WINDOWS OF OPPORTUNITY:

Similar documents
Transferring knowledge from operations to the design and optimization of work systems: bridging the offshore/onshore gap

Behaviors That Revolve Around Working Effectively with Others Behaviors That Revolve Around Work Quality

Part 5 Mindful Movement and Mindfulness and Change and Organizational Excellence (Paul Kurtin)

Getting the Best Performance from Challenging Control Loops

COMPETITIVE ADVANTAGES AND MANAGEMENT CHALLENGES. by C.B. Tatum, Professor of Civil Engineering Stanford University, Stanford, CA , USA

Academic Vocabulary Test 1:

DreamCatcher Agile Studio: Product Brochure

PRIMATECH WHITE PAPER COMPARISON OF FIRST AND SECOND EDITIONS OF HAZOP APPLICATION GUIDE, IEC 61882: A PROCESS SAFETY PERSPECTIVE

Revisiting the USPTO Concordance Between the U.S. Patent Classification and the Standard Industrial Classification Systems

MANAGING PEOPLE, NOT JUST R&D: FIVE COMPANIES EXPERIENCES

5th-discipline Digital IQ assessment

the meeting stress test study: The business impact of technology induced meeting stress

Technology Needs Assessment

in the New Zealand Curriculum

Time And Resource Characteristics Of Radical New Product Development (NPD) Projects And their Dynamic Control. Introduction. Problem Description.

Laboratory 1: Uncertainty Analysis

Mission: Materials innovation

School Based Projects

Part 1. c01.qxd 9/4/2003 8:31 AM Page 1

Human-Computer Interaction

Article. The Internet: A New Collection Method for the Census. by Anne-Marie Côté, Danielle Laroche

Automating the math makes analytics more democratic and more human

LETTER FROM THE EXECUTIVE DIRECTOR FOREWORD BY JEFFREY KRAUSE

Must the Librarian Be Underdog?

WHAT CLICKS? THE MUSEUM DIRECTORY

REPORT ON THE EUROSTAT 2017 USER SATISFACTION SURVEY

Identifying and Managing Joint Inventions

ty of solutions to the societal needs and problems. This perspective links the knowledge-base of the society with its problem-suite and may help

SATELLITE NETWORK NOTIFICATION AND COORDINATION REGULATIONS 2007 BR 94/2007

Patents. What is a patent? What is the United States Patent and Trademark Office (USPTO)? What types of patents are available in the United States?

AWARENESS Being Aware. Being Mindful Self-Discovery. Self-Awareness. Being Present in the Moment.

A review of the role and costs of clinical commissioning groups

CMS.608 / CMS.864 Game Design Spring 2008

The Job Interview: Here are some popular questions asked in job interviews:

Imagine your future lab. Designed using Virtual Reality and Computer Simulation

Chapter 6: Finding and Working with Professionals

DRAWING MANAGEMENT MISTAKES

Online Resource to The evolution of sanctioning institutions: an experimental approach to the social contract

Information Sociology

McGraw-Hill/Irwin. Copyright 2011 by the McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. All rights reserved.

System of Systems Software Assurance

"Financing for Your Startup You Got to Tell a Story

TABLE OF CONTENTS...2 SAM VIBE - OVERVIEW...3 WORKING WITH THE SAM VIBE SCHEDULE...3 PLAYBLOCKS...4 CREATING A PLAYBLOCK...4 EXAMPLE PLAYBLOCKS...

To track responses to texts and use those responses as a point of departure for talking or writing about texts

From the Experts: Ten Tips to Save Costs in Patent Litigation

Basic IMS A R E S. Amateur Radio Emergency Communications. IMS For Amateur Radio. Self Study Training Course. Amateur Radio Emergency Service

Leadership: Getting and Giving the Call for Action

The ALA and ARL Position on Access and Digital Preservation: A Response to the Section 108 Study Group

1. Historical Development of SSDMs

How Representation of Game Information Affects Player Performance

Exposure Draft Definition of Material. Issues Paper - Towards a Draft Comment Letter

A PATH DEPENDENT PERSPECTIVE OF THE TRANSFORMATION TO LEAN PRODUCTION ABSTRACT INTRODUCTION

happiness.* BY BRYAN IRWIN AND ALIZA LEVENTHAL

Robot Olympics: Programming Robots to Perform Tasks in the Real World

DOW IMPROVES INSTRUMENT RELIABILITY 66% AND SAVES MILLIONS OF DOLLARS WITH REAL-TIME HART TECHNOLOGY

Enhancing industrial processes in the industry sector by the means of service design

1. Executive Summary. 2. Introduction. Selection of a DC Solar PV Arc Fault Detector

A Guide to Prepare For Your Industry Interview

Compendium Overview. By John Hagel and John Seely Brown

CCG 360 o Stakeholder Survey

Balancing enormous financial risk with cyclical market demands is like a no limit poker game

PEY Report. Lee Holmes. E-Commerce Technical Specialist, GE Global Exchange Services

Human-computer Interaction Research: Future Directions that Matter

Beta Testing For New Ways of Sitting

20 WAYS TO IMPROVE YOUR FINANCES IN UNDER 20 MINUTES

Infrastructure for Systematic Innovation Enterprise

State of IT Research Study

INTRODUCTION TO COMPUTER NUMERICAL CONTROL

APPROXIMATE KNOWLEDGE OF MANY AGENTS AND DISCOVERY SYSTEMS

DMSMS Management: After Years of Evolution, There s Still Room for Improvement

Module 5 Design for Reliability and Quality. IIT, Bombay

INTEGRIS 3-D Printing: How Rapid Prototyping Saved Our Client Money

HOW TO HIRE AN ARCHITECT

TITLE V. Excerpt from the July 19, 1995 "White Paper for Streamlined Development of Part 70 Permit Applications" that was issued by U.S. EPA.

At the Heart of Amazing Experiences

SIMULATION IMPROVES OPERATOR TRAINING ARTICLE FOR SEP/OCT 2011 INTECH

1 NOTE: This paper reports the results of research and analysis

2. Overall Use of Technology Survey Data Report

REPORT OF THE UNITED STATES OF AMERICA ON THE 2010 WORLD PROGRAM ON POPULATION AND HOUSING CENSUSES

Paid Surveys Secret. The Most Guarded Secret Top Survey Takers Cash In and Will Never Tell You! Top Secret Report. Published by Surveys & Friends

R&D PROJECT MANAGEMENT IS IT AGILE?

Loop Design. Chapter Introduction

WORKPLACE INKJET PRINTERS: NOW BUILT TO MEET THE DEMANDS OF THE MODERN WORKPLACE

An Evaluation of Artifact Calibration in the 5700A Multifunction Calibrator

Danmarks Mediemuseum From Printing Museum to Media Museum

Coaching Questions From Coaching Skills Camp 2017

CHAPTER 8 RESEARCH METHODOLOGY AND DESIGN

CONTACTS: Michaela Hessling Executive Director Corporate Communications Tel

The Profitable Side Project Handbook

Fostering Innovative Ideas and Accelerating them into the Market

The Perfect Week. (continued on next page) Ed Howat, Jr., CLU, ChFC, LUTCF, RCC Addie Woods Consulting Co. LLC

Pro Bono Strategic Plan 03/07/05

Financial Review 2013/14. Context

all-in-one meeting guide How to Gain Control of Your Time

Randomized Evaluations in Practice: Opportunities and Challenges. Kyle Murphy Policy Manager, J-PAL January 30 th, 2017

TEMPERATURE MAPPING SOFTWARE FOR SINGLE-CELL CAVITIES*

Technology Transfer: An Integrated Culture-Friendly Approach

GOAL SETTING NOTES. How can YOU expect to hit a target you that don t even have?

Sensible Chuckle SuperTuxKart Concrete Architecture Report

Report to Congress regarding the Terrorism Information Awareness Program

Transcription:

OpVEY HD28.M414 ^' flc THIS MATERIAL M^V BE ^*" rtu ' '^'Jrn Rv COPYRIGHT LAW c "StuV7U.S.C00 ) WINDOWS OF OPPORTUNITY: TEMPORAL PATTERNS OF TECHNOLOGICAL ADAPTATION IN ORGANIZATIONS Marcie J. Tyre Wanda J. Orlikowski July 1991 (Revised September 1992) CISR WP No. 227 Sloan WP No. 3309 1991, 1992 M.J. Tyre and W.J. Orlikowski Forthcoming in Organization Science. Center for Information Systems Research Sloan School of Management Massachusetts Institute of Technology

WINDOWS OF OPPORTUNITY: Temporal Patterns of Technological Adaptation in Organizations Abstract This paper examines the introduction and adaptation of technologies that support productive operations. The authors argue that the process of technological adaptation is not gradual and continuous, as often argued in the innovation literature, but is instead highly discontinuous. Evidence from three manufactunng and service organizations indicates that there exists a relatively brief window of opportunity to explore and modify new process technology following initial implementation. Afterwards, modification of new process technologies by users is_ limited by the increasing routinization that occurs with_experience. Thus, the technology and its context of use tend to congeal, often embedding unresolved problems into organizational practice. Subsequent changes appear U3 occur in an episodic manner, niggered either by discrepant events or by new discoveries on the part of users. These findings have important implications for theones of technological change. "Here is Edward Bear, coming downstairs now, bump, bump, bump, on the back of his head behind Christopher Robin. It is, as far as he knows, the only way of coming downstairs, but sometimes he feels that there really is another way, if only he could stop bumping for a moment and think of it..." (Milne, 1926:3)

ADAPTATION OF TECHNOLOGIES IN USE New technologies are almost never perfect upon initial introduction. Instead, users' effons to apply technologies reveal problems and contingencies that were not apparent before introduction (Rosenberg, 1982; Dutton and Thomas, 1985). These problems, in turn, require adaptation of the technologies already in use. A close understanding of the process of adaptation is critical for several reasons. First, users' adaptations to technologies-in-use often help to shape funher development and research activities (von Hippel. 1988; Dutton and Thomas, 1985). Second, the operating efficiency ultimately achieved with a new technology depends heavily on users' modifications (Enos, 1958; Hollander, 1965; Dutton and Thomas. 1984). Third, modifications affect not just the technologyin-use, but also its physical and organizational context (Leonard-Banon, 1988). As Van de Ven (1986:591) points out, once in use, new technologies "not only adapt to existing organizational and industnal artangemenis, but they also transform the structure and practices of these environments." Thus, only by understanding how such adaptations occur can we begin to build more adequate theories of technological change in organizanons. The process of technological adaptation, however, is not yet well understood, and an imponant area of uncertainty involves the timing of adaptations. The objective of the current paper is to explore this issue by examining two questions. First, what is the pattern of technological adaptation in organizations? Specifically, do users' modifications accumulate over time in a gradual and continuous fashion, or do they occur in discontinuous spuns or episodes? Second, what organizational forces help to explain the pattern of adaptation observed over time? Our research finds that adaptation drops off dramatically after an initial burst of intensive activity. Organizadonal forces such as production pressure and team erosion appear to contribute to this rapid decline. We also find that this decline of adaptation is not irreversible, in that later, unexpected events can trigger new spuns of adaptive activity. These later episodes, however, are also of limited duration. This leads us to posit that the process of technological adaptation is highly discontinuous. Specifically, the initial introduction of technology as well as subsequent.

unexpected events--provide limited but valuable windows of opponuniry for expenmentation and adaptation. We argue that this discontinuous pattern has imponant implications for the theory and management of technological change. EXISTING LITERATURE The adaptation of technologies-in-use has been studied by several researchers. Their work demonstrates convincingly that it is only through experience with a new technology that users discover its ramifications. Rice and his colleagues argue that, in response to new discoveries, users often "reinvent" the technology and their procedures surrounding it, thus becoming pan of the innovation process (Rice and Rogers, 1980) and ultimately increasing their satisfaction with the new technology (Johnson and Rice, 1987). Funher research by Leonard-Banon (1988) shows that undertaking such modification is a complex, recursive process, involving "mutual adaptation" of both the new technology and the existing organization, and requiring the active cooperation of both users and technology developers. For the sake of brevity, we will use the term "technological adaptation" to refer to adjustments and changes following installation of a new technology in a given setting. In keeping with prior research, adaptations may address physical aspects of the technology, as well as users' procedures, assumptions, knowledge, or relationships. These changes may stem from users' efforts alone, or from joint efforts between users and technology developers. The Timing of Technological Adaptation Research on the process of technological adaptation has focused mainly on the shon period immediately following implementation. Thus, there has been little invesagadon of how adaptation activities vary over time. Even when authors have explicitly mapped changes over time (e.g.. Barley, 1986), they have not focused on identifying general trends in the timing of technological adaptation.

While little direct evidence exists on the timing of changes, the issue of adaptation is addressed in both the innovation and behavioral literatures. Yet these two bodies of research contain conflicting implications about the timing of technological adaptation. The innovation literature descnbes a relatively continuous pattern of technological adaptation over time, while research into the behavior of individuals, groups, and organizations suggests that the pattern of modifications is likely to be discontinuous or uneven. In the innovation field, research on expenence or learning effects in production (e.g., Conway and Schultz, 1959; Alchian, 1963) reveals regular productivity improvements over time in many industries. This has prompted theorists to suggest that such "progress can be thought of as a continuous process of adaptation" (Dutton and Thomas, 1984:244). However, these results are based on aggregate data that pool multiple technologies introduced at different times and used at different scales of operation. These studies, thus, do not reveal the timing of adaptation around a specific technology. Studies of industry evolution also treat the modification of technologies over time at an aggregate level. For example, Dosi (1982), Abemathy and Utterback (1978), and Tushman and Anderson (1986) posit long periods of continuous but gradual change in most technologies, fueled in part by existing users who encounter problems and respond with minor improvements. By contrast, only "radical" shifts in technology are seen as extraordinary and rare events (Abemathy and Clark, 1985; Tushman and Anderson, 1986). Another theme in the innovation literature is more prescriptive. This view suggests that, because many problems emerge only after a technology has been in use for a period of time, adaptive problem solving in user organizations should be gradual and persistent. Rogers (1983) states that when organizations try to rush the introduction process, they fail to identify and correct problems that later hamper productive use of the technology. Thus, "too-rapid implementation of the innovation... can lead to disastrous results" (Rogers, 1983:364). Similarly, Hughes (1971:152) maintains that "trying to force the pace" of adaptation is counterproductive, while Hage and Aiken (1970:106) suggest that "the longer the elite allow [the] period of tnal and error to

continue, the greater the chances of the new program achieving its intended objectives." Finally, Imai (1986) and Johnson and Rice (1987) argue that continuous adaptive efforts are needed to maximize the effectiveness of new technologies. While the picture of gradual and continuous technological adaptation offered by this research has gained considerable suppon, it is not compatible with widely-accepted results from behavioral research. In panicular, behavioral theories indicate that as organizations, groups, and individuals gain experience, they tend toward increasingly habitual modes of operation. Research at each level of analysis suggests that attention and effort are only occasionally or temporanly devoted to modification of routines. For example, a well-established concept in organizational theory is that organizational actors use expenence to create routines that simplify their informanonprocessing needs {March and Simon, 1958). Because such routines determine which environmental cues are noticed and the manner in which information about them is disseminated, increasing experience may lead organizational actors to overlook or ignore many problems or misfits between a technology and its setting (Kiesler and Sproull, 1982; Starbuck, 1989). Groups in organizations also develop tendencies toward routine behaviors. Over time, they become increasingly unlikely to recognize and respond to new kinds of problems (Kelley and Thibaut, 1954; Katz, 1982; Hackman, 1990). Even research teams have been shown to be reluctant to alter a given technical approach once it has been selected, and the longer the approach has been used, the greater their rigidity (Allen, 1966). At the individual level, research suggests that people's arousal, attention, and motivation to engage in effortful problem solving is not constant over time. Specifically, active problem solving and information processing appear to drop sharply as soon as tasks become familiar or manageable (Langer and Imber, 1979; Kruglanski and Freund, 1983). With increasing exposure, observers tend to "chunk" activities into larger units that convey less information than fine-grained observauons, although a sudden surprise can sometimes reverse the process (Newtson, 1973; Louis and Sutton, 1991). Familiarity also breeds rounnized response patterns; once activities are

well entrenched, even superficial resemblance to a known stimulus is sufficient to tngger a familiar response (Luchins, 1942). One of the few scholars to have considered the implications of these behavioral tendencies for technological adaptation is Weick (1990). Following Winner (1986), Weick (1990:21) suggests that "the point at which technology is introduced [may be) the point at which it is most susceptible to influence." Weick argues that "beginnings are of special imponance... because they constrain what is learned about the technology and how fast it is learned" (1990:21-22). However, he also hints that later change is not impossible, because interruptions in the regular use of a technology can increase arousal and thereby change the focus of users' attention. Taken together, these behavioral insights suggest that the attention and effon required to discover and respond to problems in the use of a given technology may be applied discontinuously over time, and not in the continuous way suggested by the innovation literature. This paper confronts these conflicting characterizanons of technological adaptation by examining the timing of adaptation activities in three organizations. The following section of the paper describes the study and research methodology employed. Next, the results of the research are discussed. The final section presents implications for a temporal theory of technological adaptation that takes into account both technological and behavioral aspects of the adaptation process. RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHODS Research Design The data for this study come from three research projects, undenaken by or with the authors, investigating the implementation and use of process technologies in production settings. Each of the three projects focused on multiple technologies within a single organization, and examined use and adaptations by groups or individual users. The studies were matched on four dimensions to ensure comparability (Leonard- B anon, 1990:253): (i) The technologies studied had passed the test of technical and organizational feasibility, hence failure of technological adaptation would not be due to either technical infeasibility or user rejection, (ii) The technologies studied

altered the work in some obvious although not radical ways, hence failure of technological adaptation would not be due to users being unaware of changes in their process technology, (iii) The technologies were open-ended in the sense that users (with or without assistance) had the means to make changes, hence failure of technological adaptation would not be attributable to an inability of users to manipulate their technologies, (iv) The focus of the research was consistent across the three studies, that is, all investigated new process technologies from the time of initial installadon of the technology until full and regular use was achieved. Given these similarities, an advantage of the research design was that it enabled us to examine adaptation of new technologies at both group and individual levels. In two of the research sites, the technologies studied were complex production systems whose implementation, use, and adaptation required group effort. Individuals either could not make changes independently to the technologies (due to technical complexity), or were prevented from doing so by work norms and procedures. other users. Since the technologies were shared, any changes made by an individual would affect In the third site, the technologies studied were stand-alone systems that were used and adapted by individuals. Any change made by one person did not affect others' use of the technologies. Funher, the technologies were designed to enable individuals to make changes without special technical skills or facilities. We deliberately sought vanety in the settings studied, the technologies introduced, and the type of users involved so as to enrich the range of insights and to enhance generalizability (Leonard-Barton, 1990; Van de Ven and Poole. 1990). The first study investigated the introducnon and adaptation of new capital equipment in eight European and U.S. factories of BBA,' a leading manufacturer of precision metal components. The second study examined the introduction and modification of computer-aided software engineenng tools in three U.S. offices of sec, a mulri-national software consulting firm engaged in the custom development of computer-based information systems. The third study investigated users' modification of usercustomizable software tools at Tech. a research university in the U.S. The technologies studied ' Names of all organizations have been disguised.

range from metal-shaping equipment to graphics software, and are used to produce physical products (in BBA), software (in SCO, and services (in Tech). Further, the studies encompass organizations with very different priorities and practices. At sec, where hours spent on software production translate directly into fees billed to clients, the dominant objective is the maximization of production for current revenues. FYiorities are more mixed at BBA, where factory personnel are directly responsible for identifying and implementing process improvements as well as for producing products. At Tech, innovation and novelty are central concerns, and many users regard these as more important than current output or productivity. Indeed the technology examined at Tech, user-customizable software, specifically allows adaptation by individuals during use. Many users at Tech have technical backgrounds, and several of those interviewed were involved in the initial development of the technology they were using. The three settings studied also span geographic locales (U.S. and Europe). This diversity reduces the nsk of our findings being merely an artifact of Amencan management practices, and increases the validity of our findings (Downs and Mohr, 1976; Van de Ven and Rogers, 1988). Research Methods The three research studies utilized mulnple data couecnon approaches. All three included indepth field research, ensunng that the concepts and patterns identified were grounded in the experiences and terminology of users (Click et al., 1990:302). Two of the studies were longitudinal, thus allowing for the situated and processual investigation of technological adaptation as it unfolded over time, without researchers or panicipants knowing the outcomes of the process being studied (Van de Ven and Rogers, 1988:640). The third study was retrospective and relied on project records and documentarion to reconstruct users' initial expectations and their activides over time. The methods used in the three studies are descnbed below and summanzed in Table 1 (see over).

Research Siie and Method ai BBA BBA is a European- based manufacturer of precision metal components, and a world leader in market share and product quality. The study was earned out in eight BBA-owned plants in three countries: Italy, Germany, and the U.S. Forty-eight projects involving the introduction and use of new process technology were studied. Due to missing data in seven cases, forty-one cases are included in the current sample. Projects were selected on three criteria: (i) they were undenaken dunng the last four years: (ii) each project represented an investment of $50,000 or more; and (iii) key project personnel were available for interviews. Process technologies included metal turning and precision machining equipment, assembly and inspection systems, thermal treatment and metal forming equipment, and handling systems. Projects were studied using three types of data. Description and experiences were obtained from retrospective, semi-structured interviews. Interviews lasted from one to four hours and occurred between zero and 18 months after project completion. One-on-one interviews were supplemented by multi-participant discussions where possible. Respondents included project managers, operating and technical personnel, and plant and division managers. Project activities and their dming were reponed on written questionnaires (see below). Participants were interviewed both before and after completing questionnaires to clarify their responses. In most cases, respondents made heavy use of project documentation in completing quesdonnaires. In addition, histoncal data were collected from company and plant documents. (For further details, see Tyre and Hauptman (1992).) Research Site and Method at SCC sec is a multi-national software consulting firm that builds customized software applications for client firms across various industries such as financial services, manufacturing, retail, and government. The software products produced by SCC typically consist of large transacaon-processing systems used to support major administrative activities such as order entry and customer service. SCC's operations are organized by project, with project teams varying from

ten to over a hundred people. Projects last from a few months to a number of years, and product budgets range from one hundred thousand to several million dollars. The research consisted of an in-depth field study conducted over eight months in three SCC offices located in the nonheast U.S. The focus of the study was the introduction and use of Computer-Aided Software Engineering (CASE) tools-a new process technology intended to automate the software production process at SCC. Five ongoing application projects (four large and one small) were selected for detailed analysis. The selection process ensured exposure to the introduction and use of the CASE technology in all major phases of the software production process (requirements analysis, conceptual design, detailed design, programming and testing). Data were collected via on-site observation of participants, unstructured and semi-structured interviews, review of project and software documentation, and informal social contact with the panicipants. One hundred and twenry-five interviews were conducted, each lasting an average of an hour and a half Participants spanned SCC's hierarchic levels from the most junior consultants and programmers, to senior project managers. Other key informants from within and outside of SCC included the director of research, sales directors, major client managers, and former SCC employees. (For funher details, see Orlikowski (1992).) Research Site and Method at Tech Tech is a research university in the northeast U.S. Its educational computing depanment provides a variety of technical and administrative computing services to the university. Members of this depanment rely heavily on computer software tools, such as electronic mail, graphics, spreadsheets, and word processing, to deliver services. Tech differs from the other sites in that individuals rather than teams perform adaptations, and individuals' modifications rarely affect others. Hence, there are fewer opponunities for conflicts to constrain adaptive behavior. Tech is thus an extreme case where ongoing adaptation may be most likely. The study focused on how various users (managers, secretaries, technical specialists, suppon stafo customized new versions of their software tools. The research was longitudinal and 10

included three primary data collection methods: questionnaires, interviews, and computergenerated records of customization activity. Each of the 51 users completed three questionnaires over a penod of four months, beginning shortly before installation of the new technology. Semistructured interviews with each user drew on data from the questionnau^es and computer-generated records to explore users' customization decisions, and the factors that facilitated or hindered customization (for critical incident technique, see Chapanis, 1969). (For funher details, see Mackay (1990).) Definition and Measurement of Adaptation Activities At BBA, adaptation activities were defined as actions intended to modify the new technology or relevant aspects oi the operating context (mcluding users' skills or procedures). Examples include debugging machine software, designing new tooling, training machine operators, or developing new maintenance procedures. Adaptation could be done through formal channels (such as engineering change orders) or through informal activities. Activities were considered pan of normal production when the new technology was used with no effort to alter the hardware, software, or related context and procedures. As pan of the wntten questionnaire at BBA, respondents rated the level of effon devoted to adaptation activities such as modification of machine software or change in factory procedures. Respondents also tilled out a project history in the form of a time-line, showing when activities were undertaken, and when unusual events (e.g., arrival of additional new equipment) took place. For each activity mentioned, respondents noted the level of adaptive activity during the penod (rated as high=3, medium=2, low=l, and not significant or none=0). Based on this information, the level of monthly adaptive activity in each project was computed as the sum of the scores for activities noted during that period. Respondents also used the time-line to note major project milestones, including date of equipment installation, date when new equipment was considered "production wonhy" (i.e., when it was producing parts on a consistent basis, even if cost, quality or other parameters were not yet satisfactory), and date when the new equipment was considered 11

fully integrated (i.e.. satisfactory efficiency and quality achieved, and operating parameters fully defined). During interviews, respondents also descnbed the specific problems they had dealt with, and when these problems were resolved. Other vanable measures for BBA (including summary statistics and correlation matrix) are shown in the appendix. At sec, project teams staned with a genenc skeleton of the CASE tools to be used in software production. Adaptations were defined as any action intended to correct, extend, or otherwise customize these tools to reflect the specific operating requirements of a panicular client context. Examples include the addition of batch routines, customization of input and output templates, and modification of file access paths. Normal production work was defined as the use of the CASE tools, with no modification, to generate software and documentation for clients. Textual accounts of adaptations descnbed or observed dunng the course of the research were capnired in detailed field notes. At Tech, adaptation activities compnsed users' individual customizanons of their softwarebased work environments. Customizations involve modifications to a panicular work environment that persist though future uses of the software (such as defining a new layout for the screen, or specifying a set of rules for automatically soning incoming elecaonic mail, or associating a series of commands with a given function key). Daily use of the software, even if it involved some new behaviors (e.g., trying a different combination of keystrokes) did not constitute adaptation unless changes were embedded into the work environment. Data on the occurrence of customizations over time were collected as described above. Method of Analysis Data analysis proceeded in four phases, the first three constituting within-study analyses, and the fourth consisting of a cross-study analysis. First, we searched for temporal patterns in the adaptation of new technologies at each site. Second, we examined the identified patterns for evidence of whether technological problems had been resolved or not. We were panicularly interested in instances where adaptation activity ceased before problems with the use of the new 12

technology were solved. Third, having identified patterns, we searched for evidence of underlying forces that would explain their occurrence in each site. This follows Eisenhardt's (1989) suggestion that observed relationships be validated by seeking reasons for them in the local context. Finally, we compared the patterns and organizational forces we had identified across the three sites and determined similanties and differences. Consistency of results was evident across multiple investigators using multiple data collection methods (Eisenhardt, 1989) and across two levels of analysis (Staw, Sandelands, and Dutton, 1981). Funher, findings from each site were corroborated by evidence from the others. This increases the likelihood that the patterns of adaptation identified are intrinsic to the process of technological adaptation, rather than consequences of a specific organizational approach or panicular type of technology. RESULTS In this section, we first present evidence of an initial, intensive episode of adaptation, and its rapid decline. Next, we discuss four organizational reasons for this decline of adaptation effon. Finally, we show that later episodes of adaptive activity do occur, but that they are also shon-lived. Timing of Technological Adaptation following Introduction A staking finding across the three research sites was that adaptation efforts appeared to fall off abrupdy after a shon initial introduction period. This initial period seemed to represent a finite window of opportunity during which users found it relatively easy to make changes to new technologies-in-use. Afterward, adaptation efforts dropped off, with users finding few opportunities to examine outstanding questions or to review initial choices. This pattern was echoed in each of the operating environments studied (see Table 2, over). Expenmentation was more likely to occur and significant changes more apt to be implemented immediately following introduction than at any later time, despite ongoing problems or additional insights that might be gained over time. For example, at SCC a large amount of adjustment and 13

i- E 5 " 5i ^ C "C Oa. a. *> Cl X E u <u * c X.- n C u ^ o O J2 5C a. o -T ^5 Q. P 5i in C 'J yt C 5 -s :; ^ 2 : = t ^ ^ =.. a ^ v» >- -3 5 ^ - 5 * >, = i > - 2 - u -3 ^ 1 - E O > 5 ii -^ - ^ ~ -3 o a a c Oi > 3 H (D on 71 c S : o u o Z 5 c o' '5 ^ I s 5 - - - o n - - -A ' " ^ -S o ^1-3 i > O Q o S CO ra ^1 c 2 5 "3 = c Z 2 3 o ^ o -' B = J = -g i2 5 5 o ^ VI so C ^ ^ o < - :^ S = 5 C. g c _2 ^ a 3 ^ -s s = - Q- ^ O ^ il E 5 5^ t^ O y H =0 c C 3 > If -3 U X : ^ O c i:! = T3 2-3 a o 3 C S o ^ n O r 'J c = ill! -3 J c > a t: i -3 so ^ u o a - j: > c i: " aj 5 W Si E 3 J o r; -J 3 -i^- C = T i» 'J s ^ 1 L 3-5 S " -> UJ 3-3.= s S = i- a a 'J 5! 5 " 5.< J u J5 o S ^ *j ** " ii ^ v> -3 ^ -3 3 " w E-5./I s 1 2 'J " u O J 2 o ^ ~. i«a * VI u «c?5 < en U ifl -3 «5. > -5 < 5 = a: o. > - m -5 o S c 3... u 14

modification took place directly following initial installation of CASE tools into a new project site to adapt them to the panicular client organization. These adjustments were accomplished by technical developers, many of whom had panicipated in the design and construction of the CASE tools. Following such initial adaptation of the tools, application programmers (who were responsible for the actual production of new application software) were brought onto the project. Once these programmers began using the CASE tools as process technology, they halted further changes to the tools, insisting that the process technology be stable and reliable to facilitate their producdon work. Thus, the programmers became frustrated when technical developers kept trying to perfect the tools. This frustration is evident in two representative comments by programmers: [The technical developers) do not want to release stuff until it is perfect. But we would rather they give us something to walk with [so we can begin production work], and then they can enhance it later to give us a racing car. But right now [at the beginning of the project] we need basic transponation. There is a tendency among our technical developers to spend too much time on technical wizardry, to come up with the perfect solution, the Rolls Royce. But what we really need is something more practical, something that will allow us to get our work done, like a Volkswagen. Once the technical developers had handed the tools over to the application programmers there was very little funher refinement of the tools. Only under extreme conditions (such as a breakdown in the CASE tools) were refinements tolerated and scheduled. Even when project members recognized the need for ongoing process modifications and incorporated that into their schedules, opportunities for change narrowed over time. This occurred at BBA despite increasing insight and experience among users and developers. For instance, in the case of a very innovative metal shaping machine, users and developers both acknowledged the need for adaptation based on accumulated shop-floor experience. The new equipment was installed in the factory under a development contract stating that machine concepts as well as tooling would be adapted further to fit emergent local requirements. But, once the equipment was installed and operatmg in the plant, users found that it became difficult to revisit basic decisions made during the development process. They complained that: We would get the development engineers in here for a meeting, but after a while it was like too many cooks -- we never got any action. We are done appeasing them. If they can prove the need, only then is acdon by us warranted. 15

Similarly at Tech, the level of customization activity fell off abniptiy soon after initial implementation of new software. In panicular, exploration or experimentation as a means of learning about the technology virtually ceased after the first few weeks of use. Instead, users quickly settled on a computing environment and actively worked to maintain its stability. One Tech member explained why she had stopped customizing after her initial efforts when she arrived at Tech four years ago: It's just the way I do it. I'm too lazy to change. It's not that it's hard, it's just that it's not worth the effon. Data from BBA provide funher detail on the dimensions of the window of opponunity provided by initial introduction of technology. Figure 1 shows the pattern of adaptation undenaken over time for 32 projects.^ To derive the curve, we calculated both the total level of adaptive activity reponed for a given project, and the percent of the total completed during the first, second, etc., months following installation. Calculations were based on month-by-month measures of adaptive activity from project timelines and written questionnaires. Results by month were then averaged across projects. They show that on average 28% of all adaptation undertaken in a project was completed during the first month following installation; an additional 16% was completed in the second month, and so on. Thus, an average of 54% of all adaptive activity was completed in the first 2.8 months, or only 12% of the average total time to full integration. The time period of this initial window was remarkably consistent across projects: despite the fact that the time to full integration varied widely, only four projects (10%) maintained their initial activity level for more than four months.^ Funher, there was no relationship between the size of the project (as measured by dollar investment) and the duration of the inidal episode of adaptation (r=.06, ns). A possible explanation for this pattern could be that adaptation activities decreased after a shon time because all (or most) problems had been idennfied and solved by then. This was not the 2 Nine projects were deleted from this calcuiauon because their total time to full integration was less than twelve months. While each of these projects reflected the pattern discussed here, including these shorter projects would have posiuveiy biased the results, since adaptation necessarily ceases early when the project is short. ^ The iruiiaj episode of adapiauon was detined as ending when the month-to-month change in the level of adaptation effort was negauve and greater than 50%. New episodes were defined as beginning when recorded adapiauon acuvity increased by more than 100%, or began again after a period of no such reported activity. 16

Figure 1: Monthly Adaptive Activity as a Percent of Total Adaptive Activities in BBA (N = 32 projects) Percent of all Adaptive Activity Completed during the Month 30%- 25%- 20%- 15%- 10%- 5%- 0% ^. Aa. ^^'^ Vx -A I I I I I I I I I I I I I I I > :^H^:-:-.^.-;4+>:-;-;-:- 1 3 5 7 9 11 13 15 17 19 21 23 25 27 29 31 33 35 Time in Months from Installation 17

case. On average, the new technologies took almost 14 months to be considered productionwonhy (i.e.. producing pans on a consistent basis), and they required another eight months to be fully integrated into the production process (all operating parameters satisfactorily defined). Funher. respondents' reponed an average of five problems still outstanding at the end of the first wave of intensive adaptation. Typically these were significant issues, such as software problems, undefined procedures or tooling, and malfunctions in automatic features. In several cases, respondents indicated that anention to problem solving fell off even though the machines remained inoperative. In sum, evidence from all three sites reveals that the introduction of a new technology opens a brief window of opportunity for an intense, initial episode of adaptation. Below, we examine evidence relating to the organizanonal forces that shape this pattern of adaptation. Four forces were consistently mentioned or observed in the three sites: (i) the pressure of production, (ii) the constraining effect of habitual patterns of use, (iii) the adjustment of expectations based on experience, and (iv) the erosion of team membership and enthusiasm over time. Organizational Forces Influencing Timing of Technological Adaptation i. Production Pressure Impedes Adaptation Data from all three studies suggest that one of the most powerful forces behind the failure of continuous modification was that, once technology was put into use, production activities quickly began to siphon off the dme, energy, and resources that were needed to identify and solve new problems. Thus, even when users wanted to conrinue to improve the technology and its applicanon, they felt they could not due to external pressures. (Table 3 shows the nature and incidence of this effect, and describes the measures used.) At sec, for example, both programmers and technical developers were acutely aware that making changes to the tools or experimenting with different technology opdons meant time away 18

2 5 5 ^ ^ S :; s o > CO - 'J w > 3- *=! a < 2 3 w i 1-2. i i-.5 «~ -3 ^ u c o e u 2 S Q. ^ 1' 3 e o * u3 a. c _ a p S a u f2 3 " o. 5 2 «C ^ n 3 U> 3 C U. 3 o. o C 3-3.Is 3 - C > 5 2 ^ :; ^ 3 T3. 2 3 = 5. 5 = * = ^ 5 2 i S 5 3-3 il O tl!? 3 J OO 3- Q..S a ^ u 1 * 3 C 3 c u O ^ i O o 5. 19

from producing application software. Since software was produced on-site to tight client specifications and time frames, SCC could not afford to let schedules slip. According to SCC managers, once software production begins: We push ourselves too hard. And the problem is thai as a result we don't have dme to leam how to do something new, or develop new tools. Such problems are consistent with SCC's intensive focus on shon-term productivity performance. More surprising is that similar patterns emerged at Tech. Despite users' stated preference for ongoing innovation and refinement, these same users were unlikely to adapt operational systems once they were in production mode unless forced to do so by external events. One user commented that making changes is something one does when one has "leisure time," while according to another typical user: [Customization] is the last thing on the queue. I feel guilty doing it. I feel that 1 should be doing something useful like testing an application [i.e., production work]. In pan, such comments reflect the conflict between the cenainty required by the producdon process, and the uncenainties involved in making changes to the technology. Users engaged in production perceived a significant risk that a seemingly straightforward adaptation would balloon into a major project. One user explained that due to work demands, "I can't afford to be a guinea pig." Funher, users recognized the potential to make a mistake that would cause greater problems than the one they were trying to fix. One Tech user commented on his prior experiences of adaptation that had resulted in major rework and therefore lost productivity: So I gave up [on customization].... There has to be a compelling reason for me to go back over that threshold. At BBA, an engineer at one of the German plants described the conflict between production and adaptation in the following terms: Once we got the equipment into the factory, time to do important engineering work was squeezed out by everyday work to keep things running. Some users expressed the conviction that, since near-term production requirements left them no time to pursue funher changes, extending the time frame for implementation would provide more opponunities for adaptation. However, our data suggest that this was not the case. As we discuss 20

below, we found that when users took a longer time to complete the mtroduction of the new technology, further barriers to adaptation often arose. a. Patterns of Use Congeal and Become Constraining Over Time Another bamer to ongoing change in all three operatmg settings was the fact that users quickly adapted themselves to their new process technologies. As users gained experience, they established stable routines, norms, and habits for using the technology which decreased the need for discussion, coordination, or effortful decision making. This constrained funher exploration and adaptation, apparently stunting the "learning" process that was expected by many managers. The constraining effects of increased experience were pronounced at BBA. For instance, in the case of one novel grinding machine, producnvity benefits were predicated on the integration of the new equipment into an existing automated processing line. However, initial integration problems forced project engineers to install a temporary manual "workaround." Although the manual workaround was inefficient, operators quickly learned to depend on it. Later, when the grinder was finally fully repaired, users clung to the system they had become accustomed to, and prevented engineers from dismantling the "temporary" workaround. Because of this, the new grinder's capabilities for efficient, high-precision machining were never fully developed and exploited. The same tendencies surfaced among software users at Tech. Once functions became habitual or automatic, users resisted further change. To illustrate, when new software versions were installed, users often simply retrofitted the new versions to mimic functions of the familiar, onginal version. For example, when a new screen management system was installed at Tech, 78% of the users found a way to maintain their existing patterns of working-either by retrofitting the new screen management system to resemble the old one (60%). or by modifying their stan-up procedures to invoke the old screen management system instead of the new one (18%). Users often hastened the process of making their use of the new technology habitual by "customizing themselves" to the software as they first received it. One manager at Tech noted. 21

(Many people] prepare personal cheat sheets, thus effectively customizing themselves rather than the software, for the uses of the software that they typically make. This manager pointed out that such an approach was cumbersome, and so it was not likely that users would change their "cheat sheets" frequently. Indeed, once a given approach had been learned, many users were very reluctant to change. One user explained that he had wanted to leam the manual commands before automating them via customizations. But, having learned the commands, he purposefully avoids automating his software because "now that I know things, I've learned the keyboard commands. I'm happier." He no longer sees any need to perform the customizations. Where production pressures were especially intense, the tendency for patterns of use to congeal was exaccerbated. In SCC. users chose tool repertoires that enabled them to meet production deadlines, and then quickly became dependent on those tools in their current form. They resisted ideas for improvements or adjustments that threatened to disrupt established ways of using the system. When such changes were occasionally introduced, users often tried to ignore them by bypassing the new versions to work with the original technology. A project manager noted that: We [were frustrated] dunng the spec stage, as the technical developers wanted continual changes to the tools... So we decided that we would just continue with [our version of the tools] so that we could get on with our schedules. Even if the new tools were potentially supenor, programmers admitted that "We often go around it [a new module of the tools]," because they were not willing to bear the cost of learning a new technique. The fact that new tools often contained system errors also induced users to stick with existing sets of CASE tools. A programmer explained: When things went wrong with the tools, we used to circumvent the tools left and right so that we could get on with our work. Table 4 shows evidence of this effect across the three sites and descnbes the measures used Ui. Expectations Adjust to Fit Experience In many of the projects studied, expectations regarding the performance capabilities of a new technology changed over time. Specifically, expectations were amended to fit actual capability 22

n o "^ 5 C/5 a c o o 30 30 B H Is V i a* u u o E 3 Z a. 5 so y5 > o a; 00 c o U «D o CO s 1/5 a 3 = 'i = 1 rz = O a Z - -2 =a c o " J^ -J ^ 5 _ ^ 2 2 n S 5. y. o ^ ^ - '^ -=^ 5o y 3 o!/l S 7J QU <*«o Q CO -= jz 2.-3 n ^ ^ ^ 3 u c "> W X 3 C o 00 3 = fl> >^ ^1 5l X - u ^ I ^ 3 1 a :2 3 u < i o c C 00 jo o 3 1 -S o 2 o 2 "3 ^ " I ^ o 2f u 3 S! : ^3^ < U in

or achievement. Therefore, as time went on, problems or opponunities often disappeared from view -- not because the technology was improved, but because standards were lowered or interpretations amended. (Table 5 shows the nature and incidence of this effect, and describes the measures used.) For example, one project at BBA involved the introduction of an advanced precision grinding machine. The onginal objective of the project, according to both development engineers and onginal project documentation, was to develop the capability to machine ajl five "faces" of a pamcularly complex metal pan. Indeed the plant manager had explained that "grinding all five faces was ihs. key objective in this project," more imponant than the productivity improvements expected from the machine. Developers had demonstrated five-face grinding in the lab, but they had not been able to test whether the machine would hold required tolerances under actual plant conditions. Therefore, the project team agreed to continue development in the factory. A development engineer was assigned to the plant to work on five-face gnnding. But as time wore on development was blocked by the very success of the project on other cntena. Within several months the new machine was operating at speeds up to six times those of the equipment it had replaced, even without the addition of five-face gnnding. Production personnel found they had sufficient slack to run complex pans through additional grinding machines to complete all five faces. Users soon reconstructed the original project objectives to fit this new reality. Several of those interviewed denied that five-face grinding had ever seriously been considered as a key project objective. As one engineer commented: We only tried doing all five faces on this machine as an experiment. It was son of an add-on that did not work. The supervisor in charge of the machine was even more adamant. When he was interviewed some 18 months after installation, he stated that: (The machine] is now doing exactly what we purchased it for -- we are getting the productivity improvements [that we wanted]. Users at Tech also displayed a variety of ways in which experience with a new technology affected their perception of potential problems and opp>onunities. For example, one user explained that he had tned to use a special feature of the software called Zephyr when his system was new, 24

c 9i e c ax CO CO e o a X u X c '> u in H

and that he had failed to get the feature working satisfactonly in his first two days of trying. Months later, he explained that he had no interest in trying Zephyr again: "Once burned off. I don't come back..." In another case, a manager noted that one of the software functions he used most had failed some time ago and was no longer available. In fact, the function had been repaired and was again available (other users were employing it at the time), but since this user's expectations had already adjusted, he had not thought to inquu-e whether the problem had been coirecied. iv. Erosion of Team Membership and Enthusiasm Another bamer to adaptation was that when projects bogged down, the relevant teams tended to dissolve and lose momentum. (Table 6 shows the nature and incidence of this effect, and descnbes the measures used.) For instance, one project at BBA involved the introduction of a novel thermal-forming approach for producing complex metal pans. The lead project engineer explained that: Our approach was to create a team consisting of a manufacturing engineer, a service technician and a skilled operator to put the machine into production. But the slow rate of production startup was a problem. Each time the machine went down, we had to disband the team and send the people to other activities while we waited for new pans or tools. We got the people back in when we received the new tools, then sent them out when the new tools broke. That really hampered our learning. And, you do not always get the team members back. We strove to keep the group together, but sometimes individual people became involved in other, more urgent projects that were not dragging on as much. Funhermore, as another project manager stated, it was difficult to keep the team focused: It's easy to get plant engineers to stan working on large projects, but it's extremely difficult to keep attention focused on the details over time. People tend to drift away to other problems when the work is only half done. On another BBA project, modifications to the technology-in-use stopped once the engineers returned to their norma] assignments. A project panicipant noted: For the first three months, we had a really intensive effort -- the engineers [assigned to the project] were 100% qu. But after that, well... Similarly at SCC, once projects reached a stage where the CASE technology had been installed and programmers began using the technology to do their production work, many of the technical developers requested assignment to other projects with "more interesting" work. One technical developer commented: 26

B H ki > o c o o H o u C *> 5 S H? i iaij g 27

I got transferred to another part of the project as all the creative work had been done, and we knew they [the tools] basically worked.... So I got involved in developing the front-end to [the product) which is much more interesting and challenging for me. Another issue was that technical developers were reassigned to production tasks on the project once the process technology was sufficiently stable. Their new task assignment effectively precluded further work on the process technology. For example: [The project manager) is pushing to disperse us [the technical developers) across the application teams to help with... code production. Such tendencies blocked the implementation of detailed process technology changes after the initial period of installation and adjustment. Timing of Subsequent Technological Adaptations The data presented above suggest that adaptation becomes increasingly difficult as process technologies become more thoroughly embedded and routinized in the user environment. Regular use of the technologies we studied was not consistent with the kind of mental and physical effort required to develop and implement new ideas. Yet, paradoxically, routine use was also necessary for ongoing adaptation; it provided the raw data that, if utilized, could lead to improvements in the technology or the way it was applied in the local context. In each of the sites studied there was evidence that users did, at least occasionally, reexamine existing technology and make important modifications after gaining additional expenence. At BBA, 31 of the 41 projects studied demonstrated a later spurt of adaptive activity. In four of these cases more than one later spun of activity was reponed. Later episodes of adaptive activity, like the initial episode, were of limited duration. At BBA these later spuns of activity lasted for an average of 2.4 months (similar to the initial period of 2.5 months, or 2.8 months for the 32 longer projects), and there was little vanation across projects (there were only two instances where the episode lasted longer than three months). Figure 2 shows the general pattern of adaptation observed at BBA..As shown, the second spurt of activity began, on average, about 1 1 months after initial installation, and accounted for an average of 23% of all reponed adaptive activities. 28

Figure 2: Episodes of Adaptation in BBA (Schematic graph showing average timing of adaptive activity) (N = 41 projects) Percent of all Adaptive Activity 30%_ 60% of adaptive actiznties y (19%; 20%_ 10%_ 23% of adaptive activities (18%) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 Time in Months from Installation Note: Standard Dcvianons in Parcniheses

In almost every case, the existence of a later spun of adaptive activity at BBA was associated with a specific, disruptive event in the project life cycle (see Table 7 for evidence and a descnption of the measures used). Most often, respondents reported that attention was refocused on the technology and its mode of use by the addition of new machines or tools to the same cell or line. In other cases, new project requirements or changed factory procedures forced panicipants to revisit decisions made earlier and to improve technical capabilities or procedures. In a few cases, a new episode of adaptation was induced by an unusual but not disruptive event, as in two cases where the arrival of new, unassigned technical personnel provided extra resources for handling outstanding problems with the process technology. Management action also triggered some new adaptive activity. This was generally linked to the amval of new managers at the company or local level. For example, one new machine was plagued with problems for more than two years because users were unable to reconfigure the technology on the shop floor. As the factory-level project leader explained: We wasted a huge amount of time... We would make some small adjustment but then, due to difficulties at a more basic level, something else would happen or a tool would break... The whole process accomplished very little until we were able to rethink some of the early choices and assumptions. Significantly, the opportunity to "rethink" early choices came about only once a new group of divisional managers took over and focused attention on the troubled project. In only one case did existing managers explicitly instigate funher adaptation effons. At Tech, users were generally reluctant to alter software systems once a serviceable configuration had been found. Yet most users (49 out of 51) did note that specific events could refocus their attention on the software and trigger funher customizations. As at BBA, triggers were often disruptive or aberrant events, such as the release of a new system or the breakdown of an existing one (see Table 8 on page 32). For example, in one case an experienced user was given a special assignment that required him to process greatly increased amounts of data in a very shon time. To cope with the resulting cnsis, he created a new set of program rules that automancaily soned, labeled, and routed his elecn-onic messages. Once the special assignment was completed, he discovered that these new rules significantly improved his effectiveness even under 30

a 3 < CQ CO c _o 2 a. ra < J= t: s2 c c 00 00 n c 3 r o a. O o O c c at 3 -Q 3 CD H

u H S o ;o 12* a < u to c: 4J c c o 3 je ^ -3 J 5 -J.-5 5 J 'J VI w - I 5 s 00 s c u 00 00 c if s s c o a. a. O o CO to 0 c E c oa. Q. O 5 00 o J3 so C UJ O Q. X UJ SO X O 5 u c so c o O SO o i 2 o -o c GO fs o c e u3 c u CQ 3 cn 00 9t a (A U «i C E.2 CNl (2

normal circumstances. In another instance, a user who normally did not travel went on an extended tnp. Upon returning, he was overwhelmed with accumulated electronic mail messages, and he quickly developed new rules to deal with the situation. He soon discovered that these modifications proved useful additions to his regular work rounne. In other cases at Tech, opportunities for change were created when normal workflow and thought patterns were interrupted by outsiders. For example, when a visitor asked whether the electronic mail system routed messages reliably, some non-technical users expressed surprise and concern that the technology might riqi work correctly. As a result of this interruption, they began to undenake new expenments with their technology. Sometimes the impetus for funher adaptation effon was internal. A.significant number of users modified their system when they thought of new ideas, or when old procedures simply became too frustrating. Disruptive events or new ideas, however, did not always mgger changes that advanced the technology..-xs noted earlier, the most common form of adaptation at Tech following new system releases was a retrofit that enabled users to continue to operate as if no change had occurred. At sec there was evidence of only one incident of technological adaptation during a later phase of a project. This is not surprising given that formal procedures explicitly dictated that the CASE tools be defined at the beginning of the project and then held stable. Even after projects were complete, there were few opportunities to revisit questions about the technology and its mode of use. A senior consultant commented that once projects were finished: We are never asked to reflect on the problems we've had... No one asks how are these tools used after their time so we can fine-tune the process or correct and eliminate the problems. Yet even ai SCC, a cnsis could create an opportunity to rethink earlier choices. In the one project where users did seek to modify the process technology later in the project cycle, they did so because the existing CASE tools had ceased to produce useful output. Many production requirements had changed over time, and the CASE tools in use no longer reflected the current requirements. Eventually, project management recognized that the tools had become inadequate to the task, and technical personnel were called in to help modify the existing technology. 33

DISCLSSION AND IMPLICATIONS FOR THEORY This research has confronted an apparent conflict in the literature over the timing of technological adaptation in organizations. While innovation research descnbes a gradual, continuous process of modification, behavioral theory indicates that the process n:iay be much more discontinuous. Our results reveal a pattern of adaptation that is distinctly discontinuous, or episodic, as illustrated in Figure 3. While full integration of a new technology may take several years, adaptation attention and effon are not applied consistently over that period, nor do they taper off gradually. Rather, they are concentrated in shon spuns during the period. This finding suggests that what appears, at an aggregate level, to be "continuous improvement" may more accurately be descnbed as the sum of discrete episodes of adaptive activity carried out at different times and applied to different technologies. We find that the initial episode of adaptation is especially imponant. The decisions and direcnons taken during a shon penod following initial installation--a period that may be as brief as two to three months-are major determinants of how the technology will be used by the organization over the longer term. Indeed, it appears that funher adaptation is rare unless some sort of unusual event or discovery (such as a breakdown in the technology, the entry of more new technology, a managerial intervention, or the culmination of users' own frustrations) triggers subsequent episodes of adaptive activity. We have called the initial period following installation a window of opponunity. It is a window in the sense that, for a time, users can view the new technology as a distinct artifact. Inidal expenences yield insights about the technology and its relationship to the context of use. Later, users' views arc obscured by integration of the technology into a complex production system, and by the habitual behaviors that sustain it. The initial period also represents a window because during this limited time users (often assisted by technical expens) can reach into the technology to change it. Once the new technology is assimilated into the larger production process, change threatens to disrupt the habits and procedures that suppon productive work. The production process and the 34

Figure 3: Relationship between Time and Users' Adaptation of Technology Cumulative Percent of all Adaptations Completed Time since Installation 35

specific technology used to support u congeal, and the window of opponunity is closed. Subsequent episodes of adaptanon occur only occasionally, and typically last only a shon time. While more work is needed to corroborate these findings, we suggest that the episodic pattern of adaptation observed in this study may be inherent in users' effons to deal with new technologies, and not just a function of a given managenal approach. Our results find support in existing research on the management of attention and sensemaking among individuals and groups in organizations. For example, our findings coincide with Weick's proposal that "beginnings are of special importance" in determining the way that users make sense of new technologies and the problems that anse (Weick, 1990:21). Weick points to Barley's (1986) work to suppon his argument, and indeed the metaphor of windows of opportunity may be helpful in explaining the divergent responses to CT technology that Barley observed in two hospitals. From our perspecnve, the window of opponunity immediately following installation of new technology was exploited at Suburban Hospital: within two months of receiving the CT, a new mode of operating and interacting had been established. At Urban Hospital, by contrast, there was little change within the first two months following installation. Although later adaptations occurred in both settings, the presence or absence of an initial episode of adaptation may have fueled Suburban's more dramatic recontlguranon, and made change at Urban slower and less significant. At a more general level, this study addresses the question of how organizational actors allocate attention to regular production work as opposed to the development and improvement of technologies and procedures. Our study contributes to this issue in several ways. First, it suppons previous findings that the assumptions, behavior patterns, and practices governing the execution of production tasks are defined early and congeal quickly. This pattern has been detected among organizations (Hedberg, Nystrom and Starbuck, 1976), groups (Gersick, 1988; Bettenhausen and Murninghan, 1985) and individuals (Luchins, 1942; Langer and Imber, 1979). Second, our study highlights the importance of interruptions in the form of problems or surprises. Interrupaons create opponunines for organizational actors to mobilize their accumulated expenence in order to redefine their task, their approach, or the tools applied. A number of researchers have 36

pointed to the power of such interruptions. For example, Gersick finds that groups working under deadlines often radically revise their approach when they have used up half their time, because "the midpoint appears to work like an alarm clock" (1988: 34) that interrupts the routine flow of work and encourages reexamination of the process. By contrast, groups that do not have such a built-in alarm can get stuck in unproductive work patterns; it is more difficult for them to tum their neganve experiences into usable insights (Hackman, 1990). Bettenhausen and Muminghan (1985) find that when group members threaten or challenge group norms, they create an opponunity to consider new altemanves and to build a new, stronger consensus. At the individual level, unexpected events have been shown to occasion revision of habits and assumptions (Langer and Piper, 1987; Pyszczynski and Greenberg, 1981; Louis and Sutton, 1991). Thus. It appears that interruptions can serve an imponant role by cnggenng actors to review and revise their procedures or processes. Nonetheless, an interruption provides only a window of opportunity; the opportunity must also be exploited. We noted that when discrepant events were evaluated from a production-oriented perspective, they often appeared to be useless disruptions which users strove to ignore. Research at the individual (Luchins, 1942; Langer, 1983; Langer and Piper, 1987) and organizational (Dutton and Jackson, 1987; Meyer, 1982; Weick, 1990) levels shows that how an unexpected event is framed or introduced helps to determine whether it is interpreted within existing routines or used to create new ways of understanding. Likewise, managers may be more likely to encourage adaptation when they frame interrupnons and surpnses as noteworthy and potentially informative. Unfonunately, managers seldom tiike advantage of this fact. In our study, for example, there were few instances where managers actively intervened to tum unusual events into opponunities for change. It is important to note that our research was carried out exclusively in Western organizations, which may manage adaptation differently from other (e.g., Asian) organizations. Therefore, it is interesting that our findings are consistent with descriptions of several "bestpractice" Japanese approaches. For example, in a study of production practices at Toyota, Hall (1983: 199) notes that when a new machine or process is first installed, the factory makes "a direct 37

engineenng assault... [that prevents the need to dribble a constant stream of engineenng changes through the formal system over a long time." Funher, Clark and Fujimoto (1991: 189) found that in Japanese automobile companies, "pilot runs are relatively shon, and the pilot run periods are compressed." Similarly, Ogawa's (1991) study of a leading Japanese steel company points out that the test period should be seen as a limited opponunity to surface all major problems with new technology, since incremental changes can be hard to implement later. These authors report that in the Japanese companies they studied, adaptation during normal production is carefully controlled to stay within prescnbed limits. Most of the time, the new process is run in a relatively stable fashion. Modifications are "lumped" into special periods marked by plant shutdowns, model changeovers, or the imposition of new operating standards (Hall. 1983; Imai, 1986; Clark and Fujimoto, 1991). Thus, conflict between production and adaptanon objectives is explicitly managed and even exploited -- it is not ignored or obscured. A Final Note Our findings on the pattern of technological adaptation are remarkably consistent across both groups and individuals, and across different organizational and technological settings. An intriguing possibility is that these findings reflect relatively pervasive aspects of human behavior. Indeed, several examples from outside of the studies discussed above suggest that the same patterns of behavior occur even in informal task settings without sophisticated process technologies. These examples suggest that, in the immediate aftermath of a major change or disruption, participants are often able and willing to revise, adapt, and crirically evaluate their new situation. The lack of an established routine, combined with the necessary (and necessarily temporary) suspension of normal pertormance demands, appears to open the way for a period of expenmentation, reflection, and modification. However, as pent-up performance demands resurface, and as modifications succeed in enabling participants to turn their attention to these demands, the openness to further adaptation diminishes rapidly. 38

A simple anecdote illustrates this point. During the drafting of this paper, both authors coincidenially moved households. At the sian of previous moves, both authors had made solemn resolutions to be better organized at home. Yet when they began packing this time, each discovered that any box that had not been unpacked within approximately two weeks following the previous move had remained untouched. It had simply become pan of the landscape, or been lost in the rubble of a back closet, or had become a constant but low-level irritation that was never severe enough to act upon. Consequently, this time both authors have resolved to attack the problem of unpacking and organizing immediately following installation in their respective new residences. Even when the technology is as simple as boxes of books in a room, we have found that patterns of behavior congeal all too rapidly. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The authors are grateful for helpful comments from numerous colleagues. Constructive suggestions from the senior editor and three anonymous reviewers were particularly imponant in refining our arguments. The research suppon of the MIT Center for Information Systems Research, the MIT Leaders for Manufacturing Program, and the Harvard Business School Division of Research is gratefully acknowledged. 39

REFERENCES Abernathy, William J. and Utterback, James (1978) "Patterns of Industnal Innovation in Industry," Technology Review. June-July: 40-47. Abernathy, William J. and Clark, K.B. (1985), "Innovation: Mapping the Winds of Creative Destruction," Research Policy. 14, 3-22. Alchian,.Anmen (1963) "Reliabilitv ot Progress Curves in Airframe Production," Econometrica. 31, 679-693. Alien. Thomas J. (1966) "Studies of the Problem-Solving Process in Engineenng Design," IEEE Transactions on Engineenng Management, Vol. EM- 13, 2, (June), 72-83. Barley, Stephen (1986) "Technology as an Occasion for Structuring: Evidence from Observation of CT Scanners and the Social Order of Radiology Departments," Administrative Science Ouanerlv. 31:78-108. Bettenhausen, Kenneth and Murninghan, J. Keith (1985) "The Emergence of Norms in Compenave Decision-Making Groups," Administrative Science Ouanerlv. 30, 350-372. Chapanis, A. (1969) Research Techniques in Human Engineenng Baltimore: John Hopkins Press. Clark. K.B. and Fujimoto, T. (1991) Product Development Performance. Boston. MA: Harvard Business School Press. Conway, R.W. and Schultz, A. (1959) "The Manufacturing Progress Function," Journal of Industrial Engineenng. 10, 39-54. Dosi, Giovanni (1982) "Technological Paradigms and Technological Trajectories: A Suggested Interpretanon of the Determinants and Directions of Technical Change," Research Policy. 1 1, 147-162. Downs, George W. and Mohr, Lawrence B. (1976) "Conceptual Issues in the Study of Innovation," Administrative Science Ouanerlv. 21, 7(X)-714. Dutton, Jane E. and Jackson, Susan E. (1987) "Categorizing Strategic Issues: Links to Organizadonal Action," Academy of Management Review. 12, 1, 76-90. Dutton, John M. and Thomas, Annie (1984) "Treating Progress Functions as Opponunity," American Management Review. 9, 2, 235-247. a Managerial (1985) "Relating Technological Change and Learning by Doing," in Richard S. Rosenbloom (Ed.), Research on Technological Innovation. Management and Policy. 2, 187-224. Kathleen (1989) "Building Theories from Case Study Research," Academy of Eisenhardt, Management Review. 14, 4, 532-550. Enos. J.L. (1958) "A Measure of the Rate of Technological Progress in Petroleum Refining," Journal of Industrial Economics. 6, 180-197. Gersick. Connie J.G. (1988) "Time and Transition in Work Teams: Toward a New Model of Group Development," Academy of Management Journal. 31,1, 9-31. 40

(1991) "Revolutionary Change Theones: A Multilevel Exploration of the Punctuated EQuilibrium Paradigm." Academv of Management Review. 16, 1, 10-36. Click. William H., Ruber, George P., Miller, C. Chet, Doty, D. Harold, and Sutcliffe, Kathleen M. (1990) "Studying Changes in Organizational Design and Effectiveness: Retrospective Event Histones and Penodic Assessments," Organization Science. 1, 3, 293-312. Hackman, J. Richard (1990) Groups that Work (and Those That Don't). San Francisco. CA: Jossey-Bass. Hage, Jerald and Aiken, Michael (1970) Social Change in Random House. Complex Organizations New York: Hall. Roben W. (1983) Zero Inventories. Homewood, IL; Dow Jones-Irwin. Hedberg, Bo, Paul Nystrom and William Starbuck (1976) "Camping on Seesaws: Prescriptions for a Self-Designing Organization." Administrative Science Ouanerly. 21, 41-65. Hollander, S. (1965) The Sources of Increased Efficiency. Cambridge: MIT Press. Hughes, William R. (1971) "Scale Frontiers in Electnc Power" in William M. Capron, (Ed.) Technological Change in Regulated Indusines. Brookings Institute, Washington D.C. Imai,.Masaaki ( 1986) Kaizen: The Kev to Japan's Competitive Success. New Hill Publishing Company. York: McGraw- Johnson, Bonnie M. and Rice, Ronald E. (1987) Managing Organizational Innovation: The Evolution from Word Processing to Office Information Svstems New York: Columbia University Press. Katz, R. (1982) "The Effects of Group Longevity on Project Communication and Performance," Administrative Science Ouanerly. 27, 81-104. Kelley. Harold H. and Thibaut. John, W. (1954), The Handbook of Social Psychology. Reading, MA:.Addison-Wesley Publishing Company. Kiesler, Sara and SprouU, Lee (1982) "Managerial Response to Changing Environments: Perspectives on Problem Sensing from Social Cognition," Administrative Science Ouanerly. 27, 548-570. Kruglanski. A.W. and Freund. T. (1983) "The Freezing and Unfreezing of Lay Interferences: Effects on Impressional Primacy, Ethnic Stereotyping, and Numencal Anchonng," Journal of Expenmental Social Psychology 19, 448-468. Langer, EUen J. (1983) The Psychology of Control Bevedy Hills, CA: Sage Press. and Imber, L. (1979) "When Practice Makes Imperfect: The Debilitating Effects of Overleaming," Journal of Personality and Social P sychology. 37, 2014-2025. and Piper, Alison 1. ( 1987) "The Prevention of Mindlessness," Toumal of Personality and Social Psychology. 53, 2, 280-287. 41

Leonard-Banon, Dorothy A. (1988) "Implementation as Mutual Adaptation of Technology and Organization," Research Policy. 17, 251-267. (1990) "A Dual Methodology for Case Studies: Synergistic Use of a Longitudinal Single Site with Replicated Multiple Sites," Organization Science. 1, 3, 248-266. Louis, Meryl Reis and Sutton, Robert I. (1991) "Switching Cognitive Gears: From Habits of Mind to Acdve Thinking," Human Relations. 44, 1, 55-76. Luchins, A.S. (1942) "Mechanization in Problem Solving: The Effect of Einstellung," Psychological Monographs. 54 (6, whole No. 246). Mackay, Wendy (1990) "Users and Customizable Software: A Co-adaptive Phenomenon," Unpublished Ph.D. Thesis, Cambndge.MA: Sloan School of Management, Massachusetts Institute of Technology. March, James G. and Simon, Herbert ( 1958) Organizations, New York: John Wiley and Sons. Meyer, A.D. (1982) "Adapting to Environmental Jolts," Administrative Science Ouanerlv. 27, 551-537. Milne A.A. (1926) Winnie-The-Pooh. New York: Dell Publishing Co. Newtson, Darren (1973) "Attnbution and the Unit of Perception of Ongoing Behavior," of Personality and Social Psychology. 28, 1, 28-38. Journal Ogawa, Hiroyuki (1991) Information Flow and Learning in New Process Development: Construction Project in the Steel Industry. Unpublished thesis, Cambridge, MA: MIT Sloan School of Management. Orlikowski, Wanda J. (1992) "The Duality of Technology: Rethinking the Concept of Technology in Organizations," Organization Science. 3, 3, 398-427. Pyszczynski, Tom and Greenberg, Jeff (1981) "Role of Disconfirmed Expectations in the Instigation of Attnbutional Processing," Joumal of Personality and Social Psychology. 40, 31-38. Rice, Ronald E. and Rogers, Everett M. (1980) "Reinvention in the Innovation Process," Knowledge. 1(4). 499-514. Rogers, Everett M. (1983) Diffusion of Innovations. (3rd. ed.). New York: The Free Press. Rosenberg, Nathan (1982) Inside the Black Box. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press. Staw, B. Sandclands, L.E and Dutton, J. E. (1981) "Threat-ngidity Effect in Organizational Behavior A Multi-level Analysis," Administrative Science Ouanerlv. 26, 501-524. Starbuck, W.H. (1989) "Why Organizations Run into Crises... and Sometimes Survive Them," in Information Technology and Management Strategy. K. C. Laudon and Jon Turner, (eds.), Englewood Cliffs, N.J., Prentice Hall. Tushman, Michael L. and Anderson, P. (1986), "Technological Discontinuities and Organizational Environments," Administrative Science Ouanerlv. 31, 439-465. 42

Tyre, Marcie J. and Hauptman, Oscar (1992) "Effectiveness of Organizational Response Mechanisms to Technological Change in the Production Process," Organization Science 3 3 301-320. Van de Ven. Andrew H. (1986) "Central Problems in the Management of Innovation," Managemen t Science. 32. 590-607. and Poole, Marshall Scott (1990) "Methods for studying Innovation Development in the Minnesota Innovanon Research Program," Or ganization Science 1,3, 313-335. and Rogers, Everett M. (1988) "Innovations and Organizations: Critical Perspectives," Communic ation Research. 15, 5, 632-651. von Hippel, Eric (1988) The Sources of Innovation. New York: Oxford University Press. Weick, Karl (1990) "Technology as Equivoque," in Goodman, P.S., SprouU. L.S., and Associates. Technology and Oganizations San Francisco: Jossey-Bass: 1-44. Wilkinson, Leland (1989) SYSTAT: The System for Statistics Evanston, IL: Systat Inc. Winner, Langdon ( 1986) The Whale and the Reactor: A Search for Limits in an Age of Hig h Technolog y Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 43

D (/5 < P3 Art («X Q u 2 < c > c Si. 'o '3«Si 3

5939 047

Date Due OEC. 5 Lib-26-67

MIT LIBRARIES l "HIWllP l!!lf W 3 TOaO 00fi39M52 4