I can t even see it. So I can t draw it. NISE Network Research and Evaluation - 7 - www.nisenet.org

Analogies. These drawings convey size relationships by comparing the size difference between a pair of objects, the target, and another pair of more familiar objects, the base. They are in the form: a is smaller than b as x is smaller than y. One of the objects in the analogy may not be drawn but simply spoken. An analogy is shown in Figure 3. Figure 3. Visitor drawing (V46) depicting how small a gluon is through the analogy, earth:sun :: gluon:atom. Notation This category includes drawings in which visitors use a lone mark to represent the object. The mark is very small, often just a dot, and stands for something in the invisible world. See Figure 4. Figure 4. Visitor drawing (V250) depicting how small an atom is. A small mark is used to describe how small the atom is. The visitor mentions, It s so small you can t see it. NISE Network Research and Evaluation - 8 - www.nisenet.org

Zooms. A zoom is similar to an object-to-object comparison; however, the drawing uses graphical elements to show magnification; that is, the smaller object is part of another, larger object. So, in addition to depicting size relationships, a zoom describes containment relationships between the different objects included. See the example in Figure 5. Figure 5. Visitor drawing (V120) depicting how small a quark is by zooming in on a human being. NISE Network Research and Evaluation - 9 - www.nisenet.org

Technology These drawings depict the type of device that would be necessary to see the small. So, something is small because we require special equipment to see it compared to simply using our unaided eyes. See Figure 6 for an example. Figure 6. Visitor drawing (V3) depicting how small a germ is. The visitor drew a microscope that would be required to see the smallest thing she could think of. NISE Network Research and Evaluation - 10 - www.nisenet.org

Others There were only 2 drawings that did not fit any of the categories above. Behavior. One visitor described small by depicting its unique properties and behaviors, which are different compared to other objects behaviors. See Figure 7. Figure 7. Visitor drawing (V56) depicting how small a quark is. This visitor described small according to what the quark does (vibrate) and the physical residues (trails) it leaves. Measure. One visitor gave the measured size. See Figure 8. Figure 8. Visitor drawing (V53) depicting how small a bacterium is by giving a numeric value of size. Table 4 gives the count of the different types of visitors representations of small. The following sections describe in more detail each of the representation types visitors used to express small and discuss possible implications for the design of visualizations to help visitors see the nanoscale. NISE Network Research and Evaluation - 11 - www.nisenet.org

Table 4. Tally of the types of representations visitors drew Representation Type Count (out of 121) Object-To-Object Comparison 48 (40%) Actual Size 21 (17%) Analogy 20 (17%) Notation 15 (12%) Zoom 10 (8%) Technology 5 (4%) Other 2 (2%) Object-to-Object Comparisons Approximately 40% of the visitors we interviewed chose to depict how small their smallest object is by comparing it to another, larger object or set of objects. We list the objects visitors used in their descriptions in Table 5 and Table 6 to catalog objects we can use in our own visualizations that similarly juxtapose different types of objects to communicate size and scale (e.g., scale ladder diagrams and perspective illustrations). These lists complement prior work (Tretter, Jones, Andre, Negishi, & Minogue, 2006 and Tretter, Jones, & Minogue, 2006), which identified anchors and landmarks that ground visitors understanding of size and scale. NISE Network Research and Evaluation - 12 - www.nisenet.org

Table 5. The larger objects visitors used in visitors object-to-object comparisons. We grouped the objects according to their actual size scale. Macroscale m cm mm Microscale Nanoscale Atomic Scale Subatomic Scale Person (1) Penny (2) Dot (14) Cell (5) Molecule (2) Atom (2) Electron (5) Bean (1) Needle Tip (2) Box (1) Pin (2) Hair (1) Circle (1) Ant (1) Hemotoid (1) Fly (1) Eyelash (1) Hand (1) Flea (1) Inch (1) Sand (1) Line (1) Tick (1) Finger (1) Teaspoon (1) Chip (1) Nano (1) Nucleus (1) Table 6. The smallest object visitors could think of (Object-to-object comparison). Objects are grouped according to their actual size scale. Macroscale m cm mm Microscale Nanoscale Atomic Scale Subatomic Scale Grain of Sand (1) Cell (6) Molecule (2) Atom (16) Electron (4) Amoeba (2) Animal Cell (1) Bacteria (1) Fluorescence Molecule (1) Line Width Of A Silicon Circuit (1) Particles Inside Sunscreen (1) Quark (4) Photon (2) Subatomic Particle (2) Microbe (1) Virus (1) Positron (1) We found that only one person (1/48 or 2%) compared his smallest object to a human. So, despite prior research that identified the human body as a common reference size, very few visitors used the human body in their object-to-object comparisons. This may be because trying to convey how very small something is, is more convincing when the comparison is made to something else that is also very small. It is also possible that because most visitors identified objects many orders of magnitude smaller than a human body, the human becomes a less relevant size reference in these NISE Network Research and Evaluation - 13 - www.nisenet.org

smaller worlds and their depictions. In fact, we found that the most common size reference used was not even a real object. Instead, it was simply a very small pencil mark or dot, which was used in the comparison to show that their smallest object is much smaller than what is visible. This implies that one way of helping visitors get a sense of the size of the very small is to include an actual size depiction of something that is small and nearly invisible. A few (8) visitors (17%) who used an object-to-object comparison indicated approximately how much smaller their smallest object is than the larger object, making guesses at the magnitude of the size difference. Otherwise, the magnitude of the size difference was not a component of these descriptions, and small in these representations is an expression of relative size only. Actual Size We found that a little over 15% of the visitors tried to depict the actual size of the smallest object they could imagine. Although it is not possible to depict the actual size of a nanosize object, these drawings suggest that actual size may be important for visitors in interpreting the representations we create. They may, in fact, give visitors a reference point for understanding the scale of the illustration or depiction. By extension, we may want to explore including actual size objects to establish size and scale in visualizations we design. Analogy Approximately 15% of the visitors we interviewed used an analogy to describe small. Unlike most of the object-to-object comparisons and zooms, these analogies tried to describe the magnitude of how small something is. These analogies do so by comparing the smallest object visitors could think of to a larger object and then comparing that pairing, which we call the target pair, to a more familiar object pair, called the base pair. Table 7 lists the analogies visitors used to visualize the size of the smallest thing they could name. NISE Network Research and Evaluation - 14 - www.nisenet.org

Table 7. Analogies visitors used to describe the smallest thing they can think of. The base is a set of familiar objects and relationships, and the target is a set of less familiar objects and relationships. Comparing the target to the base elicits the shared relationships between the two, in these cases, the scale of the size differences. Those analogies that are within 2 orders of magnitude in accuracy are in green; those within 4 are in orange, and those that are 5 or more orders of magnitude from accuracy are in purple. The example shaded gray could not be determined. Base A is smaller than B :: Earth : Sun :: Gluon : Atom Human Life Scale : Night Sky :: Quark : Sand Moon : Sun :: Electron : Atom Penny : Earth :: Electron : Atom Penny : Earth :: Electron : Atom Dot : Earth :: Electron : Dot Dot : Galaxy :: Neuron : Dot Dot : Room :: Proton : Dot Pencil : Football Field :: Proton : Dot Pin : Soccer Field :: Proton : Golf Ball Target X is smaller than Y ( X is the smallest object visitors named ) Me : Bldg :: Things In Atoms (Contains Quarks) : Atom Tennis Ball : Football Field :: Nucleus : Outside Of Atom Object : Football Field :: Atom : Bee Dot : Earth :: Atom : Dot Dot : Earth :: Atom : Dot Dot : Empire State Building :: Atom : Dot Dot : Skyscraper :: Atom : Dot Sand : Room :: Atom : Sand Basketball Size : Earth :: Atom : Tire Man : Empire State Bldg :: Nanoparticle : Needle Building : Earth :: Microorganism : Person Table 7 shows that in all cases, visitors chose macroscale objects for the base pair, with a majority, 14 out of 20 (70%) visitors, using a base object that was larger than 100m in size. In fact, when we looked through all the representations visitors drew, we found that visitors used larger objects (i.e. > 10m) only in analogies, in order to convey the magnitude of the size differences without using numbers. NISE Network Research and Evaluation - 15 - www.nisenet.org

Only 7 out of 20 (35%) visitors came within 2 orders of magnitude in accuracy in their size comparisons. However, since people tend to be more facile at judging the size of large as opposed to small objects (Tretter, Jones, & Minogue, 2006), analogies that use familiar macroscale objects as the base may be a powerful way to help visitors visualize the magnitude of the size difference between objects in the nanoscale and more familiar objects. Notation When we looked through visitors representations, we were surprised to find a few diagrams that consisted simply of a dot. Although some of these dots were intended to represent the actual size of something small in the macroscopic scale, others (15/121 or 12%) were clearly just marks to indicate small. Often, visitors used these dots as a placeholder for something invisible. This suggests that using objects in visualizations that are on the edge of being visible could be a helpful means of introducing the invisible world of the micro and nanoscale. Zoom Zooms, like object-to-object comparisons, describe relative size, but they also express containment relationships, in which the smaller object is part of a larger object that, in turn, is part of yet a larger object. Zooms are a common device used to help people visualize the very small, and a few (10/121) visitors drew zooms to try to convey the size of the smallest thing they could think of. Although the count is low (only 8% of all the visitors depictions we collected), these drawings may, nonetheless, help us design zooms to communicate the nanoscale. Table 8 lists the objects that were included in each of the ten zooms visitors drew. Like Table 5 and Table 6, this list gives us an idea of small objects that visitors may be familiar with, which can inform what we may want to include in our visualizations. NISE Network Research and Evaluation - 16 - www.nisenet.org

Table 8. Objects included in visitors zooms. Blue indicates the starting, or largest, object of the zoom, and red indicates the last, or smallest, object of the zoom. person person person person person Macro foot hand hand hand hand water things in foot skin finger finger fingertip fingertip fingertip Micro things in thing vessel in tip cells skin cells cell cell blood cell cells bacteria cells cell nucleus Zoom Nano protein molecule molecule molecule molecule molecule molecule Subatomic atom atom atom atom atom atom atom atom electron electron nucleus Atomic subatomic particle neutron proton quark quark quark We specifically looked to see what objects visitors chose to zoom in from. The largest, or the starting, object may be the reference point for understanding the size of the smaller, subsequent objects in the sequence. We found that 5 out of the 10 visitors, who drew a zoom, started with the human being. This suggests, that although the human is not a common size reference in other representation types, the human body may be more relevant in zooms. Yet, not all zooms need to begin with a depiction of a human being. Six out of the 10 zooms included an object that is (likely) in the nanoscale 2, and in half of these drawings, a nanoscale object was included as an object between a known larger object and a known smaller object. Although most visitors example of a nanoscale object was a molecule, the drawings suggest that zooms can be useful in introducing visitors to 2 In this analysis, we put molecules in the nanoscale, even though some molecules can be microscopic in size. NISE Network Research and Evaluation - 17 - www.nisenet.org

the nanoscale as a size that exists between the familiar, larger scales and the smaller scales. Like the object-to-object comparisons, the zooms that visitors drew predominantly showed relative size. But, unlike the other representations, zooms communicate composition and may, therefore, be useful for conveying other messages beyond size and scale. Technology A very small percentage of visitors (4%) drew the technology that would be necessary to see the very small. In all of these drawings, visitors depicted a microscope or a special lens, or portal, for viewing the very small. No one distinguished between the different technologies that would be needed to see the different size scales. These drawings, though few (5 out of 121 or 4%), point to an additional way of orienting visitors to the nanoscale: Define size according to the type of equipment that would be needed to see these worlds. Also, use graphical elements that indicate a shift in how we see the different scales of small. Unfortunately, we suspect that few lay people know about the different technologies that are needed to see the microscale versus the nanoscale versus the atomic and subatomic scales. So, cuing visitors to the size scale with the different technologies used to see these worlds will not be straightforward. A Look Across the Different Representations In addition to looking at each type of representation, we looked across all the different descriptions visitors gave to convey small to see if there were more general patterns. Doing so, we found that small was often associated with something invisible. That is, 33 out of 121 visitors (27%) used a dot or a very small mark, something that can be drawn but yet close to being invisible, to convey how small their smallest object is. 3 This suggests that the boundary between the visible and invisible world may partition what is small from what is not and be an important part of some people s understanding of size. This points to a potentially powerful way of helping visitors understand small: depict the actual size of macroscale objects that are barely visible with the naked eye; this object can then become the anchor, or size reference, for much smaller objects in the invisible world. 3 These 33 include drawings from the object-to-object comparison and notation categories. NISE Network Research and Evaluation - 18 - www.nisenet.org

We also found that a small minority, 13 out of 121(11%) visitors, used numbers somewhere in their drawings and descriptions. This can reflect an unfamiliarity with measures at these small scales, which echoes earlier findings (Tretter, Jones, & Minogue, 2006). Although this does not preclude us from including numbers, it does mean that visitors may need support in interpreting numbers in visualizations of the very small. Looking at all the drawings, we were surprised to find that very few visitors drew the human body although previous studies have found that to be a common size reference. The exception was the zoom, the only representation type that had multiple objects and steps. Yet, even there, only half the people started with the human body. So, perhaps in depicting the very small, the human is too big of an object to serve as the main size reference. Concluding Thoughts and Next Steps This study is an initial glimpse into the types of representations that visitors can invent to help them communicate and visualize small. The invented representations that were analyzed in this study were drawn in a very short time 4 as part of a longer interview on the Exploratorium floor. This study, therefore, does not provide a detailed look at the reasoning people use as they create and refine representations, and we hesitate to draw any conclusions about how visitors iteratively craft or carefully interpret visualizations about the very small. Instead, this study gives us clues about visitors first, quick attempts at conveying small. The findings are meant to suggest some potentially promising ways to create meaningful representations to visualize the very small based on visitors initial takes. Further development and research can look specifically at the visualizations we design, as guided by these suggestions, to assess if and how they help the public see the nanoscale. 4 often under a minute NISE Network Research and Evaluation - 19 - www.nisenet.org

References disessa, A. A. (2004). Metarepresentation: Native Competence and Targets for Instruction. Cognition and Instruction, 22(3), 293-331. Ebeling, K. S., & Gelman, S. A. (1994). Children's Use of Context in Interpreting "Big" and "Little". Child Development, 65(4), 1178-1192. Tretter, T. R., Jones, M. G., Andre, T., Negishi, A., & Minogue, J. (2006). Conceptual Boundaries and Distances: Students' and Experts' Concepts of the Scale of Scientific Phenomena. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 43(3), 282-319. Tretter, T. R., Jones, M. G., & Minogue, J. (2006). Accuracy of Scale Conceptions in Science: Mental Maneuverings across Many Orders of Spatial Magnitude. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 43(10), 1061-1085. NISE Network Research and Evaluation - 20 - www.nisenet.org