CONSIDER THE CHIPMUNK; RECONSIDER THE ROAD: ROAD NOISE EFFECTS ON EAVESDROPPING SYSTEMS IN EASTERN CHIPMUNKS

Similar documents
ANIMAL COMMUNICATION and INFORMATION TRANSFER

The effect of interspecific competition on the foraging behavior of the Eastern Gray Squirrel

Differential Foraging Patterns of Rodents and Birds in a Restored Prairie

Investigating the effect of differential elevation of food on foraging behavior of the Eastern grey squirrel (Sciurus carolinensis)

Abstract The American Redstart is a wood warbler that is in population decline in northern Michigan.

GENERAL PROTOCOL CONTENTS

Wintering Bird Occupancy and Detection in Response to Proximity to Water and Eastern Screech-Owl Call Playback

Oak Woodlands and Chaparral

THE EFFECT OF ANTHROPOGENIC NOISE ON VEERY SINGING BEHAVIOR

SONG SPARROWS (Melospiza melodia; Emberizidae) RELIABLY BROADCAST INFORMATION ABOUT PERCEIVED THREATS USING ALARM CALLS

Species-Specific Effects on Occupancy

Birdify Your Yard: Habitat Landscaping for Birds. Melissa Pitkin Klamath Bird Observatory

Distance from protective tree cover affects foraging times of urbanized gray squirrels at the University of Maryland Abstract

Stranger Danger: Interspecific Vocal Responses of Selected Diurnal Passerines to Indirect Predator Alarm Calls

DIET DIFFERENTIATION AND HABITAT SELECTION OF BIRDS IN FORESTED AND CLEAR-CUT AREAS

Wildlife Habitat Patterns & Processes: Examples from Northern Spotted Owls & Goshawks

Eavesdropping of woodchucks (Marmota monax) and eastern chipmunks (Tamias striatus) on heterospecific alarm calls

Beadle Plasticus Evolution Teacher Information

Foraging Behavior of Rodent and Songbird Populations, Examined with Variation of Predatory Risk

PSY 2364 Animal Communication

Effects of a New Pedestrian Pathway in Grand Teton National Park on Breeding Sagebrush Songbirds

Appendix A from S. Lingle and T. Riede, Deer Mothers Are Sensitive to Infant Distress Vocalizations of Diverse Mammalian Species

Eastern gray squirrel (Sciurus carolinensis) foraging patterns in the presence of aerial predators

Protocol for Censusing Yellow-billed Magpies (Pica nuttalli) at Communal Roosts

2010 Ornithology (B/C) - Training Handout

Introduction to Birding

of Nebraska - Lincoln

~ BIRD SURVEY'S ON Mr. MANs~.-LELD

What Limits the Reproductive Success of Migratory Birds? Warbler Data Analysis (50 pts.)

* * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * For Judges Use Only

Evidence of a four-year population cycle for the Rusty Blackbird (Euphagus carolinus)

In vivo, in silico, in machina: ants and robots balance memory and communication to collectively exploit information

Effects of Background Noise and Playback on Avian Detection Probabilities in a Piedmont Forest

1. Report No. FHWA/TX-12/ Government Accession No. 3. Recipient's Catalog No.

Antipredator calls of tufted titmice and interspecific transfer of encoded threat information

Peregrine Falcon Falco peregrinus

National Parks Challenges A True to Our Nature Educational Resource

UCLA International Journal of Comparative Psychology

1 Chickadee population trends

Are pine martens the answer to grey squirrel control?

Avian Project Guidance

Guidance note: Distribution of breeding birds in relation to upland wind farms

Alarm calls of tufted titmice convey information about predator size and threat

Effects of human activity on the foraging behavior of sanderlings Calidris alba

Birds Prefer to Feed near Predators than Feed near a Competitor. by Emerson Harris. Center for Teaching and Learning Glenn Powers 3o March 2018

Watching for Whoopers in Wisconsin Wetlands

The effects of vegetation and water depth on wading bird foraging habitat selection and foraging success in the Everglades

A REPORT ON EFFICACY OF METHIOCARB AS AN AVIAN REPELLENT IN FIGS AND RESULTS OF INDUSTRY-WIDE BIRD DAMAGE ASSESSMENTS

American Kestrel. Appendix A: Birds. Falco sparverius. New Hampshire Wildlife Action Plan Appendix A Birds-183

Bats in Hampshire. Nik Knight Chairman and Recorder Hampshire Bat Group

Bird And Habitat Scan

Emily Gillmore. Intern at the Beaverhill Bird Observatory

Varying levels of bird activity within a forest understory dominated by the invasive glossy buckthorn (Rhamnus frangula)

The Role of Avian Preen Gland Secretions in Host-Seeking Behavior of Culex pipiens Mosquitoes Research Advisor: Mary Garvin

Effects of Temperature on Mobbing Responses

PROJECT WILDBIRD Food and Feeder Preferences of Wild Birds in the United States and Canada

2011 Ornithology (B/C) - Training Handout

4/24/08. Behavioral Ecology / Evolutionary Ecology

Seasonal changes in the response of oystercatchers Haematopus ostralegus to human disturbance

Protecting the Endangered Mount Graham Red Squirrel

Birds use Audio Clues just as much as Visual Clues when Foraging

Population Patterns. Math 6.SP.B.4 6.SP.B.5 6.SP.B.5a 6.SP.B.5b 7.SP.B.3 7.SP.A.2 8.SP.A.1. Time: 45 minutes. Grade Level: 3rd to 8th

PSY 2364 Animal Communication. Courtship and mating systems. Mating systems. Mating systems. Mating systems. Mating systems 11/12/2018

Predation Risk and Feeding Site Preferences in Winter Foraging Birds

Research Article Leadership of Winter Mixed-Species Flocks by Tufted Titmice (Baeolophus bicolor): Are Titmice Passive Nuclear Species?

. Summary of nest box monitoring at Elkhorn Slough National Estuarine Research Reserve

A reprint from American Scientist

EMERGENCE OF COMMUNICATION IN TEAMS OF EMBODIED AND SITUATED AGENTS

Mixed Conifer Working Group Meeting February 17, 2011 Wildlife Habitat Management Considerations

B IRD CONSERVATION FOREST BIRD SURVEY ENTERS FINAL WINTER V OLUME 11, NUMBER 1 JANUARY Board of. Trustees. Forest bird survey 1

COVER PAGE. Home address 5875 Brasstown Creek Road, Young Harris GA 30582

FOREST BIRD SURVEYS ON MT. MANSFIELD AND UNDERBILL

Golden Eagle (Aquila chrysaetos) Management Indicator Species Assessment Ochoco National Forest

Community Crepuscular Define population Crypsis Natural History Ecosystem. each other and their Fecundity Fledgling Diet, habitat,

Birds and their Adaptations Student Activity Book Answer Key

Appendix A Little Brown Myotis Species Account

Determining the Dominant Bird Species among the Northern Cardinal, Darkeyed Junco, Tufted Titmouse, and the American Goldfinch in Lonaconing,

Ferruginous Hawk Buteo regalis

8/25/14. Introduction. Introduction. ~30 million acres across 450 installations

BIOLOGY 1101 LAB 6: MICROEVOLUTION (NATURAL SELECTION AND GENETIC DRIFT)

USING CHRISTMAS BIRD COUNT DATA TO DETERMINE POPULATION TRENDS OF FIVE BIRD SPECIES. by Thomas R. Hamilton

Abstract. Introduction

Population Dynamics Simulation

Estimating Seasonal Avian Diversity in an Urban Wetland in Columbus, Ohio. Kaitlin Carr 20 April 2018

Farr wind farm: A review of displacement disturbance on dunlin arising from operational turbines

Influence of Habitat on the Aggressive Behavior of 13-lined Ground Squirrels (Spermophilus tridecemlineatus) Hui Chien Tan

Lecture 6 Behavior ethology Communication

1.0 Performance Measure Title Wetland Trophic Relationships Wading Bird Nesting Patterns. 2.0 Justification

WindWise Education. 2 nd. T ransforming the Energy of Wind into Powerful Minds. editi. A Curriculum for Grades 6 12

Removed. Scientific Skills. Gel Chromatography Thin Layer Chromatography NMR Reading Spectrophotometer Reading Centrifuge Handling

Notes on a Breeding Population of Red-headed Woodpeckers in New York State. Jacob L. Berl and John W. Edwards

NEST BOX TRAIL HISTORY

Endangered Species Profile: The Sun Parakeet. By Student Name, Class Period

Differential Timing of Spring Migration between Sex and Age Classes of Yellow-rumped Warblers (Setophaga coronata) in Central Alberta,

Long-term monitoring of Hummingbirds in Southwest Idaho in the Boise National Forest Annual Report

Plover: a Subpopulation-Based Model of the Effects of Management on Western Snowy Plovers

The effects of nest box location on Tree Swallow ( Tachycineta bicolor ) productivity and nest. success at Beaverhill Bird Observatory, Alberta

threatens their survival.

What is a Bird of Prey?

Nuthatches eavesdrop on variations in heterospecific chickadee mobbing alarm calls

Transcription:

CONSIDER THE CHIPMUNK; RECONSIDER THE ROAD: ROAD NOISE EFFECTS ON EAVESDROPPING SYSTEMS IN EASTERN CHIPMUNKS JULIE JUNG Williams College, Williamstown, MA 01267 MENTOR SCIENTIST: DR. KENNETH SCHMIDT Texas Tech University, Lubbock, TX 79409 Abstract. The ability of a forager to gain information about its surrounding environment may influence its decision of where and how long to forage (Brown 1988, Valone and Giraldeau 1993). Typically eastern chipmunks, Tamias striatus, eavesdrop on the alarm calls of birds in the Paridae family such as eastern tufted titmice Baeolophus bicolor and black capped chickadees Poecile atricapilus to gather information about the cost of predation in the surrounding area. Since chipmunks vary their time spent foraging in relation to perceived foraging costs, the giving-up density (GUD) or the amount of food left over when the chipmunk decides to stop foraging is an indication of the animal s patch use behavior and perception of predation risk. The purpose of this research is to measure giving-up densities (GUDs) of chipmunks in the presence of different alarm calls with and without background road noise to quantify the consequential changes in perceived predation risk. If road noise masks sounds in the environment, including alarm calls, chipmunks may have elevated predation risk. Chipmunks that perceive a greater risk from road noise increase vigilance and cease risky behavior faster as a result, leaving more food behind and yielding a greater giving-up density. Our results indicate that road noise can interfere with the chipmunk s eavesdropping systems and consequently affect their cost-benefit analysis of foraging. INTRODUCTION Eavesdropping Systems When making decisions about how to navigate the world, many of us eavesdrop or take cues from the people around us. Eavesdropping is the use of information in signals by individuals other than the primary target (Schmidt et al. 2008, Peake 2005). This behavior is not species specific; small mammals shift their day-to-day activities based on the vocalizations of their avian neighbors. Con- or heterospecific alarm calls that vocally communicate a predator s presence create public information regarding predation risk in the environment (Schmidt et al. 2008, Valone and Giraldeau 1993). For instance, eastern chipmunks alter their feeding behavior based on the alarm calls of tufted titmice. Titmice are small grey songbirds that are vigilant and vociferous. When they identify a threat, they announce it by engaging in a sophisticated system of alarm calls that conveys information about both the size of a predator and its degree of threat (Templeton et al. 2005). Two different alarm calls are supposed to help organize titmouse defense: a soft, high-pitched seet call warns of flying predators while a louder mobbing call recruits others to harass or mob a perched predator (Templeton et al. 2005, Hetrick and Sieving 2011). This elaborate system of vocalizations mediates social interactions within flocks; but more than that, since chipmunks share many of the same predators as titmice, their decision of where and how long to forage is strongly influenced as a result of this information transfer. The purpose of this research is to test whether and to what degree chronic road noise alters this transfer of information through eavesdropping. Road Noise Effects Roads are ubiquitous across vast stretches of the world. In fact, 83% of the United States is within 1 km of a road (Riitters and Wickham 2003). Although some authors have suggested the positive effects of Cary Institute of Ecosystem Studies 1

roads on wildlife, the overall effects across taxa are overwhelmingly negative (McClure et al. 2013). Many studies document declines in populations of wildlife near roads; In order to identify noise as one of the primary mechanisms underlying these effects, McClure et al. (2013) experimentally apply traffic noise to a roadless area at a landscape scale. Here, we will note how the foraging patterns of chipmunks change as a result. Patch-Use Intensity and the Giving-Up Density Optimal foraging theory assumes that an individual should exploit a patch until the benefits from foraging equal the costs. Foraging costs should include the predation cost of foraging, the missed opportunity costs of not engaging in alternative activities, and the metabolic costs of foraging (Brown 1988, Stephens and Krebs 1986). When you consider the marginal rates of substitution of different inputs into fitness, all inputs can be expressed as a single currency of energy in Joules (Brown 1992). As long as harvest rates are a function of patch type and resource density, the giving up density, or the amount of food remaining in a patch at the cessation of foraging, is a highly correlated surrogate for the quitting harvest rate (QHR) and an accurate measure of the animal's costs and patch use intensity (Whelan 2005, Brown 1988). If predation cost increases in response to a stimulus, the forager spends less time foraging and the GUD increases. In order to quantify road noise effects, we measure GUDs of chipmunks in the presence of different alarm calls with and without background road noise. METHODS AND MATERIALS Study Site and Species The study area was located on the Cary Institute of Ecosystem Studies (IES), Dutchess County, NY. The site contains oak-dominated forest home to several diurnal rodents such as chipmunks, gray and red squirrels, and woodchucks and a diverse avian community including the veery (Catharus fuscescens) and two species of parids: eastern tufted titmouse (Baeolophus bicolor) and black capped chickadee (Poecile atricapilus). We chose to study the titmice, eastern chipmunks, and veeries because titmice demonstrate alarm calls that contain information about the type of predator and immediacy the threat (Hetrick and Sieving 2011, Templeton 2005, Schmidt et al. 2008); chipmunks are diurnal rodents that are small enough to potentially share common predators with titmice (Schmidt et al. 2008); and the veery s song is distinct, commonly heard in our research location, and nonthreatening to chipmunks. Experimental Food Patches and Design There are four sites total in the Greens region of IES near a setup of three speakers playing recorded road noise placed about 25m apart (Figure 1). Each site takes up a space about 50m by 50m at least 200m away from each other and contains five subsites arranged equal distances away from the speaker broadcasting alarm calls and varying distances away from the line of speakers sounding road noise (Figure 1). Each subsite should in turn have two artificial resource patches, provided by plastic trays (30 x 20 x 6cm) filled with millet seeds mixed into about 3 pints of sifted sand. One tray should have 10g of seeds mixed into the sand and the other only 5g seeds to provide a lower food density. This dual patch design can detect density-dependent or independent harvest by chipmunks and tell us if the chipmunks are foraging with no, little, or perfect information. After a baiting period of a couple weeks to allow chipmunks to associate the trays with a food source, confirmed by identifying chipmunk spoor, we collected foraging data from the seed trays for 10 days with one treatment played per site per day. Trays were open roughly between 0700 and 1430 h. The playback equipment was positioned at a height of 1.5 m in a nearby tree approximately 20m away from each Cary Institute of Ecosystem Studies 2

subsite of trays at each site. At the end of each experimental day, we noted any footprints in the sifted soil to identify the forager, took down the playback equipment, and sieved trays to recover uneaten food, which was then taken back to the lab, cleaned of debris, and weighed to measure giving-up density. Recordings There were four treatments or acoustic stimuli rotated at each of the four sites over nine days in July- August, 2014. The treatments included a veery control (represents a non-threatening song), a titmouse seet call, a titmouse mobbing call, and a grey squirrel chipping call. We recorded three exemplars of each of the four vocalizations together with tracks of various lengths of silence and uploaded the playlists onto an ipod shuffle. The pattern of vocalizations with respect to random intervals of silence was then determined by random shuffling of tracks. Vocal tracks played only 30% of the time on average. Although this frequency of vocalizing is higher than what naturally occurs, rapid rates are observed to occur over a period of 15 minutes or more (Schmidt et al. 2008). Furthermore, since individual chipmunk foraging bouts in the food patches take within 5 and 20 minutes, a relatively high call rate is necessary to insure that chipmunks are exposed to the vocalizations while they are actively foraging (Schmidt et al. 2008). Statistical Analysis We used a Latin-square design to randomize presentations over time while giving chipmunks only one treatment per day for 9 days (Table 1). Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) used rank-transformed means to test for differences in GUDs among several sources of variance. Our model includes day (to check for chipmunk habituation to calls or road noise), site, treatment, initial seed density (to test for densitydependent foraging), presence of road noise, and distance to road noise. If the resulting F-ratio is high in each of these analyses, there is a more statistically significant variance in GUDs. RESULTS & DISCUSSION Our model for ANOVA found several sources of variance, including a road effect, to be statistically significant (Table 2). In particular, we found that different call treatments significantly affect chipmunks perceived risk of predation. This finding corroborates that of a previous study (Schmidt et al. 2008). Chipmunks perceive their highest level of predation risk under the mobbing and seet calls, and lower levels under chipmunk and veery vocalizations (Figure 2). Statistically, GUDs during mobbing and seet calls were not significantly different from each other (P=1.000; Bonferroni HSD), and likewise between chipmunk and veery vocalizations (P>0.05; Bonferroni HSD). Brown (2004) suggests a model of fitness where the energetic cost of predation is the predation risk multiplied by survivor's fitness divided by the marginal fitness value of energy: µf/(δf/δe). Houston et al. (1993) also derive the same model for the cost of predation. You can think of the marginal fitness value of energy (δf/δe) as the benefit of harvesting or what you have to fain from foraging. It makes sense that this value is in the denominator because if that value is super high (i.e. the forager is starving), the cost of predation is relatively low and the forager is likely to continue to forage despite predation risk (µ) as a result. Survivor s fitness (F) is like what you have to lose when you get killed. For example, pregnant or young squirrels have relatively more to lose. This term considers current and future reproductive success, likelihood of surviving, etc. We should consider how the model of predation cost µf/(δf/δe) would change with the addition of road noise. Cary Institute of Ecosystem Studies 3

For instance, our main finding is that there is a significant road effect on eavesdropping systems in eastern chipmunks (Figure 3). Road noise may generally enable predators to get closer to their prey before they are recognized, increasing predation risk (µ), cost of predation, and GUD. Road noise could also have an impact from the perspective of the forager: noisy environments may mask alarm calls and render chipmunks less able to eavesdrop and collect useful information regarding predators, thus increasing predation risk (µ) and yielding the observed increase in GUD. Since our sources of variation (road, treatment, and distance) could impact each other, we include interaction effects in our model. The one we found to be significant is the road treatment effect, meaning that not all treatments respond the same way to roads. When road noise is present, mobbing and chipmunk calls yield increased GUDs whereas seet and veery vocalizations decrease GUDs (Figure 4). The chipmunk treatment might signal to nearby foragers that there are more of them around. As a result, foragers sense a greater value for survivor s fitness (F) and that they have more to lose in the event of predation. So, in the presence of road noise or potential danger, GUDs collected under chipmunk calls increase (Column 1, Figure 4). The decrease in GUDs observed under seet treatments might be because the traffic noise masks the warning call. The chipmunk consequently cannot tell where the predator is coming from, so they forage more (Column 3, Figure 4). This observation is reversed in mobbing because while seet calls have an average frequency of around 8000 Hz, mobbing calls have a much wider range of frequencies. Since anthropogenic noise usually occurs at low frequencies (<200 Hz), chipmunks may be able to hear these mobbing frequencies even in the presence of a road. Since there is not as much masking of mobbing occurring, the chipmunks have more information regarding predation risk, so it is less risky for them to forage more (Column 2, Figure 4). In the last 10 years, eavesdropping behavior has blossomed into a rapidly growing field of behavioral ecology. Auditory cues are given and received all the time. This finding that road noise has the ability to disrupt eavesdropping systems and GUDs collected under mobbing calls in particular are enhanced in the presence of road noise may be an indication that chronic road noise could also disrupt other activities like mating, avoiding predators, finding nests. When you consider that 83% of the US is within 1km of a road, this is a conservation issue that no one is considering enough. In a day like ours with species in decline and roads being built every day, it is more important than ever to understand these interactions when making decisions about conservation needs. LITERATURE CITED Brown, J.S. 1988. Patch use as an indicator of habitat preference, predation risk and competition. Behavioral Ecology & Sociobiology 22:37 47. Brown, J.S. 1992. Patch use under predation risk: I. Models and predictions. Annales Zoologici Fennici 29:301 309. Brown, J.S., and B.P. Kotler. 2004. Hazardous duty pay and the foraging cost of predation. Ecology Letters 7:999 1014. Hetrick, S.A., and K.E. Sieving. 2011. Antipredator calls of tufted titmice and interspecific transfer of encoded threat information. Behavioral Ecology. doi:10.1093/beheco/arr160. Houston, A.I., J.M. McNamara, and J.M.C. Hutchinson. 1993. General results concerning the trade-off between gaining energy and avoiding predation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 341:375 397. Cary Institute of Ecosystem Studies 4

Peake, T.M. 2005. Eavesdropping in communication networks. In: McGregor P, editor. Animal communication networks. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge University Press. Pages 13-37. Riitters, K.H., and J.D. Wickham JD. 2003. How far to the nearest road? Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment. 1:125-129. Schmidt, K.A., E. Lee, R.S. Ostfeld, and K. Sieving. 2008. Eastern chipmunks increase their perception of predation risk in response to titmouse alarm calls. Behavioral Ecology. doi:10.1093/beheco/arn034. Whelan, C.J., and G.G. Maina. 2005. Effects of season, understory vegetation density, habitat edge and tree diameter on patch-use by bark-foraging birds. Functional Ecology 19:529-536. Templeton, C. N., E. Greene, and K. Davis. 2005. Allometry of alarm calls: black-capped chickadees encode information about predator size. Science 308:1934. APPENDIX TABLE 1. Treatment times and locations. Green background indicates the presence of road noise, while red indicates its absence. TABLE 2. Analysis of Variance results. Source df MS F ratio P DAY 6 0.289 8.449 0.000 SITE 3 1.718 50.150 0.000 INITIAL 1 6.490 189.432 0.000 TREATMENT 3 0.208 6.064 0.001 DISTANCE 1 0.296 8.636 0.004 ROAD*TREATMENT 3 0.157 4.579 0.004 ROAD 1 0.153 4.468 0.036 TREATMENT*DISTANCE 3 0.070 2.029 0.112 ROAD*DISTANCE 1 0.043 1.257 0.264 Error 152 0.034 Cary Institute of Ecosystem Studies 5

FIGURE 1. Schematic diagram of each site with 3 speakers playing road noise in a line, treatment speaker in middle, and 5 subsites numbered with distance (m) from road. LN(GUD) Treatments FIGURE 2. Mean treatment ranks (±standard error) based on least square means. Cary Institute of Ecosystem Studies 6

FIGURE 3. Log-transformed GUDs based on least square means when road noise is and is not present. FIGURE 4. Log-transformed GUDs of road treatment effects based on least square means. Cary Institute of Ecosystem Studies 7