EFFECTS OF CONTEXTUAL INFORMATION ON BEHAVIOR OF CAL1llRIS SANDPIPERS FOLLOWING ALARM CALLS

Similar documents
Recent work on the development of feeding skills by young birds has

2008 San Francisco Bay Shorebird Census

Effects of human activity on the foraging behavior of sanderlings Calidris alba

Identifying Winter Sandpipers. Audubon Coastal Bird Survey Training Webinar 29 Jan 2013 Erik I. Johnson

Shorebird Identification

A Rising Tide: Conserving Shorebirds and Shorebird Habitat within the Columbia River Estuary

SHOREBIRDS! Brief Background. World Travelers 11/6/2016

HERON AND EGRET MONITORING RESULTS AT WEST MARIN ISLAND: 2003 NESTING SEASON

GENERAL PROTOCOL CONTENTS

THE SHOREBIRDS OF MONTEZUMA NATIONAL WILDLIFE REFUGE

Differential Timing of Spring Migration between Sex and Age Classes of Yellow-rumped Warblers (Setophaga coronata) in Central Alberta,

AERIAL SURVEY OF BIRDS AT MONO LAKE ON AUGUST 24, 1973

OLD NESTS AS CUES FOR NEST-SITE SELECTION: AN EXPERIMENTAL TEST WITH RED-WINGED BLACKBIRDS

Peregrine Falcon Falco peregrinus

OCCURRENCE IN A SAN FRANCISCO BAY MARSH

THE SPRING MIGRATION OF THE OVER EUROPE.

Fallow Field Shorebird Survey Training

Lasiurus blossevillii (Red Bat)

High Priority Shorebirds 2004

EEB 4260 Ornithology. Lecture Notes: Migration

A Guide To Birding Lytchett Bay

Greenlaw Mountain Hawk Watch Fall 2012

Abstract The American Redstart is a wood warbler that is in population decline in northern Michigan.

THE BREEDING STATUS OF THE GLOSSY IBIS IN NEW YORK

Golden Eagle (Aquila chrysaetos)

BREEDING SEASON DIET OF SHORT-EARED OWLS IN MASSACHUSETTS

I n fall 1942, a young biologist named BIRDS IN THE ARBORETUM REVISITED T E X T A N D P H O T O S B Y L A R R Y H U B B E L L

Greenlaw Mountain Hawk Watch Fall 2014

AUTUMN HAWK MIGRATIONS AT FORT JOHNSON, CHARLESTON, S.C.

Miki Taoka*, Tetsu Sato*1, Tsutomu Kamada*2 and Hiroshi Okumura*3

May 25, 2005 Forum Meeting

RAPTOR STUDY. raptor charts and posters

A volunteer-based program for the study of international migrations of shorebirds in the Western Hemisphere

ANIMAL COMMUNICATION and INFORMATION TRANSFER

THE MERSEY GATEWAY PROJECT (MERSEY GATEWAY BRIDGE) AVIAN ECOLOGY SUMMARY PROOF OF EVIDENCE OF. Paul Oldfield

Appendix A from S. Lingle and T. Riede, Deer Mothers Are Sensitive to Infant Distress Vocalizations of Diverse Mammalian Species

LOUISIANA BIRD RECORDS COMMITTEE

132 THE WILSON BULLETIN. Vol. 95, No. 1, March 1983

ASPECTS OF THE WINTERING ECOLOGY OF PIPING PLOVERS IN COASTAL ALABAMA

Investigating the effect of differential elevation of food on foraging behavior of the Eastern grey squirrel (Sciurus carolinensis)

Greenlaw Mountain Hawk Watch Fall 2011

How do you identify shorebirds?

California Gull Breeding Surveys and Hazing Project, 2011.

Species of Greatest Conservation Need Priority Species for NYC Audubon. May 12, Susan Elbin Director of Conservation and Science

Waterbird Nesting Ecology and Management in San Francisco Bay

Shorebird use of the Giacomini Wetlands Restoration area: 2011 Update

44. MARINE WILDLIFE Introduction Results and Discussion. Marine Wildlife Cook Inlet

PREDICTING AND MANAGING CLIMATE CHANGE: IMPACTS ON GREAT BASIN WETLANDS, SHOREBIRDS, AND THEIR PREY

THE ABUNDANCE AND MIGRATION OF SHOREBIRDS AT TWO PUGET SOUND ESTUARIES

Project Summary. Predicting waterbird nest distributions on the Yukon-Kuskokwim Delta of Alaska

ANIMAL COMMUNICATION. What is it??

Alvord Lake BCS number: 48-2

SHOREBIRD MIGRATION AT NORMAN, OKLAHOMA:

Distance from protective tree cover affects foraging times of urbanized gray squirrels at the University of Maryland Abstract

Migrational Survey and Habitat Usage of Shorebirds in the Lake Erie Marsh Region PROGRESS REPORT-2008 BSBO-08-3

Introduction. Previous and Ongoing Studies on This Issue

A Guide To Birding Lytchett Bay

WindWise Education. 2 nd. T ransforming the Energy of Wind into Powerful Minds. editi. A Curriculum for Grades 6 12

REINTRODUCTION TECHNIQUES FOR REHABILITATED AND CAPTIVE BRED BIRDS OF PREY

Teacher. Description By competing in math/science and physical activities, student will learn that shorebirds have incredible physical abilities.

OVERVIEW INTRODUCTION TO SHOREBIRDS MANAGEMENT FOR SHOREBIRDS TVA REGIONAL SHOREBIRD PROJECT ESTIMATING SHOREBIRD NUMBERS

What is a Bird of Prey?

The Effect of Feeder Hotspots on the Predictability and Home Range Use of a Small Bird in Winter

Perceived depth is enhanced with parallax scanning

NON-MIGRATORY MOVEMENTS OF DUNLINS ON THEIR CALIFORNIA WINTERING GROUNDS

Nova Scotia Christmas Bird Count 2014

What is Migration? CMS COP12 Regional Preparatory Workshop for Asia. [Tim Dodman] [What is migration?] August 2017 Bonn, Germany

Snake River Float Project Summary of Observations 2013

The design of Ruthroff broadband voltage transformers M. Ehrenfried G8JNJ

Roberts Bank Terminal 2 Project Field Studies Information Sheet

Seasonal changes in the response of oystercatchers Haematopus ostralegus to human disturbance

THE EFFECTS OF DISTANCE AND ISOLATION ON SONG-TYPE SHARING IN THE CAROLINA WREN

The Effects of Climate Change on the Breeding Behavior and Migration Patterns of Birds and Mammals. Dr. Susan Longest Colorado Mesa University

ENR 2360: Ecology and Conservation of Birds

Listed Birds along the Stony Brook Corridor Impacted by BMS Zoning Change

UC Davis Recent Work. Title. Permalink. Author. Publication Date. Impacts of highway construction and traffic on a wetland bird community

BTO Research Report No. 145

Apparent age - segregation of Dunlin within Bolinas Lagoon - a preliminary study

2010 Ornithology (B/C) - Training Handout

Increase of the California Gull Population in the San Francisco Bay and the Impacts on Western Snowy Plovers

LOUISIANA BIRD RECORDS COMMITTEE

MUD, BIRDS and POPPYCOCK*

Kingston Field Naturalists

APPENDIX 11.2 BRENT GEESE SURVEY REPORT

Shorebird Identification. Jason Hoeksema

IMPORTANCE OF BEACH, MUDFLAT AND MARSH HABITATS TO MIGRANT SHOREBIRDS ON DELAWARE BAY

Lecture 6 Behavior ethology Communication

An Ecological Risk Assessment of Wind Energy Development in Montana

Disturbance and feeding shorebirds on the Exe estuary

Charette Vision #1 for 2050

SIERRA NEVADA ADAPTIVE MANAGEMENT PLAN

CONTACTING US When ing, please use the following subject line BIOD48. s that do not include this subject line may not be answered.

EMERGENCE OF COMMUNICATION IN TEAMS OF EMBODIED AND SITUATED AGENTS

British Birds of Prey. British Birds of Prey Published on LoveTheGarden.com (

COMPARATIVE FEEDING BEHAVIOR OF IMMATURE AND ADULT HERRING GULLS NICOLAAS A. M. VERBEEK

Migrational Survey and Habitat Usage of Shorebirds in the Lake Erie Marsh Region, 2010 PROGRESS REPORT-2010 BSBO-10-3 INTRODUCTION

Site Selection by Migratory Shorebirds in Oregon Estuaries Over Broad and Fine Spatial Scales

ANTI-PREDATORY BEHAVIOR OF LAPWINGS: FIELD EVIDENCE OF DISCRIMINATIVE ABILITIES

Figure 1. Jamaica Bay Wildlife Refuge Shorebird Survey Areas. (Adapted from Davis, 1982) THE KINGBIRD

I N T H E F L Y W A Y A GS NORCAL COUNCIL S OWN INTEREST PROJECT PROGRAM

Transcription:

Wilson Bull., 94(3), 1982, pp. 322-328 EFFECTS OF CONTEXTUAL INFORMATION ON BEHAVIOR OF CAL1llRIS SANDPIPERS FOLLOWING ALARM CALLS DANIEL W. LEGER AND JAMI L. NELSON Two classes of individuals-signalers and recipients--exist in all animal communication systems (Wilson 1975, Smith 1977). Signals make information available and recipients use this information when choosing a response (e.g., Leger and Owings 1978; Seyfarth et al. 1980a, b). Contextual information, which exists outside signals, is also usually available to recipients and may be appraised by them when selecting a response (Smith 1965, 1977). Consequently, to fully understand the responses of recipients we must identify contextual information in addition to signal-conveyed information. Vertebrate alarm signals (signals emitted in the presence of potential predators) may be multi-functional and appear to vary interspecifically in information content. Some, such as the hawk alarm calls of small passerines, may only inform others that a raptor has been detected, but not inform recipients of the hawk s or the caller s locations (Charnov and Krebs 1975). In contrast, the alarm calls of other species, including vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops) and California ground squirrels (Spermophilus beecheyi), provide extensive information about the type of predator, the location of the caller and even the callers age and sex (Seyfarth et al. 1980a, b; Leger et al. 1980; Owings and Leger 1980). Regardless of the type and amount of information contained in alarm signals, it would seem beneficial for recipients to appraise as much contextual information as possible, because of the serious danger posed by predators. An important form of contextual information for alarm call recipients is the individual s vulnerability at the time the signal is detected. In fact, recipients of alarm calls do behave differently when vulnerable than when relatively safe (e.g., beaver [Castor canadensis] [Hodgdon and Larson 19731, vervet monkeys [Seyfarth et al. 1980a, b], California ground squirrels [Leger et al. 19791, hoary marmots [Marmota culigutul [Noyes and Holmes 19791). Shorebirds feeding on mudflats adjacent to marshy areas are exposed to attacks by raptors which may use marsh vegetation for concealment during their approach (Rudebeck 1950, 1951; Hunt et al. 1975; Page and Whitacre 1975; Dekker 1980). When they detect predators, shorebirds utter loud calls that usually elicit immediate flock formation and synchronous, erratic flight (Owens and Goss-Custard 1976). False alarms, i.e., calls occurring in the apparent absence of predators, are also fairly com- 322

Leger and Nelson * SANDPIPER ALARM CALLS 323 mon in shorebirds (Leger, pers. obs.). If most attacks by raptors come from the direction of the marsh vegetation, and if the vision of shorebirds is partly occluded by the vegetation, we would expect that individuals closest to the vegetation would be most vulnerable to attack. Thus, individuals hearing alarms while near the marsh should take immediate anti-predator action. Shorebirds farther from the marsh might be able to look around for the predator, and, not finding one, resume foraging. This study was conducted to determine whether shorebirds vary their responses to alarm calls as a function of their distance from the predatorconcealing marsh. METHOD Subjects and study site.-response to alarm call playbacks was studied in two species of wintering shorebirds, the Western Sandpiper (Calidris mauri) and the Dunlin (C. a&a). As the tide recedes, these and other shorebird species assemble to feed on mudflats adjacent to the marsh (Recher 1966). They risk predation by Northern Harriers (Circus cyaneus) and Kestrels (F&o sparuerius), both of which often hunt shorebirds by flying fast and low over the marsh, then suddenly bursting out over the mud to catch their prey by surprise. This experiment took place in February 1980 along San Francisco Bay at the Baylands Nature Area, in Palto Alto, San Mateo Co., California. The Baylands has a wooden walkway, slightly elevated over the marsh vegetation. At some points the walkway projects out over the mudflat, permitting views of the marsh/mud interface. The specific study site was a section of mudflat with a nearly straight line of marsh vegetation abutting it. Shorebirds at this site seem unresponsive to stationary humans. Equipment and procedure.-two months before the experiment, Leger recorded ca. 1.5 set of alarm calls given by American Avocets (Recumirostra americana) in response to a harrier flying nearby. The recording was made on a Uher 4400 recorder with Uher microphone at 19 cm/set tape speed. Field observations indicated that such calls usually evoked generalized escape reactions in all shorebirds. In the morning, before the receding tide had exposed any mud, playback equipment was set up on a section of boardwalk that was elevated about 2.5 m above the mud surface and about 20 m from the marsh vegetation. The playback speaker was oriented from the mudflat toward the marsh. As the water receded, shorebirds began assembling on elevated areas of marsh. As soon as some mud was exposed birds began foraging there. When a 2-3-m strip of mud adjacent to the vegetation was exposed, we began playbacks of either avocet calls (N = 10) or a comparable segment of blank audiotape (designated as no-sound ) as a control for the movements associated with equipment operation (N = 7). Playbacks used the Uher 4400 recorder and an Electrosonics Voice-Projector amplifier with its integral speaker. Playback volume was adjusted ip advance so that it approximated (by ear) that of naturally occurring avocet calls. Immediately before each playback, the area was photographed with a tripod-mounted 35-mm camera (Olympus OMl) equipped with a loo-mm lens and slide film. Immediately after taking the photograph and advancing the film, a playback occurred. Within 1 set following the end of a playback, a second photograph was taken of the same area. Alarm and no-sound playbacks were alternated with at least 5 min between playbacks. Also, at least 5 min elapsed following naturally occurring alarms of any species, and trials were aborted if an alarm call occurred between the first photograph and the playback. Because many shorebirds followed the receding tide, only a few playbacks could be done

324 THE WILSON BULLETIN * Vol. 94, No. 3, September 1982 2 3 4 FIG. 1. Outline map of the study site as viewed through the camera. The white area is mudflat and the darker area is marsh vegetation. The parallel lines drawn through the mudflat are ca. 0.5 m apart. each day before the number of birds in the field of view became too small. Thus, the experiment was conducted on 4 different days during a 3-week period. Data analysis.-we projected the slides onto outline maps of the marsh and marked the locations of all birds, as well as their postures (head-up or head-down [probing]). We could not obtain exactly the same camera placement each day, so a separate map was drawn for each day s view. The outline maps (Fig. 1) included straight lines drawn approximately parallel to the edge of the marsh and ca. 0.5 m apart. The distance between adjacent lines was estimated by scaling the standing height of dowitchers (Limnodromus) and Dunlins from the slides according to heights measured on mounted museum specimens. From the Dunlin scale the lines were judged to be ca. 53 cm apart; from the dowitcher scale they were ca. 49 cm apart. We used these strips to reference birds locations progressively farther from the marsh. Dependent variables included (1) the number of birds in each strip, (2) the total number of birds, and (3) the percentage of birds in head-up postures. Unless indicated otherwise, statistical tests are P-tailed t-tests for correlated means. Because it was extremely difficult to consistently distinguish C. mauri from C. alpina on the slides and because some Least Sandpipers (C. minutilla) were probably photographed and counted, our data are for C&&is species combined. Larger shorebird species, primarily dowitchers and plovers (Charadrius vociferus, C. semipalmatus), were also photographed. Unfortunately, the numbers of these larger species varied substantially from dayto-day and their density in the camera s field of view dropped precipitously during a field session (due to the pronounced tendency of dowitchers to forage in the receding water). Therefore, we have too few data on a species other than Calidris for meaningful analysis.

Leger and Nelson * SANDPIPER ALARM CALLS 325 TABLE 1 MEAN NUMBER OF CALIDRIS SANDPIPERS ON EACH STRIP OF MUDFLAT BEFORE AND AFTER PLAYBACKS OF AVOCET ALARM CALLS OR NO-SOUND (CONTROL) PLAYBACKS Alarm call No sound Strip Before After Before After * P < 0.02. 1 5.4 2.9* 5.3 6.1 2 5.3 5.0 6.3 5.1 3 2.0 2.1 2.4 2.4 4 1.7 1.0 0.6 0.9 RESULTS Before alarm playbacks (N = lo), an average of 14.4 Calidris was counted in strips l-4 combined, and before no-sound playbacks (N = 7) a similar mean of 14.6 birds was present in each strip. Birds were not equally distributed among the four strips: strips 1 and 2 averaged 5.4 and 5.7 Calidris, respectively, whereas strips 3 and 4 (farther from the marsh) averaged 2.2 and 1.2, respectively. These means differed significantly (F = 8.64; df = 3, 45; P < 0.01 split-plot factorial analysis of variance [Kirk 19681). However, there was no significant interaction between distance from marsh and playback condition (F = 0.33; df = 3, 45; NS). Following alarm playbacks there was an average net loss of 3.4 birds from the total area (from 14.4-ll.O), but the mean totals before vs after playback did not differ (t = 1.08, df = 9, P < 0.20). However, strip 1 had only 53.7% as many Calidris following alarm calls as before (t = 2.87, df = 9, P < 0.02). Net losses in the other three strips were not significantly different (Table 1). In contrast there was no significant net loss of birds following no-sound playbacks, and none of the before vs after playback comparisons were statistically significant (Table 1). The latter indicates that experimenter movements while operating the camera and playback equipment did not measurably alter the birds behavior. Significantly more birds were head-up after alarm call playbacks (68.0%) than before (32.4%) (t = 2.64, df = 7, P < 0.05; two trials were dropped from this analysis because all birds left the field of view following playbacks). In contrast, 29.4% of all birds were head-up after no-sound playbacks, but 45.% were head-up before. This difference was non-significant, however (t = 1.36, df = 6, P < 0.20). Head-up posturing also varied with distance from the marsh following alarm playbacks. In strips 1 and 2, 69% and 62% of the birds were head-up, respectively, as compared with 38% and 40% in strips 3 and 4.

326 THE WILSON BULLETIN * Vol. 94, No. 3, September 1982 DISCUSSION These data suggest that Calidris sandpipers monitor their distance from the marsh edge and use this information when responding to alarm calls. It is intriguing that individuals farther from the source of the alarm, but closer to the marsh, reacted more vigorously than did birds in the reverse situation. This finding supports the notion that sandpipers view the marsh as a source of potential danger. Moreover, sandpipers behave as though alarm calls are indicative of danger from the direction of the marsh. At no time did birds fly toward the marsh in response to playbacks or naturally occurring calls. This differs dramatically from the behavior of Yellow-eyed Juncos (Junco phaeonotus) foraging at varying distances from a small tree. They use the tree as a refuge, flying to it when alarmed (Caraco et al. 1980). Presumably they would also be more likely to take flight the farther they are from vegetation. Our data also suggest that there may be some critical distance from the edge of the marsh at which birds can see far enough back over the vegetation to reliably detect an approaching raptor. This distance would vary with the height of the vegetation, the birds head-up height and the raptor s altitude and approach velocity. We can only speculate on the critical distance here because our measures of location were rather coarse. However, because birds in strip 1 were highly likely to fly or to look up, whereas birds in the second strip tended only to look up, the critical distance for Calidris at this site may be ca. 1 m from the marsh. Birds closer than that tend to fly but those farther away tend only to look up, and if a predator is seen, would then undoubtedly fly away. Indeed, naturally occurring appearances of harriers during the study led to all birds in the area taking flight. Finally, it should be noted that the behavior described above is not restricted to avocet alarm calls, nor to alarm calls at all. Throughout the course of the study many fly-ups were observed in response to calls by Willets (Catoptrophorus semipalmatus), Marbled Godwits (Limosa fedoa), and Killdeer (Charadrius vociferus), as well as Limnodromus and Calidris species. In addition, before conducting this study, Leger observed that Calidris sandpipers nearest the marsh were far more likely to fly in response to a single handclap than ones farther from the marsh (but closer to the sound source). Clearly, the marsh seems to be a potent determinant of shorebird behavior. SUMMARY Following playbacks of an American Avocet alarm-call recording, Calidris sandpipers foraging on a mudflat either flew away from the nearby marsh vegetation, looked up without flying, or continued foraging. The probability of flying and of looking up was highest in birds closest to the marsh, even though they were farthest from the playback speaker. The results

Leger and Nelson * SANDPIPER ALARM CALLS 327 suggest that sandpipers assess their distance from the predator-concealing marsh vegetation and modify their behavior accordingly. Thus, contextual information and information in the alarm call jointly affect sandpiper behavior. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This research was funded through a grant to Leger from the Chancellor s Patent Fund at the University of California, Davis (#3-444080-08613-5). We thank Ted Chandik and Lawrence White of the Parks and Open Space Management Office of the City of Palo Alto, California, for permission to work at the Baylands Nature Center. Edward H. Miller provided many helpful comments on an earlier version of the manuscript. LITERATURE CITED CARACO, T., S. MARTINDALE AND H. R. PULLIAM. 1980. Avian time budgets and distance to cover. Auk 97:872475. CHARNOV, E. L. AND J. R. KF~EBS. 1975. Th e evolution of alarm calls: altruism or manipulation? Am. Nat. 109:107-112. DEKKER, D. 1980. Hunting success rates, foraging habits, and prey selection of Peregrine Falcons migrating through central Alberta. Can. Field-Nat. 94:371-382. HODGDON, H. E. AND J. S. LARSON. 1973. Some sexual differences in behaviour within a colony of marked beavers (Castor canadensis). Anim. Behav. 21:147-152. HUNT, W. G., R. R. ROGERS AND D. J. SLOWE. 1975. Migratory and foraging behavior of Peregrine Falcons on the Texas coast. Can. Field-Nat. 89:111-123. KIRK, R. E. 1%8. Experimental design: procedures for the behavioral sciences. Brooks/ Cole, Belmont, California. LEGER, D. W. AND D. H. OWINGS. 1978. Responses to alarm calls by California ground squirrels: effects of call structure and maternal status. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 3:177-186. -- AND L. BOAL. 1979. Contextual information and differential responses to alarm whistles in California ground squirrels. Z. Tierpsychol. 49:142-155. -- AND D. L. GELFAND. 1980. Single-note vocalizations of California ground squirrels: graded signals and situation-specificity of predator and socially evoked calls. Z. Tierpsychol. 52:227-246. NOYES, D. H. AND W. G. HOLMES. 1979. Behavioral responses of free-living hoary marmots to a model golden eagle. J. Mammal. 60:408411. OWENS, N. W. AND J. D. GOSS-CUSTARD. 1976. The adaptive significance of alarm calls given by shorebirds on their winter feeding grounds. Evolution 30:397398. OWINGS, D. H. AND D. W. LEGER. 1980. Chatter vocalizations of California ground squirrels: predator- and social-role specificity. Z. Tierpsychol. 54:163-184. PAGE, G. AND D. F. WHITACRE. 1975. Raptor predation on wintering shorebirds. Condor 77z73-83. RECHER, H. F. 1966. Some aspects of the ecology of migrant shorebirds. Ecology 47:393-407. RUDEBECK, G. 1950. The choice of prey and modes of hunting of predatory birds with special reference to their selective effect. Oikos 2:66-88. -. 1951. The choice of prey and modes of hunting of predatory birds with special reference to their selective effect. Oikos 3:200-231. SEYFARTH, R. M., D. L. CHENEY AND P. MARLER. 1980a. Monkey responses to three different alarm calls: evidence of predator classification and semantic communication. Science 210:801-803.

328 THE WILSON BULLETIN * Vol. 94, No. 3, September 1982 - -AND -. 1980b. Vervet monkey alarm calls: semantic communication in a free-ranging primate. Anim. Behav. 28:1070-1094. SMITH, W. J. 1%5. Message, meaning and context in ethology. Am. Nat. 99:405X)9. -. 1977. The behavior of communicating: an ethological approach. Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts. WILSON, E. 0. 1975. Sociobiology: the new synthesis. Belknap/Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts. DEPT. PSYCHOLOGY, UNIV. NEBRASKA, LINCOLN, NEBRASKA 68588-0308. ACCEPTED 15 OCT. 1981.