Validation of an Economican Fast Method to Evaluate Situationspecific Parameters of Traffic Safety

Similar documents
The Perception of Optical Flow in Driving Simulators

Development and Validation of Virtual Driving Simulator for the Spinal Injury Patient

Multi variable strategy reduces symptoms of simulator sickness

EVALUATION OF DIFFERENT MODALITIES FOR THE INTELLIGENT COOPERATIVE INTERSECTION SAFETY SYSTEM (IRIS) AND SPEED LIMIT SYSTEM

EFFECTS OF A NIGHT VISION ENHANCEMENT SYSTEM (NVES) ON DRIVING: RESULTS FROM A SIMULATOR STUDY

CAN GALVANIC VESTIBULAR STIMULATION REDUCE SIMULATOR ADAPTATION SYNDROME? University of Guelph Guelph, Ontario, Canada

Comparison of Wrap Around Screens and HMDs on a Driver s Response to an Unexpected Pedestrian Crossing Using Simulator Vehicle Parameters

Assessments of Grade Crossing Warning and Signalization Devices Driving Simulator Study

The application of Work Domain Analysis (WDA) for the development of vehicle control display

Driver Education Classroom and In-Car Curriculum Unit 3 Space Management System

Image Characteristics and Their Effect on Driving Simulator Validity

THE EFFECTS OF PC-BASED TRAINING ON NOVICE DRIVERS RISK AWARENESS IN A DRIVING SIMULATOR

Abstract. 1. Introduction

Chapter 9. Conclusions. 9.1 Summary Perceived distances derived from optic ow

Using Driving Simulator for Advance Placement of Guide Sign Design for Exits along Highways

Fig.2 the simulation system model framework

Validation of stopping and turning behavior for novice drivers in the National Advanced Driving Simulator

Early Take-Over Preparation in Stereoscopic 3D

Perception in Immersive Environments

Work Domain Analysis (WDA) for Ecological Interface Design (EID) of Vehicle Control Display

The Impact of Road Familiarity on the Perception of Traffic Signs Eye Tracking Case Study

Haptic Camera Manipulation: Extending the Camera In Hand Metaphor

PHOTOGRAMMETRIC ADVANCED DETECTION SOLUTION INCIDENT DETECTION IN TUNNELS

Real Time and Non-intrusive Driver Fatigue Monitoring

Running an HCI Experiment in Multiple Parallel Universes

P1.4. Light has to go where it is needed: Future Light Based Driver Assistance Systems

IMPROVEMENTS TO A QUEUE AND DELAY ESTIMATION ALGORITHM UTILIZED IN VIDEO IMAGING VEHICLE DETECTION SYSTEMS

Outlook on Candidate Performance Specifications for QRTV

Adaptive Controllers for Vehicle Velocity Control for Microscopic Traffic Simulation Models

A reduction of visual fields during changes in the background image such as while driving a car and looking in the rearview mirror

Research on visual physiological characteristics via virtual driving platform

SET-UP QUALIFYING. x7 x4 x2. x1 x3

DESIGN OF VOICE ALARM SYSTEMS FOR TRAFFIC TUNNELS: OPTIMISATION OF SPEECH INTELLIGIBILITY

Depth Perception in Virtual Reality: Distance Estimations in Peri- and Extrapersonal Space ABSTRACT

Connected Car Networking

NAVIGATIONAL CONTROL EFFECT ON REPRESENTING VIRTUAL ENVIRONMENTS

Engage Examine the picture on the left. 1. What s happening? What is this picture about?

ERDS Simulator Emergency Response Driving Simulator

TRAFFIC SIGN DETECTION AND IDENTIFICATION.

Time Scales Comparisons Using Simultaneous Measurements in Three Frequency Channels

Proposed Watertown Plan Road Interchange Evaluation Using Full Scale Driving Simulator

MEM380 Applied Autonomous Robots I Winter Feedback Control USARSim

Advancing Simulation as a Safety Research Tool

Preliminary evaluation of a virtual reality-based driving assessment test

Activity 1 Position, Velocity, Acceleration PHYS 010

Motion Graphs. Plotting distance against time can tell you a lot about motion. Let's look at the axes:

DIFFERENCE BETWEEN A PHYSICAL MODEL AND A VIRTUAL ENVIRONMENT AS REGARDS PERCEPTION OF SCALE

Visual Physics Lab Project 1

Experimental study of traffic noise and human response in an urban area: deviations from standard annoyance predictions

Traffic Control for a Swarm of Robots: Avoiding Group Conflicts

Acoustic Based Angle-Of-Arrival Estimation in the Presence of Interference

Evaluation of automated vehicle behavior in intersection scenarios

ITDNS Design and Applications (2010 present)

Managing traffic through Signal Performance Measures in Pima County

Theoretical Aircraft Overflight Sound Peak Shape

THE EFFECTIVENESS OF SAFETY CAMPAIGN VMS MESSAGES - A DRIVING SIMULATOR INVESTIGATION

Frequently Asked Questions

Trip Assignment. Lecture Notes in Transportation Systems Engineering. Prof. Tom V. Mathew. 1 Overview 1. 2 Link cost function 2

Experimental Setup of Motion Sickness and Situation Awareness in Automated Vehicle Riding Experience

Nagoya University Center of Innovation (COI)

Volumetric positioning accuracy of a vertical machining center equipped with linear motor drives (evaluated by the laser vector method)

New Method for Evaluating Light Source Color Rendition (IES TM-30-15)

Comparison of Three Eye Tracking Devices in Psychology of Programming Research

Adaptive Tutoring on a Virtual Reality Driving Simulator

2. Overall Use of Technology Survey Data Report

Human Factors and Road Engineering Mechanism Engineering-Psychologist Dr. Sibylle Birth, SoRSA Conference, 26./27.06.

Motion Graphs Teacher s Guide

HCM Roundabout Capacity Methods and Alternative Capacity Models

Steering a Driving Simulator Using the Queueing Network-Model Human Processor (QN-MHP)

Poles for Increasing the Sensibility of Vertical Gradient. in a Downhill Road

An Application for Driving Simulator Technology: An Evaluation of Traffic Signal Displays for Protected-Permissive Left-Turn Control

The Representational Effect in Complex Systems: A Distributed Representation Approach

Evaluation based on drivers' needs analysis

Cybersickness, Console Video Games, & Head Mounted Displays

Developing Frogger Player Intelligence Using NEAT and a Score Driven Fitness Function

The development of a virtual laboratory based on Unreal Engine 4

Evaluation of Connected Vehicle Technology for Concept Proposal Using V2X Testbed

Simulator Sickness Questionnaire: Twenty Years Later

EFFECT OF SIMULATOR MOTION SPACE

Multi-Modality Fidelity in a Fixed-Base- Fully Interactive Driving Simulator

WHITE PAPER. Sensor Comparison: Are All IMXs Equal? Contents. 1. The sensors in the Pregius series

COMPARING TECHNIQUES TO REDUCE SIMULATOR ADAPTATION SYNDROME AND IMPROVE NATURALISTIC BEHAVIOUR DURING SIMULATED DRIVING

Use of Probe Vehicles to Increase Traffic Estimation Accuracy in Brisbane

Deep Learning for Autonomous Driving

ADAS Development using Advanced Real-Time All-in-the-Loop Simulators. Roberto De Vecchi VI-grade Enrico Busto - AddFor

Stanford Center for AI Safety

Discriminating direction of motion trajectories from angular speed and background information

The effect of 3D audio and other audio techniques on virtual reality experience

AGING AND STEERING CONTROL UNDER REDUCED VISIBILITY CONDITIONS. Wichita State University, Wichita, Kansas, USA

Urban Accessibility: perception, measurement and equitable provision

Phased Array Velocity Sensor Operational Advantages and Data Analysis

CogniTo ltd. Effective Traffic Psychology. Or: Preventing crashes is possible, predictable and profitable

Consideration of Tool Chamfer for Realistic Application of the Incremental Hole-Drilling Method

Intelligent Agents & Search Problem Formulation. AIMA, Chapters 2,

The criterion of noise attenuation by hedges

Available online at ScienceDirect. Procedia Engineering 142 (2016 )

Intelligent Driving Agents

Probabilities and Probability Distributions

Machine Trait Scales for Evaluating Mechanistic Mental Models. of Robots and Computer-Based Machines. Sara Kiesler and Jennifer Goetz, HCII,CMU

PeakVue Analysis for Antifriction Bearing Fault Detection

Transcription:

Validation of an Economican Fast Method to Evaluate Situationspecific Parameters of Traffic Safety Katharina Dahmen-Zimmer, Kilian Ehrl, Alf Zimmer University of Regensburg Experimental Applied Psychology Unit Regensburg, Germany The need for efficient simulators for training and investigating complex and especially risky tasks is obvious. Accordingly simulators for as diverse tasks as flying a plane, driving a car, flight control and operating nuclear reactors have been developed. While the benefits of simulator training are easy to see, for instance, dangerous situations can be trained without any risk for personnel of machinery, the question how valid a simulation is, that is, how similar is the behavior in the simulator to the behavior in real-life situations quite often is left open or only answered by appealing to the obvious simularity in the layout of instruments plus the physical characteristics of controls and the realism of the depictured visual scene. Especially, the lacking degree of realism in computer generated visual scenes has been criticized: the lack of non-geometric perspective cues as haze or blue-shift, the unnatural regularity of buildings, the crispness of contures, and, in general, the lack of realistic clutter starting from pedestrians on seemingly random courses to debris and discolorations of the surfaces. This situation has led to the development of video-simulators (2D and 3D) at the Experimental Applied Psychology Unit at the University of Regensburg, among others (for an overview, especially for research in the US, see MacAdam, 1993); in this simulation methodology videos from real traffic scenes are used. The simulator developed in the Experimental Applied Psychology Unit at the University in Regensburg consists of a BMW limousine, where all the controls and displays are linked to a computer, and a video projector producing for the driver a visual scene with a visual angle of about 45º. It is obvious, that such a kind of simulation is sensitive only to the skills on the basic level of Janssen s (1979) hierarchy of the driving task (van der Molen & Bötticher, 1988): Subjects can accelerate and decelerate the speed of the video, and steering control can be simulated by keeping a target in the middle of the lane. Even on this level the task is not as interactive as in full fledged computer simulations because deceleration and acceleration influence not only the simulated car in relation to the stable environment but also influences all other moving objects in the video. Despite these draw-backs, the advantage of video

simulation lies in the realism of the depicted scenes. In our experiments we were especially interested in drivers speeding behavior, for the adjusted speed is the main cause of traffic car accidents. In order to determine the validity of this simulation technique the correspondences with reallife driving have been determined on three different levels corresponding to three separable research questions: (i) Do individually different driving styles induce a corresponding regulation of velocity in the simulation and in the real-life driving? (ii) Do different driving tasks (velocity maintenance vs. self paced driving) lead to the same effects in those situations? and (iii) Do corresponding situations in the video and in the real world give rise to the same pattern of acceleration or deceleration? Additionally, subjective evaluations of both tasks have been elicited with questionnaires in order to check if they are rated in such a different way that the experiences cannot be compared. Method The general experimental plan is shown in Table 1. Table 1: TASK SEX FIRST CONDITION female real life driving style simulatordriving style speed maintenance male real life driving style simulator driving style female real life driving style simulator driving style free driving male real life driving style simulator driving style Additionally, in a questionnaire data about the subjects driving style were obtained. As in Assmann (1985) nearly all variance could be attributed to three factors, namely, Factor 1: following the traffic flow,

Factor 2: attitudes towards car driving, and Factor 3: judging once own driving ability. Using factor scores to classify subjects it was possible to determine the influence of the driving style in the two experimental situations. Table 2 gives an overview over the route used in the experiments; numbers indicate the corresponding situations. In order to determine if accidental influences in the real-life driving influence the choice of velocity any of the following observations were timecoded: oncoming traffic, following traffic, slow traffic ahead, passing or being passed, cyclists on the lane, pedestrians on the lane or immediately beside the lane, and children close to the road. The real-life driving was done in a BMW 730i with an automatic transmission. During the field experiment all the relevant driving parameters were entered into an on-board computer. Table 2: # Traffic situation or road section respectively Speed limit Description 1 Segment of a county road 60 km/h straight, flat 2 Entering a village with a reduced speed zone 60 km/h slightly curved, incline (Oberisling) 30 km/h 3 Passage through a reduced speed zone 30 km/h slight decline, straight ahead (Oberisling) 4 Leaving the reduced speed zone 30 km/h right turn, approximately 60 (Oberisling) 60 km/h 5 Entering the reduced speed zone 60 km/h straight ahead, flat (Leoprechting) 30 km/h 6 Leaving the reduced speed zone 30 km/h straight ahead, flat (Leoprechting) 60 km/h 7 Entering the reduced speed zone (Graß) 60 km/h 30 km/h straight ahead, priority of the turning road 8 First T-intersection in Graß (without traffic signs) 30 km/h 90 -turn to the right, with right of the way 9 Second T-intersection in Graß 30 km/h 90 -turn to the right, while (Sign: yield") respecting the yield sign 10 Free lane road 60 km/h straight ahead, slight decline 50 km/h 11 Sleeping policemen with following cross-walk for 30 km/h straight ahead, slight decline pedestrians 12 Sleeping policemen 30 km/h straight ahead, slight decline 13 Connecting road 30 km/h straight ahead, slight decline

52 subjects participated in the experiment, 26 female and 26 male, in the age range from 19 to 26 years (mean 24,1 years). Subjects had their driving licences between 1.8 and 11.8 years (mean 6 years). 26 of the subjects were driving more than 10 000 km/year, 26 less. Half of the subjects have had experience with an automatic transmission. Subjects got a fee of 15 DM. Two subjects missed either the simulator or the real-life driving situation, therefore their data could not be used in the comparison of the corresponding behavior. The simulator consists of a BMW 325i, where by means of the above-described projection technique driving can be simulated. Figure 1 shows what has been simulated for the speed regulation and what variables have been skipped. Projection LCD- Videoof scenes Projector recorder Visual simulation Vision Speedometer Kinaesthetics Calibrated Computer with (partially) amplifier car model Brain Motor effectors Controls Videofilm simulated Hearing Noise simulation not simulated Vestibular system Movement simulation Driver Simulator Figure 1: Influence diagram of the driver-simulator system

During simulation a virtual velocity is shown on the speedometer in the car. In order to influence the speed of the video realistically, the pressure on the effectors in the car (brakes and accelerator) are fed into a computer where by means of a formal car model the virtual speed is determined according to the following formula: tf v = psd ( 2 u ( g b / u ) u v psd y ) dt t0 u = 1 - ( vpsd / vmax ) acceleration depending on the given velocity g = g 0 * 1,5 pressure on the accelerator b = b 0 * 4,0 pressure on the brake y = y 0 * 0,15 = 0,0 03 rolling and air resistance Due to the fact that the available videorecorder (Panasonic AG 7330) can not increase and decrease continually the following relation between the virtual velocity as shown on the speedometer and the projection velocity is as in Figure 2. Figure 2: Relation between pseudospeed and recorder speed

Results In general, subjects regard driving in real-life situations as easier (median answer: easy ) than in the simulator (median answer: rather diffucult ). Furthermore, the simulator driving is regarded as needing more attention. Despite these differences, subjects when asked if the simulator influences their driving behavior, describe this influence as negligable (modal answer: slightly if at all ). For this reason it can be assumed that the driving experience is comparable for both the field and the simulator. Three analyses of variance regarding driving styles with the factor scores of the questionnaire on driving styles as the dependent variable, reveal no significant differences between real-life driving and simulator-driving with one exception: There is an interaction between the values of the factor 1 ( going with the traffic flow ) and the driving situation. The Scheffé-Test reveals that subjects who go with the flow increase the speed in the simulator in contrast to the subjects with negative factor scores who drive faster in the real-life situation. An analysis of variance of the task ( speed maintenance vs. self-paced driving) shows significant main effects for the speed maxima and the average speed. The task speed maintenance induces slower driving (53,5 km/h) with less variance (1,62) than self-paced driving (67,8 km/h and 2,71 respectively) but no significant interaction with the driving situation (real-life vs. simulator). Furthermore, there is no significant influence of sex on the velocity regulation.

If one determines the average maximal velocity for the 13 situations (see Table 2) the following curves for real-life driving (upper curve) and simulator driving (lower curve) result (see Figure 5) Figure 5: Mean values of the maximal velocities for the 13 situations For all 13 situations (see Table 2) and all subjects the data for the real-life driving and for the simulator driving results of the scattergram in Figure 6. Figure 6: Scattergram of driven velocities per situation in real-life driving and in simulator driving for all subjects (r =.7158; Regression (real-life) speed = 12.983 +.852 * simulatorspeed

For all 13 situations (see Table 2) and all subjects the data for the real-life driving and for the simulator driving results of the scattergram in Figure 6. Figure 6: Scattergram of driven velocities per situation in real-life driving and in simulator driving for all subjects (r =.7158; Regression (real-life) speed = 12.983 +.852 * simulatorspeed If one reduces the noise and uses only the mean velocities for the 13 situations the scattergram of Figure 7 results.

Figure 7: Real-life driving velocities vs. simulator driving velocities (r =.982; Regression: (real-life) speed = 6.48 + 1.02 * simulatorspeed) Discussion The data from the questionnaire where the subjects describe their experiences in the real-life driving situation and the simulator situation reveal that both situations are regarded as equally interesting and not influencing ones driving behavior. However, driving in the simulator is regarded as more difficult and requiring more attention. The reason for this might be that the simulator situation is novel and that the velocities in the simulator are not changed continuously but in steps. The fact that driving in the simulator is experienced as difficult and requiring special attention indicates that the motivation of the subjects is high even in the simulator situation. That is, when driving in the simulator subjects tried to exhibit normal driving behavior. This can be seen not only from the subjective reports but also from the fact that the observed driving style does not influence the driving in the two situations differently except for the interaction between going with the flow and average speed regulation, but also this influence is very slight. The conclusion can be drawn that in the two situations there is no differentiating influence of driving attituedes on the real-live vs. simulator driving.

Most important for the validation of the simulator are the situation dependent correlations of situation specific velocities in the simulator and in the real-life situation. Accelerating and decelerating behavior in real-life and simulator driving correspond nearly perfectly. The main influence on the driving behavior are the situational characteristics independently from the fact whether subjects experience real-life driving or simulator driving. If one analyzes the individual correlations between the simulator data and the real-life data for the 13 situations, it turns out that only 4 subjects show correlations less than.5 but more than 75 % of the subjects show correlations above.85. The effect that the regression coefficient does not differ significantly from 1 shows that there is nearly a one-to-one correspondence between the driving behavior in real-life and in the simulator, only the additive constant of about 6 km/h shows the influence of a tunnel effect in the simulator. Conclusion Standard approaches for estimating the validity of measures start from the theoretical assumption that the true validity only depends on the correlation between the true values in the measure and the true values in the criterion. These true values cannot be measured directly and are practically always confounded with the covariation of systematic errors. For these reasons the selection of situations, measures etc. has to be planned exactly: For instance, in the case of the evaluation of our video simulator it was not important that individual characteristics of drivers ( speeding vs. dawdling etc.) show in both situations but that the same situational factors elicit corresponding kinds of behavior, that is, for validating a simulator aimed at the improvement of traffic conditions the decisive carrier of information is not the individual driver but the specific situation. Generalizability of results therefore depends on the representativity of the situations. Insofar the video simulator can be regarded as a valid system for determinating situational characteristics of traffic safety. Since finishing this field and simulator study the video simulator has been further improved, (i) it is now possible to increase and decrease the projection velocity continually, (ii) steering control can be simulated additionally by shifting the video picture to the left or right in accordance with a computerbased model of steering.

References Janssen, W.H. (1979) Routeplanning en geleiding: een literatuurstudie (Rapport IZF 1979- C13). Soesterberg (NL): Institute for perception. MacAdam, C.C., Green, P.A. & Reed, M.P. (1993) An overview of current UMTRI driving simulators. UMTRI Research Review, 24 (1), 1-8. van der Molen, H.H., Bötticher, A.M.T. (1988) A hierarchical risk-model for traffic participants. Ergonomics, 31, 537-555.