A reduction of visual fields during changes in the background image such as while driving a car and looking in the rearview mirror

Similar documents
Mitigation of Visual Fatigue through the Use of LED Desk Lights that Provide Uniform Brightness on Visual Work Surfaces

PERIMETRY A STANDARD TEST IN OPHTHALMOLOGY

A New Method for Estimating Effects of Visual Field Loss in a Panoramic Driving Environment

THE RELATIVE IMPORTANCE OF PICTORIAL AND NONPICTORIAL DISTANCE CUES FOR DRIVER VISION. Michael J. Flannagan Michael Sivak Julie K.

Driving simulators in hemianopia rehabilitation research

Automated Perimeter PTS 1000

Vision Science I Exam 2 31 October 2016

Instruments Commonly Used For Examination of the Eye

CSC Stereography Course I. What is Stereoscopic Photography?... 3 A. Binocular Vision Depth perception due to stereopsis

A Road Traffic Noise Evaluation System Considering A Stereoscopic Sound Field UsingVirtual Reality Technology

EXAMINATION OF THE CENTRAL VISUAL FIELD AT

Developments of a peripheral vision system using immersive virtual environment

Wide-Band Enhancement of TV Images for the Visually Impaired

Novel 3D Computerized Threshold Amsler Grid Test CA, USA

Low Vision Assessment Components Job Aid 1

Virtual Reality I. Visual Imaging in the Electronic Age. Donald P. Greenberg November 9, 2017 Lecture #21

COPYRIGHTED MATERIAL. Overview

Visual Effects of Light. Prof. Grega Bizjak, PhD Laboratory of Lighting and Photometry Faculty of Electrical Engineering University of Ljubljana

COPYRIGHTED MATERIAL OVERVIEW 1

Low Vision Rehabiliation

Visual Effects of. Light. Warmth. Light is life. Sun as a deity (god) If sun would turn off the life on earth would extinct

AGING AND STEERING CONTROL UNDER REDUCED VISIBILITY CONDITIONS. Wichita State University, Wichita, Kansas, USA

A Three-Dimensional Evaluation of Body Representation Change of Human Upper Limb Focused on Sense of Ownership and Sense of Agency

The impact of non-immersive head-mounted displays (HMDs) on the visual field

Development and Validation of Virtual Driving Simulator for the Spinal Injury Patient

Examination of the ten degrees of visual field surrounding fixation

3-D Computer-Automated Threshold Amsler Grid Test

This is the author s version of a work that was submitted/accepted for publication in the following source:

Human Vision and Human-Computer Interaction. Much content from Jeff Johnson, UI Wizards, Inc.

Three-Dimensional Viewing of Distant Objects

Limitations of the Oriented Difference of Gaussian Filter in Special Cases of Brightness Perception Illusions

Frey AP-300 Humphrey Zeiss HFA II 740 perimeters

Application of 3D Terrain Representation System for Highway Landscape Design

Introduction to Psychology Prof. Braj Bhushan Department of Humanities and Social Sciences Indian Institute of Technology, Kanpur

Information Guide. Synoptophore (Major Amblyoscope) Heading. Body copy. Body copy bold

User Interfaces in Panoramic Augmented Reality Environments

Marco TRS Total Refraction System

Goldmann vs. Humphrey

the dimensionality of the world Travelling through Space and Time Learning Outcomes Johannes M. Zanker

Visual acuity finally a complete platform

Modified Bagolini striated glass test: clinical applications of starlight test in binocular visual field screening

TRAFFIC SIGN DETECTION AND IDENTIFICATION.

Chapter 8: Perceiving Motion

Centre Hospitalier Universitaire et Psychiatrique de Mons-Borinage. B-Flex Multifocal. Dr Emmanuel Van Acker Belgium

Air-filled type Immersive Projection Display

Self-motion perception from expanding and contracting optical flows overlapped with binocular disparity

Paper on: Optical Camouflage

OCULUS Twinfield 2. Perimeter. We focus on progress

Retinal stray light originating from intraocular lenses and its effect on visual performance van der Mooren, Marie Huibert

The quarry truck is typically a 40 to 100 ton rigid frame dump truck. Following (Figure 1) is a photo of a typical quarry truck.

Assessments of Grade Crossing Warning and Signalization Devices Driving Simulator Study

Don t twinkle, little star!

(Day)light Metrics. Dr.- Ing Jan Wienold. epfl.ch Lab URL: EPFL ENAC IA LIPID

ERDS Simulator Emergency Response Driving Simulator

Behavioural Realism as a metric of Presence

Suppression in strabismus

ISO 8596 INTERNATIONAL STANDARD. Ophthalmic optics Visual acuity testing Standard optotype and its presentation

Low Vision and Virtual Reality : Preliminary Work

September November 2010

Perceiving Motion and Events

CAN GALVANIC VESTIBULAR STIMULATION REDUCE SIMULATOR ADAPTATION SYNDROME? University of Guelph Guelph, Ontario, Canada

P1.4. Light has to go where it is needed: Future Light Based Driver Assistance Systems

Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion

THRESHOLD AMSLER GRID TESTING AND RESERVING POWER OF THE POTIC NERVE by MOUSTAFA KAMAL NASSAR. M.D. MENOFIA UNIVERSITY.

Multifocal Electroretinograms in Normal Subjects

Peripheral Prism Glasses for Hemianopia Giorgi et al. APPENDIX 1

Speckle Mitigation in Laser-Based Projectors

Retina. Convergence. Early visual processing: retina & LGN. Visual Photoreptors: rods and cones. Visual Photoreptors: rods and cones.

C501-E029C. RADspeed. GENERAL RADIOGRAPHIC SYSTEM Automatic

Huang Ke 1,2 *, Weng Ji 1 1 Faculty of Architecture and Urban Planning, Chongqing University, Chongqing,

We focus on progress OCULUS CENTERFIELD 2

FLASH LiDAR KEY BENEFITS

Poles for Increasing the Sensibility of Vertical Gradient. in a Downhill Road

DIGITAL IMAGE PROCESSING LECTURE # 4 DIGITAL IMAGE FUNDAMENTALS-I

Three-dimensional computer-automated threshold Amsler grid test

Effects of Pupil Center Shift on Ocular Aberrations

Nagoya University Center of Innovation (COI)

/ Impact of Human Factors for Mixed Reality contents: / # How to improve QoS and QoE? #

FRAUNHOFER INSTITUTE FOR OPEN COMMUNICATION SYSTEMS FOKUS COMPETENCE CENTER VISCOM

CSE 190: Virtual Reality Technologies LECTURE #7: VR DISPLAYS

Research report. Simple 1-D image enhancement for head-mounted low vision aid. Eli Peli

DIFFERENCE BETWEEN A PHYSICAL MODEL AND A VIRTUAL ENVIRONMENT AS REGARDS PERCEPTION OF SCALE

State of the Science Symposium

Varilux Comfort. Technology. 2. Development concept for a new lens generation

Efficacy of the Pelli-Levi Dual Acuity Chart in diagnosing amblyopia

The Perception of Optical Flow in Driving Simulators

ATLAS Corneal Topography System

Perceived depth is enhanced with parallax scanning

Distributed Vision System: A Perceptual Information Infrastructure for Robot Navigation

Effects of Viewing Angle and Contrast Ratio on Visual Performance using TFT-LCD

Evaluation of High Intensity Discharge Automotive Forward Lighting

Patients in your area are ready to set appointments with you. Keep reading on to learn why they re eager to use our system.

STUDY OF ADULT STRABISMUS TESTING PROCEDURES MANUAL

Slide 4 Now we have the same components that we find in our eye. The analogy is made clear in this slide. Slide 5 Important structures in the eye

Chapter 1 Virtual World Fundamentals

SYSTEM CHART SC-1700 Pola / 2000

Real-time Simulation of Arbitrary Visual Fields

The Appearance of Images Through a Multifocal IOL ABSTRACT. through a monofocal IOL to the view through a multifocal lens implanted in the other eye

OCULAR MEDIA* PHOTOGRAPHIC RECORDING OF OPACITIES OF THE. development by the control of diabetes, the supply of a deficient hormone

APPENDIX GLOSSARY OF TERMS

Transcription:

Original Contribution Kitasato Med J 2012; 42: 138-142 A reduction of visual fields during changes in the background image such as while driving a car and looking in the rearview mirror Tomoya Handa Department of Rehabilitation, Orthoptics and Visual Science Course, School of Allied Health Sciences, Kitasato University Objective: We attempted to measure visual fields during changes in background image to simulate driving a car using the new virtual reality system. Methods: Twenty-nine healthy young volunteers were enrolled in this study to experience visual perceptions, like being surrounded by visual images, which is a feature of the "CyberDome" hemispherical visual display system. Visual images to the right and left eye were projected and superimposed on the dome screen, allowing test images to be seen independently by each eye with polarizing glasses. Visual fields were measured during changes in background image. Background image speed was changed between 0, 60, 150, and 300 km/h. Subjects gazed at the center and pushed a button when a target presenting from the top, bottom, left, or right was perceived. Results: With dynamic background images, visual fields were smaller (about 7%) than during standstill (0 km/h). Visual field reduction (lower, left, and right visual fields) increased with increased speeds (60, 150, and 300 km/h) of the background image. Upper visual field reduction tended to be somewhat greater than that at 0 km/h, but the difference was not significant. Conclusions: These findings suggest that visual fields are reduced when viewing dynamic background images such as while driving a car looking forward and then suddenly looking in the rearview mirror. Key words: virtual reality, hemispherical visual display system, dynamic visual field test Introduction S tatic and kinetic perimetry are used for most visual field testing in modern ophthalmology. Static perimetry measures the threshold for visibility of a static target in a grid (retinal sensitivity), and kinetic perimetry maps a visual field by an illuminated moving target in a grid (movement from nonvisible to visible). Both differ in terms of target presentation and methods of measurement, but apart from the target image, other test conditions are similar in the sense that surrounding objects are static. However, in the everyday environment, surrounding objects are moving, so whether current visual field tests accurately evaluate realistic visual perception remains unclear. In everyday life, the brain processes far more moving visual information than static visual information. For example, perception of moving images is extremely important when driving, given the constant movement of background images outside of the central vision. Intact peripheral vision is also essential to drive safely and be constantly aware of surrounding traffic signals, signs and pedestrians. Cognitive scientific research has found that useful visual fields reduce as vigilance increases, such as when driving a car. Relationships between traffic accidents and driving ability, age, visual acuity, contrast sensitivity and useful visual field have been extensively investigated. Results show that the most important factor associated with traffic accidents is useful visual field. 1 Useful visual field is defined as central vision and the surrounding spatial area from which one gets information to perform a visual task. In clinical ophthalmology, "visual range" and "retinal sensitivity" measured as visual fields differ. To date, however, visual field tests have been determined only under static conditions (surrounding objects other than the target image are static). Little ophthalmological research has been performed under dynamic conditions (i.e., surrounding objects other than Received 11 November 2011, accepted 7 February 2012 Correspondence to: Tomoya Handa, Department of Rehabilitation, Orthoptics and Visual Science Course, School of Allied Health Sciences, Kitasato University 1-15-1 Kitasato, Minami-ku, Sagamihara, Kanagawa 252-0373, Japan E-mail: thanda@kitasato-u.ac.jp 138

Visual field test using virtual reality the target image are also moving) as experienced in daily life, such as when driving. Due to the obvious difficulty in evaluating visual function when actually driving a car, we devised an effective visual function test system in the dynamic conditions as experienced daily life using the CyberDome 1400 virtual reality display system. 2 In present study, we investigated the changes of the visual fields by driving car using a virtual reality display system. Subjects and Methods Sample Subjects comprised 29 volunteers without ocular disorders (mean age, 20.97 years; range, 18-24 years). Selected subjects were able to understand the test procedure and test results were reproducible. Corrected visual acuity was -0.08 (Log MAR) or better and near stereopsis was evaluated with the Titmus stereo test (Stereo Optical, IL, USA) to confirm that all subjects had stereoacuity better than 60 seconds of arc. There was no significant abnormality in the visual field test using a Humphrey Field Analyser (Carl Zeiss Meditec, CA, USA), like that determined from the criteria proposed by Anderson and Patella. 3 All subjects provided written informed consent before participating in this study and the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki were followed. We certify that all applicable institutional and governmental regulations concerning the ethical use of human volunteers were followed during this research. Test apparatus The CyberDome 1400 virtual reality display system (Panasonic Electric Works) was used in this study to change background image speed while measuring visual fields with both eyes open. The CyberDome 1400 is a hemispherical visual display system for widescreen projection. 4 Two liquid crystal projectors are used to display polarization images at horizontal and vertical viewing angles of 100 on a dome-shaped screen with a diameter of 1.4 meters. Corrected images are reflected by a flat mirror in front of the projectors, and the compact image is projected on the dome screen. Figures 1 and 2 depict the display and system configuration. Synchronous processing is performed by two slave computers and one master computer to project a 3-dimensional image. Figure 1. The CyberDome 1400 virtual reality display system (Panasonic Electric Works) Figure 2. CyberDome 1400 system configuration Corrected images are reflected by a flat mirror in front of the projectors, and the 3-D image is projected on the dome screen. 139

Handa Projector angle of view, screen configuration and placement represent input parameters for software used for image correction to provide distortion-free images on the dome screen. Test design Subjects, after complete correction of refraction, wore polarized glasses and fixed the gaze on a central target on the dome screen 1 m in front of the eyes. The size of a central target is 1 and a luminance is 7 cd/m 2. Visual fields were measured with both eyes open. A test target (luminance 17 cd/m 2 ; size 5 ) from one of 4 directions (top, bottom, left or right) moving at a speed of 5 degrees/ second was presented and subjects pushed a button when they saw the target. The size of test target is 5 and a luminance is 17 cd/m 2. During testing, the head of the subject was restrained by chin and forehead supports. Figure 3 shows the test apparatus. The background image was a straight road in an urban area. The four background image speeds were 0, 60, 150, and 300 km/h. Visual fields were measured under each condition. Test target direction and background image speeds were randomized into 16 different patterns. Figure 3. (Left) Test image displayed by virtual reality display system using the CyberDome 1400; (right) test apparatus setup: Subjects fixed the gaze on a central target and pushed a button when they saw the test target. Figure 4. Visual field results with changes in background image speed. Black, 0 km (standstill); horizontal hatching, 60 km; diagonal hatching, 150 km; white, 300 km. *P < 0.05; Fisher's PLSD 140

Visual field test using virtual reality Statistical analysis Data were examined by one-way analysis of variance and Fisher's protected least significant difference (PLSD). The level of statistical significance was less than 5%. Results Visual field testing using this apparatus could be performed for all subjects. Figure 4 shows the upper, lower, left and right visual field results for each background image speed. A change in speed of the background image (60, 150, and 300 km/h) resulted in reduction of the lower, left and right visual fields as compared to standstill (0 km/h) (Fisher's PLSD, P < 0.05). In the upper visual field, a change in speed of the background image (60, 150, and 300 km/h) resulted in was no significant difference compared to standstill (0 km/h) (Fisher's PLSD, P > 0.05). Total visual field angle for both eyes (upper, lower, left, and right) was 334.7 at standstill, 310.2 at 60 km/h, 312.9 at 150 km/h and 310.3 at 300 km/h. Compared to standstill, visual field reduction was 7.3% at 60 km/h, 6.5% at 150 km/h, and 7.2% at 300 km/h. Discussion In this study using virtual reality display system to simulate driving a car, we confirmed that visual fields reduced with changes in background image, as compared to standstill (0 km/h). However, visual field reduction is not significant increased with a rise in background image speed. In current ophthalmology practice, visual field testing is usually performed using a Humphrey Field Analyzer (Carl Zeiss Meditec, CA, USA) for static perimetry and a Goldmann perimeter (Haag-Streit International., Switzerland) for kinetic perimetry. However, measurements in both tests are performed under conditions in which surrounding objects other than the target image are static. Furthermore, measurements under conditions where surrounding objects are actually moving, as when driving a car, are obviously difficult. This study used a CyberDome 1400 hemispherical visual display system to simulate the experience of driving a car. Compared to standstill, the overall visual field (left, right, upper, and lower) reduced about 7% with changes in background image. The reasons for visual field reduction include effects in attentional capacity (useful visual field), an apparent increase in target speed due to movement of the background image in a direction opposite to the target, 5 decreased detection of motion in 141 peripheral visual fields 6 and peripheral visual field reduction with changes in background image. The background image in the upper visual field was always "blue sky." Subjects, therefore, may not have perceived as much motion of this background image or apparent increases in speed. This is probably the reason why the upper visual field was the only field not to display significant reduction. In present study, the lower visual field in background speeds at 150 and 300 km/h was superior to that of background speed at 60 km/h. Because the lower visual field play an important part in daily life, for example, in jogging and driving a car. Therefore, under high speed at 150 and 300 km/h, the spatial visual attention of the lower visual field improved for crisis prevention. With only a change in background image, visual field reduction was still about 7%. When driving a car in real life, other factors are involved that may further increase visual field reduction, including operating the vehicle, acceleration, possible fear or anxiety and aging. A higher risk of traffic accidents with visual field reduction as compared to age-matched normal controls, and a higher risk of falls and traffic accidents in moderate or severe glaucoma as compared to normal controls 7 have been reported. Our test results performed under dynamic conditions as seen when driving a car detected greater visual field reduction than during conventional testing using a Goldmann perimeter. This highlights the need in clinical ophthalmology to measure visual fields in elderly patients with abnormalities under more realistic conditions. The static perimetry is not necessarily evaluating the practical visual field. In the future, we aim to develop a realistic visual test system that estimates the practical visual fields which can actually be used in daily life. Acknowledgements This study was supported by a grant from Kitasato University School of Allied Health Sciences (Grant-in- Aid for Research Project, No. 2007-1029, and 2008-1025). References 1. Owsley C. Vision and driving in the elderly. Optom Vis Sci 1994; 71: 727-35. 2. Handa T, Ishikawa H, Shimizu K, et al. A novel apparatus for testing binocular function using the 'CyberDome' three-dimensional hemispherical visual display system. Eye (Lond) 2009; 23:2094-8.

Handa 3. Anderson DR, Patella VM. Automated static perimetry. 2nd edition, St. Louis: Mosby; 1999; 121-90. 4. Shibano N, Hareesh PV, Kashiwagi M, et al. Development of VR experiencing system with a hemispherical immersive projection display. Asia Display/IDW'01, pp. 1369-72. 5. Tynan P, Sekuler R. Simultaneous motion contrast: velocity, sensitivity and depth response. Vision Res 1975; 15: 1231-8. 6. McKee SP, Nakayama K. The detection of motion in the peripheral visual field. Vision Res 1984; 24: 25-32. 7. Haymes SA, Leblanc RP, Nicolela MT, et al. Risk of falls and motor vehicle collisions in glaucoma. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 2007; 48: 1149-55. 142