Implementation and testing of the driving scenarios in the TNO driving simulator

Similar documents
Evaluation of High Intensity Discharge Automotive Forward Lighting

Visual Effects of Light. Prof. Grega Bizjak, PhD Laboratory of Lighting and Photometry Faculty of Electrical Engineering University of Ljubljana

Discomfort and Disability Glare from Halogen and HID Headlamp Systems

Visual Effects of. Light. Warmth. Light is life. Sun as a deity (god) If sun would turn off the life on earth would extinct

Section 2 concludes that a glare meter based on a digital camera is probably too expensive to develop and produce, and may not be simple in use.

Spectral and Temporal Factors Associated with Headlight Glare: Implications for Measurement

LA DOTD s Nighttime Standards for Construction Operations. Presented by: Tom Ervin Traffic Solutions, Inc.

IMPACT OF MODERN HEADLAMPS ON THE DESIGN OF SAG VERTICAL CURVES. A Thesis Proposal by Madhuri Gogula

Roadway Glare & Reflection Technical Data

UMTRI EFFECTS OF OVERALL LOW-BEAM INTENSITY ON SEEING DISTANCE IN THE PRESENCE OF GLARE

Reflection and retroreflection

Module 3. Illumination Systems. Version 2 EE IIT, Kharagpur 1

Photometric Measurements in the Field. Ronald B. Gibbons Group Leader, Lighting and Infrastructure Technology

P1.4. Light has to go where it is needed: Future Light Based Driver Assistance Systems

ISO INTERNATIONAL STANDARD

Measurement of Visual Resolution of Display Screens

UMTRI EFFECTS OF REALISTIC LEVELS OF DIRT ON LIGHT DISTRIBUTION OF LOW-BEAM HEADLAMPS

Economic and Social Council

Tunnel and underpass lighting

Light and sight. Sight is the ability for a token to "see" its surroundings

Work on reflection properties at DELTA

SOUTH AFRICAN NATIONAL STANDARD

Effects of high power illuminators on vision through windscreens and driving behavior

Camera monitoring systems,

This paper is being distributed electronically under the Fair Use Copyright Act. One print or electronic copy may be made for personal use only.

Chapter Ray and Wave Optics

MEASURING HEAD-UP DISPLAYS FROM 2D TO AR: SYSTEM BENEFITS & DEMONSTRATION Presented By Matt Scholz November 28, 2018

Retinal stray light originating from intraocular lenses and its effect on visual performance van der Mooren, Marie Huibert

Huang Ke 1,2 *, Weng Ji 1 1 Faculty of Architecture and Urban Planning, Chongqing University, Chongqing,

Michael J. Flannagan Mchael Sivak

Optics. Experiment #4

CIE 标准目录. Spatial distribution of daylight - CIE Standard General Sky. CIE Standard Colorimetric Observers. CIE Standard llluminants for Colorimetry

RECOMMENDED TEST VOLTAGE FOR WORLDWIDE HARMONIZED HEADLAMP PHOTOMETRIC SPECIFICATIONS. Michael Sivak Michael J. Flannagan Toshio Miyokawa

(Day)light Metrics. Dr.- Ing Jan Wienold. epfl.ch Lab URL: EPFL ENAC IA LIPID

Portable retroreflectometers and structured pavement markings

soft vue 15 Like Us On:

Development of Hybrid Image Sensor for Pedestrian Detection

ISO 2575 INTERNATIONAL STANDARD. Road vehicles Symbols for controls, indicators and tell-tales

EVALUATION OF DIFFERENT MODALITIES FOR THE INTELLIGENT COOPERATIVE INTERSECTION SAFETY SYSTEM (IRIS) AND SPEED LIMIT SYSTEM

Miniaturized Spectroradiometer

How to Optimize the Sharpness of Your Photographic Prints: Part I - Your Eye and its Ability to Resolve Fine Detail

Image Characteristics and Their Effect on Driving Simulator Validity

ABSTRACT 1. INTRODUCTION

HIGHWAY ENGINEERING MODULE-IV

Report #17-UR-049. Color Camera. Jason E. Meyer Ronald B. Gibbons Caroline A. Connell. Submitted: February 28, 2017

TRAFFIC SIGN DETECTION AND IDENTIFICATION.

Introduction to Lighting

Range of Acceptable Illuminance by LED Colors in Indoor Spaces

ISO INTERNATIONAL STANDARD. Ophthalmic instruments Slit-lamp microscopes. Instruments ophtalmiques Microscopes avec lampe à fente

Design Process. ERGONOMICS in. the Automotive. Vivek D. Bhise. CRC Press. Taylor & Francis Group. Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business

Light enables organisms

Don t miss surprising. facts about the way we see

07-Lighting Concepts. EE570 Energy Utilization & Conservation Professor Henry Louie

THE RELATIVE IMPORTANCE OF PICTORIAL AND NONPICTORIAL DISTANCE CUES FOR DRIVER VISION. Michael J. Flannagan Michael Sivak Julie K.

The UK Civil Aviation Authority (CAA), reference 1, states that for onshore turbines there is:

EVALUATION OF RECENT U.S. TUNGSTEN-HALOGEN AND HID HEADLAMPS USING CHESS

Night-time scenarios in simulators

SOUTH AFRICAN NATIONAL STANDARD

Chapter 36. Image Formation

12.1. Human Perception of Light. Perceiving Light

Chapter 36. Image Formation

MERCURY-FREE HID HEADLAMPS: GLARE AND COLOR RENDERING

Development of a System to Study the Impact of Headlight Glare in a Driving Simulator

EXPERIMENT 4 INVESTIGATIONS WITH MIRRORS AND LENSES 4.2 AIM 4.1 INTRODUCTION

UMTRI EFFECTS OF REALISTIC LEVELS OF DIRT ON LIGHT OUTPUT OF REAR SIGNAL LAMPS

ID: A. Optics Review Package Answer Section TRUE/FALSE

EN Official Journal of the European Communities

DOUGLAS COUNTY ZONING RESOLUTION Section 30 Lighting Standards 3/10/99. -Section Contents-

Test 1: Example #2. Paul Avery PHY 3400 Feb. 15, Note: * indicates the correct answer.

Further reading. 1. Visual perception. Restricting the light. Forming an image. Angel, section 1.4

Contact person Date Reference Page Mikael Lindgren MTk4P (31) Measurement Technology

Visibility, Performance and Perception. Cooper Lighting

Observing a colour and a spectrum of light mixed by a digital projector

LED flicker: Root cause, impact and measurement for automotive imaging applications

Appearance Match between Soft Copy and Hard Copy under Mixed Chromatic Adaptation

Why is blue tinted backlight better?

Work environment. Retina anatomy. A human eyeball is like a simple camera! The way of vision signal. Directional sensitivity. Lighting.

Provläsningsexemplar / Preview

Solid-State Lighting Photometry Issues

ANSI/IES RP-8-14 Addendum 1 Illuminating Engineering Society; All Rights Reserved Page 1 of 2

Optical basics for machine vision systems. Lars Fermum Chief instructor STEMMER IMAGING GmbH

GEOMETRICAL OPTICS Practical 1. Part I. BASIC ELEMENTS AND METHODS FOR CHARACTERIZATION OF OPTICAL SYSTEMS

Americans with Disabilities Act and Architectural Barriers Act Accessibility Guidelines

Measurement of reflection and retroreflection

IMAGE SENSOR SOLUTIONS. KAC-96-1/5" Lens Kit. KODAK KAC-96-1/5" Lens Kit. for use with the KODAK CMOS Image Sensors. November 2004 Revision 2

Development of Gaze Detection Technology toward Driver's State Estimation

End-of-Chapter Exercises

Our Color Vision is Limited

Time Course of Chromatic Adaptation to Outdoor LED Displays

FederationofMalysia EDICTOFGOVERNMENT±

EMVA1288 compliant Interpolation Algorithm

Camera-Monitor Systems as a Replacement for Exterior Mirrors in Cars and Trucks

CS 443: Imaging and Multimedia Cameras and Lenses

AGING AND STEERING CONTROL UNDER REDUCED VISIBILITY CONDITIONS. Wichita State University, Wichita, Kansas, USA

5 _. Light. II < Object. Lesson at a Glance

IMPROVING AUTOMOTIVE INSPECTION WITH LIGHT & COLOR MEASUREMENT SYSTEMS

Chapter 23 Study Questions Name: Class:

Chapter 18 Optical Elements

LIGHT REFLECTION AND REFRACTION

APPLICATIONS FOR TELECENTRIC LIGHTING

Transcription:

TNO Memorandum TM-04-M062 Kampweg 5 P.O. Box 23 3769 ZG Soesterberg Nederland www.tno.nl Implementation and testing of the driving scenarios in the TNO driving simulator T +31 346 356 211 F +31 346 353 977 info@tm.tno.nl Date December 3rd 2004 Author(s) J.W.A.M. Alferdinck &.M. Hoedemaeker No. of copies 4 Number of pages 15 Number of appendices 2 Customer Advanced Medical Optics, Santa Ana, CA Project name Driving simulator study AMO Project number 013.74197.01.01 All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced and/or published by print, photo print, microfilm or any other means without the previous written consent of TNO. In case this report was drafted on instructions, the rights and obligations of contracting parties are subject to either the Standard Conditions for Research Instructions given to TNO, or the relevant agreement concluded between the contracting parties. Submitting the report for inspection to parties who have a direct interest is permitted. 2004 TNO

TNO memorandum TM-04-M062 December 3 rd, 2004 2 / 15 Contents 1 Introduction... 3 2 Driving scenarios... 4 2.1 Clear weather... 4 2.2 Fog 5 2.3 Glare 5 3 Traffic signs... 6 4 Hazardous events... 9 5 Pupil measurements... 11 6 Experimental design... 12 7 References... 13 8 ANNEX 1... 14 9 ANNEX 2... 15

TNO memorandum TM-04-M062 December 3 rd, 2004 3 / 15 1 Introduction Advanced Medical Optics (AMO) have asked TNO Human Factors to conduct a driving simulator study in which the performance of intraocular lenses (IOLs) is measured. In the study it is tested whether there is a significant difference between driving with the Enhanced Monofocol IOL and the standard spherical monofocol IOL. The visual aspects of driving with the above described lenses will be established by measuring traffic sign detection and recognition distances, hazard detection, and driving behavior. For this study TNO has modeled appropriate driving scenarios in our driving simulator. As requested by AMO, these scenarios consist of clear weather at night, fog at night and glare at night. The task of the participants (wearing two different IOLs) will be detection and recognition of traffic signs and detecting and avoiding hazardous events (pedestrians and objects). - The detection distance is defined as the distance at which an object is detected, without knowing the type of the object (sign, pedestrian, etc.). At this distance the object in the driving simulator is perceived as a vague blob at the horizon. In general these distances are rather large (up to 800 m). - The recognition distance is defined as the distance at which a sign or objects used in the hazard events can be identified (e.g., warning sign with to the right, place name sign with Aachen Ost, pedestrian looking to the right). This memorandum describes the implementation and testing of the driving scenarios. More specifically, it describes the imaging capabilities of the driving simulator in terms of the screen luminances and the contrasts of the signs and hazardous events. With this detailed description, the relationship between sign and hazard recognition in the simulator and in the real world is demonstrated. This memorandum also describes the details of the experimental design in terms of the order of events and the balancing of conditions over the participants.

TNO memorandum TM-04-M062 December 3 rd, 2004 4 / 15 2 Driving scenarios The driving simulator experiment consists of three different scenarios: 1. Clear weather at night 2. Fog at night 3. Glare at night In this chapter, the exact imaging capabilities in terms of luminances of the driving simulator will be described. 2.1 Clear weather All driving scenarios are modeled in a night scene. In order to get an idea of the luminance performance of the driving simulator at low light level we measured various luminances in the traffic scene, as a function of the so-called time-of-day variable (TOD), a parameter of the driving simulator. The TOD value can be varied between zero (midnight) and unity (midday), thereby controlling the corresponding light level of the driving simulator. Fig. 1 shows the measured luminances at three positions in the traffic scene of the driving simulator as a function of the TOD value: 1) road (asphalt), 2) roadside (grass) and 3) sky. The traffic scene was modeled with an overcast sky outside the buildup area. The simulator car is driving with the headlamps on. luminance (cd/m 2 ) 100 10 1 sky road road side TOD=0.07 0.1 0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1 time of day parameter TOD Fig. 1 Luminances at three position (road, roadside, sky) in the traffic scene of the driving simulator as a function of time-of- day parameter TOD. TOD=0 corresponds to midnight, TOD=1 to midday. The proposed setting of 0.07 is indicated by the dotted (vertical) line. Between a TOD of 0.1 and 1 the luminances of the three positions increase in the same manner. Below a TOD of 0.1 the luminances converge to a common luminance of 0.1 cd/m 2, the lowest luminance level the driving simulator can reach. This, in fact, is straylight luminance caused by imperfections of the projector system.

TNO memorandum TM-04-M062 December 3 rd, 2004 5 / 15 After visual inspection of the traffic scenes in the driving simulator it appears that a TOD of 0.07 is a good compromise, representing the nighttime scene rather well, without getting into trouble with the limitations of the straylight luminance. The luminance at a TOD of 0.07 has nocturnal values. The luminances of the road, road side and sky at this TOD setting are 0.28 cd/m 2 0.33 cd/m 2 and 1 cd/m 2, respectively In the literature, a ground luminance of 0.34 cd/m 2 is described as deep twilight and 0.034 cd/m 2 as clear, moonlight, measured in the real world (Douglas & Booker, 1977). 2.2 Fog The fog was simulated in the driving simulator by setting the meteorological variables at the appropriate levels. We used previously measured recognition distances as a guideline (Godthelp, 1980). A dense fog with a meteorological visibility of 200 m decreases the shape recognition distance to about 60% and the symbol recognition distance to about 75% of the distance with good meteorological visibility (Z=15 km). In a pilot experiment with two participants without IOL s we found that a meteorological visibility setting in the driving simulator of 50 m causes an average reduction of the recognition distance of signs with a factor 1.6. The effect of fog on the detection distances is high; these distances decrease with a factor ten. The detection distances are limited to about 80 m. A visibility setting of 200 m was regarded as not a natural fog scene. In close consultation with AMO (Roland Pohl) we decided that a visibility setting of 60 m is a good compromise that will probably result in detection distances of about 100 m and a realistic natural fog modeling. 2.3 Glare We have installed two glare sources. Each glare source consists of three white LEDs and a reflector built in a small lamp housing. The glare sources are fixed in the car above the dashboard at the left and the right from the line of sight. The size of the lamps is 2.5 cm x 2.5 cm. The angle between the (forward) line of sight and the glare source is 34 degrees to the left and 34 degrees to the right. The average illuminance of the combined glare sources at the eyes of the patient is 35 lx to 40 lx. The glare sources are switched on automatically when a glare condition is due. The positions and the illuminance level are chosen in close consultation with AMO (Roland Pohl). This glare condition corresponds to a discomfort glare level of about 1.3 on the scale of De Boer, which can be described as somewhere between unbearable and disturbing (Schmidt-Clausen & Bindels, 1974; Alferdinck, 1996). The disability glare, expressed in terms of the veiling luminance is 40.10/33 2 = 0.35 cd/m 2 (CIE, 2002). This veiling luminance will reduce the contrast in the scene and reduce the visibility distances of the objects. In the pilot experiment we measured that the detection distance and the recognition distance both reduce with about 20%.

TNO memorandum TM-04-M062 December 3 rd, 2004 6 / 15 3 Traffic signs When driving through the above described scenarios, the participants will be confronted with traffic signs listed in Fig. 2. The luminances of these traffic signs are modeled in such a way that the luminances and contrasts are as realistic as possible. Note that we have used four different colors, white, yellow, red, and blue. Since the participants in the experiment are from Germany and Italy, the text of the place name signs are in German or Italian. Sign type Picture Advisory Place-name Warning for either a sharp curve or an intersection Fig. 2 Traffic signs presented in the experiment We assumed that traffic signs are equipped with retroreflective sheeting, which generally is the case for almost all traffic signs on the road. Due to the retroreflection effect the signs will become visible in the light of the headlamps. The luminance of retroreflective traffic signs depends on the type of sheeting, the color of the sheeting, the luminous intensity of the headlamp, and the position of headlamp and drivers eyes. For our experiment we assumed the signs equipped with Type II retroreflective sheeting and an 50-percentile European headlamp on a passenger car. Fig. 3 shows the luminance of Type I and II retroreflective sheeting as a function of the distance between the driver s eyes and the sign (Godthelp, 1980). Due to the properties

TNO memorandum TM-04-M062 December 3 rd, 2004 7 / 15 of the retroreflective sheeting and the position of the drivers eyes, headlamps and traffic sign, the luminance is zero at a short distance, reaches a maximum between 50 m and 100 m and decreases to lower values at large distances. Note that the maximum luminance of Type II is larger than the maximum luminance of Type I, and occurs at a larger distance. The curve for Type II retroreflective material was modeled in the driving simulator. However, after visual inspection of the simulator screen it appeared that the signs were unnaturally dark at distances lower than 50 m. The signs appeared black against the relatively light sky which is caused by the limited night performance of the simulator. Therefore we decided to leave the luminance at the maximum level for distances lower than 100 m (Fig. 3). 10 Data Type I Data Type II Model Type I Model Type II Type II, simulator luminance (cd/m 2 ) 1 0.1 0 50 100 150 200 250 300 350 distance (m) Fig. 3 Luminances of the color white of Type I and II retroreflecting traffic signs as a function of viewing distance. Data = measured data (Godthelp, 1980). Model = interpolated data points. Simulator = data for use in the driving simulator. The sign dimensions are derived from the size requirements for signs on a Dutch rural road with a maximum speed of 80 km/h (NVV, 2001). For such a road the circular sign has a diameter of 800 mm, a triangular shaped sign has a side of 900 mm and a square shaped sign has a side length of 800 mm. The place name signs has a height of 340 mm. The resolution of the projection system of the driving simulator is limited to 2,1 arcminutes (1280 x 1024 pixels, width = 45 degrees, 5 degrees overlap, distance eyescreen = 3.75 m). The resolution of the human eye is about 1 arcminute. Therefore the signs shall be enlarged by about a factor two in order to end up with recognition distances similar the recognition distances measured in practice. The recognition distances were measured in the driving simulator in a pilot experiment. The results are listed in Table I and compared with the recognition distances of traffic signs in practice (Godthelp, 1980). The recognition distances on the road assume a Landolt-C visual acuity of 1 (similar to Snellen 20/20), being the 85-percentile value of the visual acuity of road users.

TNO memorandum TM-04-M062 December 3 rd, 2004 8 / 15 In order to be comparable to the distances measured on the road, the distances in the simulator are recalculated for a Landolt-C acuity of 1. Symbol recognition on the road can be compared to recognition in the simulator (Table I). Given the spread in the simulator data (two subjects) it can be concluded that the data correspond reasonably well. The detection distances are much higher than the shape recognition distances. This difference can be understood knowing that at the detection distance the shape not yet can be recognized. Table I. Distances for the shape and symbol recognition of traffic signs in practice (road) and the detection and recognition of traffic signs in the TNO driving simulator (simulator). Figures between brackets are the standard deviations. Sign Distance (m) Road Simulator Shape Symbol Detection Recognition Advisory 182-564 (44) 94 (11) Warning 253 56 475 (99) 88 (23) Speed limit 177 85 300 84 Guide - 112 516 (109) 59 (46)

TNO memorandum TM-04-M062 December 3 rd, 2004 9 / 15 4 Hazardous events The hazardous events in the experiment consist of a pedestrian looking either to the left or the right, a traffic cone, or a suitcase (see Fig. 4). Pedestrian Traffic Cone Suitcase Fig. 4 Hazardous events presented in the experiment The luminances of these object were calculated as follows. First, the headlamp luminous intensities (I) in the direction of the objects were calculated using the data for a European low beam (Sivak et al., 1994), assuming that the object appears right in front of the car at distances (d) between 10 m and 300 m. Secondly, the illuminance (E) at the objects was calculated using the inverse square law (E=I/d 2 ). Finally, the luminance (L) of the objects was calculated using the reflection properties (luminance factor β) of the objects (L=βE/π). The luminance factor of the fluorescent orange of the cone was 0.4, based on the requirements for color "fluorescent orange-red" of the international standard for fluorescent materials (CEN, 2003). The luminance factor of the shirt of the pedestrian was set at 0.8 and the luminance factor of the brown suitcase was set at 0.1. The calculated luminances were multiplied by a calibration factor in order to get realistic recognition distances in the driving simulator. The results are plotted in Fig. 5. Note that the luminances of the hazardous objects increase with decreasing distance, and are lower than the luminances of retroreflecting traffic signs for distances more than 70 m. The luminance of the T-shirt of the pedestrian on the roadside is much lower than the luminance of the other objects on the road. This is due to the properties of the headlamp beam. At short distances the luminances of the cone and suitcase are limited by the maximum luminance of the driving simulator (about 20 cd/m 2 ).

TNO memorandum TM-04-M062 December 3 rd, 2004 10 / 15 100 cone, fluorescent orange-red (0.4) 10 suitcase, brown (0.1) pedestrian, road side, white (0.8) luminance (cd/m2) 1 0.1 0.01 0 50 100 150 200 250 300 350 distance (m) Fig. 5 Luminances of three objects, illuminated by car headlamps, modeled in the driving simulator, as a function of viewing distance; two objects at the road surface (cone, fluorescent orange-red; suitcase, brown) and a white T-shirt of a pedestrian at the road side. The color of the cone was derived from the international standard, being the color in the center of the allowed color box (CIE chromaticity coordinates x=0.593 and y=0.363). After the modeling the detection and recognition distances of the objects for the hazardous event were measured in the same pilot study with two participants. The results are listed in Table II. Note that the pedestrian is detected at a rather large distance. This is due to the fact that the pedestrian is partly perceived as a silhouette against the sky. The pedestrian is recognized (recognition of the face orientation) at a rather short distance of about 50 m. The cone and the suitcase are detected only at distances between 40 m and 80 m. The recognition distance of these objects is about 50% of the detection distance. Note that due to its dark brown color, the suitcase has much shorter distances than the cone. The short detection and recognition distances of the cone and the suitcase are caused by the European headlamp shape which has a lightdark cut-of at about 60 m (Sivak et al., 1994). Table II. Detection and recognition of the objects used for the hazardous events in the TNO driving simulator, measured in a pilot experiment. The average standard deviations is about 50%.. Object Detection distance (m) Recognition distance (m) Pedestrian 206 49 Cone 75 45 Suitcase 39 17

TNO memorandum TM-04-M062 December 3 rd, 2004 11 / 15 5 Pupil measurements During the runs in the driving simulator the pupil diameter is recorded continuously. The pupil measurement device consists of a small camera and an IR illuminator mounted on a baseball cap (see Fig. 6). During the simulator drives one of the patient s eyes will be patched with a black eye patch. So, the performance and pupil size during the drives will be only of one of the eyes. Half of the drives the patients drove with the left eye and the other half with the right eye. The raw measurements are done in arbitrary pixel units. Thus, intra-subject relative pupil size is accurately recorded for all test conditions. However, each patient s absolute pupil size will be calibrated for as follows. We will measure the size of a calibration target, which is placed on the relevant (closed) eye of each subject. The calibration target consists of black circle of 10 mm diameter printed on a white thin strip (2 cm x 10 cm). Knowing the camera field of view, the diameter of the calibration target, strip thickness and an estimation of the thickness of the eyelid, the absolute pupil diameter can be calculated. Fig. 6. Participant wearing baseball cap with pupil size measurement equipment.

TNO memorandum TM-04-M062 December 3 rd, 2004 12 / 15 6 Experimental design The experiment follows a within subjects design. This means that every participant drives the 3 different scenarios in two conditions (once with each eye). After consultation with AMO it was decided that in the nighttime clear weather condition, all signs and hazards are presented in a random order. In the glare and fog conditions, half of the signs will be presented in a random order to keep driving time at a minimum. All six conditions are split in 2 drives in the simulator to reduce fatigue and allow the subjects to rest. So every participant will make 12 different drives in the simulator. In Annex 1, Table III, the distribution of the events (traffic signs and hazardous events) within each condition is described in detail. These drives or conditions are balanced as follows: - Half of the participants start driving with the right eye, the other half starts driving with the left eye. - The order of the conditions (per participant) is the same for each eye. - The order of the conditions (between participants) is different. The Latin-square method is used to balance the order of conditions over participants. In Annex 2, Table IV, the complete table with the condition order of every participant is shown. Please note that participant 1 to 16 start out with their right eye and participant 17 to 32 start out with their left eye. Participant number 33 to 37 are reserve numbers for starting out with the right eye. Participant number 38 to 42 are reserve numbers for starting out with the left eye.

TNO memorandum TM-04-M062 December 3 rd, 2004 13 / 15 7 References Alferdinck, J. W. A. M. (1996). Traffic safety aspects of high-intensity discharge headlamps: Discomfort glare and direction indicator conspicuity. In: A. G. Gale (Ed.), Vision in Vehicles V (pp. 269-276). Amsterdam: Elsevier. CEN (2003). High-visibility warning clothing for professional use - Test methods and requirements (European Standard EN 471, October 2003). Brussels: European Committee for Standardisation (CEN). CIE (2002). CIE equations for disability glare (CIE publication 146). Vienna: International Commission on Illumination CIE. Douglas, C. A. & Booker, R. L. (1977). Visual range: concepts, instrumental determination, and aviation applications (NBS Monograph 159). Washington: National Bureau of Standards NBS. Godthelp, J. (1980). Het zien van verkeersborden bij nacht [The recognition of traffic control signs during nighttime] (Rapport IZF 1980-C21). Soesterberg: TNO Technische Menskunde. NVV (2001). Bordenboek. (3 ed.) Arnhem: Nederlandse Vereniging van fabrikanten van Verkeersborden en bebakeningsmateriaal. Schmidt-Clausen, H. J. & Bindels, J. T. H. (1974). Assessment of discomfort glare in motor vehicle lighting. Lighting Research and Technology, 6, (2), 79-88. Sivak, M., Flannagan, M., & Sato, T. (1994). Light output of U.S., European, and Japanese low-beam headlamps. Transportation Research Record (1456). Soesterberg, December 3 rd 2004 J.W.A.M. Alferdinck D.M. Hoedemaeker 1 th author projectleader

TNO memorandum TM-04-M062 December 3 rd, 2004 14 / 15 8 ANNEX 1 Table III. Experimental conditions. Ped. Left, Ped. Right = pedestrian looking to the left or right. Baseline = condition with no sign or hazard event, which is used to measure the baseline driving behaviour. Condition 1A: nighttime clear weather Parking sign Ped. Left Aachen Süd Aachen Ost Baseline Traffic cone Condition 1B: nighttime clear weather Suitcase Baseline Curve sign Ped. Right Inters. Sign Straight sign Condition 2A: nighttime clear weather Curve sign Baseline Parking sign Suitcase Aachen Süd Ped. left Condition 2B: nighttime clear weather Aachen Ost Traffic cone Ped. Right Inters. sign Straight sign Baseline Condition 3A: Fog right eye Aachen Süd Baseline Suitcase Ped. Left Straight sign Condition 3B: Fog right eye Traffic cone Ped. Right Baseline Curve sign Condition 4A: Fog left eye Baseline Parking sign Ped. Left Suitcase Aachen Ost Condition 4B: Fog left eye Ped. Right Inters. sign Traffic cone Baseline Condition 5A: Glare right eye Ped. Left Traffic cone Straight sign Baseline Condition 5B: Glare right eye Baseline Ped. Right Aachen Süd Curve sign Suitcase Condition 6A: Glare left eye Parking sign Aachen Ost Baseline Ped. Right Condition 6B: Glare left eye Baseline Traffic cone Ped. Left Suitcase Inters. Sign

TNO memorandum TM-04-M062 December 3 rd, 2004 15 / 15 9 ANNEX 2 Table IV. Order of the experimental conditions for each patient (pp). For experimental condition see Table III. pp 1 1a 3a 5a 1b 3b 5b 2a 4a 6a 2b 4b 6b 2 5b 1a 3a 5a 1b 3b 6b 2a 4a 6a 2b 4b 3 3b 5b 1a 3a 5a 1b 4b 6b 2a 4a 6a 2b 4 1b 3b 5b 1a 3a 5a 2b 4b 6b 2a 4a 6a 5 5a 1b 3b 5b 1a 3a 6a 2b 4b 6b 2a 4a 6 3a 5a 1b 3b 5b 1a 4a 6a 2b 4b 6b 2a 7 5b 3b 1b 5a 3a 1a 6b 4b 2b 6a 4a 2a 8 1a 5b 3b 1b 5a 3a 2a 6b 4b 2b 6a 4a 9 3a 1a 5b 3b 1b 5a 4a 2a 6b 4b 2b 6a 10 5a 3a 1a 5b 3b 1b 6a 4a 2a 6b 4b 2b 11 1b 5a 3a 1a 5b 3b 2b 6a 4a 2a 6b 4b 12 3b 1b 5a 3a 1a 5b 4b 2b 6a 4a 2a 6b 13 1a 5a 3a 1b 5b 3b 2a 6a 4a 2b 6b 4b 14 3b 1a 5a 3a 1b 5b 4b 2a 6a 4a 2b 6b 15 5b 3b 1a 5a 3a 1b 6b 4b 2a 6a 4a 2b 16 1b 5b 3b 1a 5a 3a 2b 6b 4b 2a 6a 4a 17 2a 4a 6a 2b 4b 6b 1a 3a 5a 1b 3b 5b 18 6b 2a 4a 6a 2b 4b 5b 1a 3a 5a 1b 3b 19 4b 6b 2a 4a 6a 2b 3b 5b 1a 3a 5a 1b 20 2b 4b 6b 2a 4a 6a 1b 3b 5b 1a 3a 5a 21 6a 2b 4b 6b 2a 4a 5a 1b 3b 5b 1a 3a 22 4a 6a 2b 4b 6b 2a 3a 5a 1b 3b 5b 1a 23 6b 4b 2b 6a 4a 2a 5b 3b 1b 5a 3a 1a 24 2a 6b 4b 2b 6a 4a 1a 5b 3b 1b 5a 3a 25 4a 2a 6b 4b 2b 6a 3a 1a 5b 3b 1b 5a 26 6a 4a 2a 6b 4b 2b 5a 3a 1a 5b 3b 1b 27 2b 6a 4a 2a 6b 4b 1b 5a 3a 1a 5b 3b 28 4b 2b 6a 4a 2a 6b 3b 1b 5a 3a 1a 5b 29 2a 6a 4a 2b 6b 4b 1a 5a 3a 1b 5b 3b 30 4b 2a 6a 4a 2b 6b 3b 1a 5a 3a 1b 5b 31 6b 4b 2a 6a 4a 2b 5b 3b 1a 5a 3a 1b 32 2b 6b 4b 2a 6a 4a 1b 5b 3b 1a 5a 3a 33 1a 3a 5a 1b 3b 5b 2a 4a 6a 2b 4b 6b 34 5b 1a 3a 5a 1b 3b 6b 2a 4a 6a 2b 4b 35 3b 5b 1a 3a 5a 1b 4b 6b 2a 4a 6a 2b 36 1b 3b 5b 1a 3a 5a 2b 4b 6b 2a 4a 6a 37 5a 1b 3b 5b 1a 3a 6a 2b 4b 6b 2a 4a 38 2a 4a 6a 2b 4b 6b 1a 3a 5a 1b 3b 5b 39 6b 2a 4a 6a 2b 4b 5b 1a 3a 5a 1b 3b 40 4b 6b 2a 4a 6a 2b 3b 5b 1a 3a 5a 1b 41 2b 4b 6b 2a 4a 6a 1b 3b 5b 1a 3a 5a 42 6a 2b 4b 6b 2a 4a 5a 1b 3b 5b 1a 3a