Characteristics of the Visual Perception under the Dark Adaptation Processing (The Lighting Systems for Signboards)

Similar documents
Visibility, Performance and Perception. Cooper Lighting

LIGHT AND LIGHTING FUNDAMENTALS. Prepared by Engr. John Paul Timola

Time Course of Chromatic Adaptation to Outdoor LED Displays

Radiometric and Photometric Measurements with TAOS PhotoSensors

20W TL 324 smd LED Warm White by Simplify-It

LED T5 30cm Warm White by BS Ledlight

EFFECTS OF AUTOMATICALLY CONTROLLED BLINDS ON VISUAL

Lighting Terminologies Introduction

CGT 511 Perception. Facts. Facts. Facts. When perceiving visual information

Light-Emitting Diodes

Work environment. Retina anatomy. A human eyeball is like a simple camera! The way of vision signal. Directional sensitivity. Lighting.

THE PERCEPTION OF LIGHT AFFECTED BY COLOUR SURFACES IN INDOOR SPACES

Digital Image Processing

Pupil Lumens and their impact on the choice of lighting

Light and sight. Sight is the ability for a token to "see" its surroundings

12/02/2017. From light to colour spaces. Electromagnetic spectrum. Colour. Correlated colour temperature. Black body radiation.

Work environment. Vision. Human Millieu system. Retina anatomy. A human eyeball is like a simple camera! Lighting. Eye anatomy. Cones colours

We have already discussed retinal structure and organization, as well as the photochemical and electrophysiological basis for vision.

Visual Effects of Light. Prof. Grega Bizjak, PhD Laboratory of Lighting and Photometry Faculty of Electrical Engineering University of Ljubljana

Why is blue tinted backlight better?

Evaluation of High Intensity Discharge Automotive Forward Lighting

ABB i-bus EIB Light controller LR/S and light sensor LF/U 1.1

Economic and Social Council

Basic lighting quantities

Digital Image Processing

Discomfort and Disability Glare from Halogen and HID Headlamp Systems

Colour. Why/How do we perceive colours? Electromagnetic Spectrum (1: visible is very small part 2: not all colours are present in the rainbow!

Roadway Glare & Reflection Technical Data

Visual Effects of. Light. Warmth. Light is life. Sun as a deity (god) If sun would turn off the life on earth would extinct

Colour. Electromagnetic Spectrum (1: visible is very small part 2: not all colours are present in the rainbow!) Colour Lecture!

Visual Imaging and the Electronic Age Color Science

Huang Ke 1,2 *, Weng Ji 1 1 Faculty of Architecture and Urban Planning, Chongqing University, Chongqing,

Why Optimizing VLT* May Not Be Optimal. *VLT = Visible Light Transmission

Reading. 1. Visual perception. Outline. Forming an image. Optional: Glassner, Principles of Digital Image Synthesis, sections

CS-2000/2000A. Spectroradiometer NEW

Lecture 2 Digital Image Fundamentals. Lin ZHANG, PhD School of Software Engineering Tongji University Fall 2016

Spectroradiometer CS-2000/2000A. The world's top-level capability spectroradiometers make further advances with addition of second model to lineup.

Dynamic Visual Performance of LED with Different Color Temperature

Vision and Visibility. Human Eye. Eye Components. Cones and Rods. Typical Vision Impairments. CVEN 457 & 696 Lecture #3 Gene Hawkins

Range of Acceptable Illuminance by LED Colors in Indoor Spaces

Spectroradiometer CS-2000/2000A. The world's top-level capability spectroradiometers make further advances with addition of second model to lineup.

Colorimetry and Color Modeling

UNIVERSITY OF HONG KONG

Lighting: Basic Concepts

EC-433 Digital Image Processing

Example 1: The visitors are guided to the building entrance by means of increasing illumination densities.

Basic Lighting Design Seminar

Human Senses : Vision week 11 Dr. Belal Gharaibeh

It is important to use the right ballast to ensure the luminaire also ignites at low temperatures.

3 Laboratory measurement of the illuminance level at the eye

Tokina AT-X F2 PRO DX. Short zoom that took all the best from prime lenses

APPENDIX GLOSSARY OF TERMS

Project: IEEE P Working Group for Wireless Personal Area Networks (WPANs)

STUDY NOTES UNIT I IMAGE PERCEPTION AND SAMPLING. Elements of Digital Image Processing Systems. Elements of Visual Perception structure of human eye

Applicability of Advanced Light Control Concepts with KNX

(Day)light Metrics. Dr.- Ing Jan Wienold. epfl.ch Lab URL: EPFL ENAC IA LIPID

An enhanced roof-mounted light pipe supplemented with LED lamps

This paper is being distributed electronically under the Fair Use Copyright Act. One print or electronic copy may be made for personal use only.

Uniformly Illuminated Efficient Daylighting System

Tunnel and underpass lighting

Further reading. 1. Visual perception. Restricting the light. Forming an image. Angel, section 1.4

LIGHT AND HEALTH IN FACTORY WORK PLACES

The new technology enables 8K high resolution and high picture quality imaging without motion distortion, even in extremely bright scenes.

Image and Multidimensional Signal Processing

This is due to Purkinje shift. At scotopic conditions, we are more sensitive to blue than to red.

DIGITAL IMAGE PROCESSING LECTURE # 4 DIGITAL IMAGE FUNDAMENTALS-I

SIM University Color, Brightness, Contrast, Smear Reduction and Latency. Stuart Nicholson Program Architect, VE.

CS 565 Computer Vision. Nazar Khan PUCIT Lecture 4: Colour

Section 2 concludes that a glare meter based on a digital camera is probably too expensive to develop and produce, and may not be simple in use.

What is LED? What is LED? LED = Light emitting diode that will emit lights when it is given electricity

E X P E R I M E N T 12

E/ECE/324/Rev.1/Add.86/Rev.3/Amend.1 E/ECE/TRANS/505/Rev.1/Add.86/Rev.3/Amend.1

Solar lighting system HIMAWARI

Electrical Illumination and Design

A HYBRID MODULATION METHOD FOR DIMMING IN VISIBLE LIGHT COMMUNICATION

The Science Seeing of process Digital Media. The Science of Digital Media Introduction

Analysis of Outdoor Lighting Control Systems and Devices for the Creation of Outdoor Lighting Automatic Control System Using the Traffic Flow Value

Fundamentals of Radiometry & Photometry

COLOR. and the human response to light

Visual Perception of Images

Refraction of Light. Refraction of Light

Our Color Vision is Limited

Chapter 20 Human Vision

Chapter 16 Light Waves and Color

Vision Science I Exam 2 31 October 2016

Contours, Saliency & Tone Mapping. Donald P. Greenberg Visual Imaging in the Electronic Age Lecture 21 November 3, 2016

COLOR and the human response to light

Where to Consider Photocontrol for Inside Lighting

07-Lighting Concepts. EE570 Energy Utilization & Conservation Professor Henry Louie

Module 3. Illumination Systems. Version 2 EE IIT, Kharagpur 1

Agus Hariyadi a,b,* and Hiroatsu Fukuda b a. Abstract

Estimation of Illuminance/Luminance Influence Factor in Intelligent Lighting System Using Operation Log Data

Portable Noise Monitoring Report March 5 - April 24, 2016 The Museum of Vancouver. Vancouver Airport Authority

Lumen lm 1 lm= 1cd 1sr The luminous flux emitted into unit solid angle (1 sr) by an isotropic point source having a luminous intensity of 1 candela

Lecture Color Image Processing. by Shahid Farid

Hint: Think about linear systems and how they behave).

Vision and Color. Reading. Optics, cont d. Lenses. d d f. Brian Curless CSEP 557 Fall Good resources:

Vision and Color. Brian Curless CSEP 557 Fall 2016

Physics 4C Chabot College Scott Hildreth

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I

Transcription:

66 IJCSNS International Journal of Computer Science and Network Security, VOL.11 No.8, August 2011 Characteristics of the Visual Perception under the Dark Adaptation Processing (The Lighting Systems for Signboards) Kazuo Sendai, Hiroki Fujita, Mamoru Takamatsu, and Yoshio Nakashima General Manager of Panasonic Electric Works C & T Co.,Ltd., Osaka, 571-8686 Japan Ph.D. student in the Department of Intellectual Information Systems Engineering, Toyama University, Toyama, 930-0887 Japan Lecturer of Graduate School of Science and Engineering for Research Life, Toyama University, Toyama, 930-0887 Japan Faculty of Graduate School of Science and Engineering for Research Life, Toyama University, Toyama, 930-0887 Japan Summary Outdoor signboards on the street provide not only advertising but also effective scenery of the town, and they are useful for the activation of the town. Sometimes they have different faces according to the effect of the lighting systems in daytime and evening. In this experiment, we use lighted signboards to examine how human eyes see object colors under the dark conditions. Advertising signboards that we recognize as object colors begin to change into light source colors, when it darkens gradually in the evening. When this change occurs, the horizontal illuminance decreases from 1000 to 100 lx. In addition, brightness feeling and the ratio of light source colors rise rapidly when the luminance difference between the sky and signboard falls below 200cd/m 2. Key words: Dark adaptation, Object color, Llight source color, Sky luminance, Lighting systems of signboards considering the characteristics of the visual dark adaptation. One of the familiar examples of this is tunnel lighting design, in which the illuminance near the portal of a tunnel is changed in a step-by-step manner according to the sky luminance depending on weather and time. That is, when a vehicle enters a tunnel and moves toward the inside of the tunnel, it takes time for the driver to adapt to darkness. For driver s clear eyesight and preventing black-out phenomenon, the tunnel must be lighted so that the portal of the tunnel gets higher illuminance. Such eyesight adjustment by ambient luminance level change in visual environment frequently occurs in our daily life. 1. Introduction Humans get information from the outside world by means of their five senses, i.e., eyesight, hearing, smell, taste, and touch. Out of these senses, eyesight is most important, and it is said that the percentage of the information gotten with eyesight is 80% or more of the total amount of the information gotten with the five senses. We can see objects in the daylight as well as in the evening. Illuminance in direct sunlight in midsummer is very high, about 100,000 lx, but illuminance in the evening is very low, about 0.001 lx. Because our visual system changes its sensitivity appropriately, we can see visual objects properly in various environments with very high or very low illuminance. This sensitivity change of the visual system is called adaptation. Especially, the visual sensitivity change caused when the light environment becomes dark is called dark adaptation. Color and light senses in our visual system largely change in the dark adaptation process because rod cells on the retina become active instead of cone cells. Therefore, illumination design must be performed by fully Figure 1 (a) Signboard in daytime (b) Signboard in evening Visible condition of signboard in the daytime or the evening A typical example of the effective eyesight adjustment Manuscript received August 5, 2011 Manuscript revised August 20, 2011

IJCSNS International Journal of Computer Science and Network Security, VOL.11 No.8, August 2011 67 is lighting of outdoor advertising signboards. Outside advertising signboards that are placed on the roofs of buildings or along streets are lighted by sunlight in the daylight and perceived as object colors (see Figure 1 (a) ). On the other hand in the evening, these signboards are lighted with floodlights placed above or below the signboards, and are seen in a different way, that is, perceived as light source colors (see Figure 1 (b) ). That is, we see these signboards in the dark of the night as if the signboards themselves are gleaming. The signboards that are perceived in the object color mode in the daytime are perceived in the light source color mode in the evening. The purpose of outside signboards is to transmit information to more people at any time of the day or night. Therefore, more effective lighting of signboards is requested to make them more impressive also in the evening. In addition, environmental measures mainly for energy saving have been advanced in all business fields according to, for example, the amended Law Regarding the Rationalization of Energy Use enacted in 2010. Designing the guideline for the concrete measures has been promoted to establish the standards for energy saving management. The important problem in this activity is to design the lighting that provides high efficiency and high quality without reducing lighting effects. Therefore, it is considered that effective lighting timing depending on human s dark adaptation also contributes to energy consumption reduction. The purpose of our study is to analyze and examine how the perception of lighted-up visual objects changes when the visual system changes from the light adaptation mode to the dark adaptation mode. At the same time, we think that the basic data acquired regarding the visual property in brightness level change in visual environment will be very useful in the future signboard lighting system design. 2. Experimental Method Figure 2 shows the advertising signboard used in our experiment, and Figure 3 shows the general view of the experimental system. Our observation and measurement using the signboards started in the daytime (photopic vision), continued to the twilight (mesopic vision), and terminated in the evening (scotopic vision). In our study, we focused attention on the light source color phenomenon in which the signboard surface which was seen in object color mode in the daytime was seen in the evening as if it was gleaming. We called this phenomenon light source color mode. As the light source color mode and the object color mode could exist at the same time, we measured the ratio of the light source color mode to the object color mode on the signboard surface in each visual environment. By the way, the feeling of brightness that is perceived about visual objects depends on various elements such as luminance around the signboards. Therefore, also the feeling of brightness that was directly sensed by the participants of our experiment was measured as brightness feeling. (a) Signboard in daytime (photopic vision) (b) Signboard in evening (scotopic vision) Figure 2 Experimented signboard in the daytime and the evening The ratio of the light source color mode to the object color mode was calculated by the quantity estimation method. That is, when the illuminance of the visual environment of the signboards was 0 lx (complete darkness), the participants observed the signboards previously. In this state, the rate of the light source color mode on the signboard surface was counted as 10 points. Next, when the visual environment changed, the ratio of the light source color mode to the object color mode was newly counted so that the sum of the light source color mode and the object color mode was 10 points. For example, when the ratio of the sensed light source color mode to the sensed object color mode is the same, the

68 IJCSNS International Journal of Computer Science and Network Security, VOL.11 No.8, August 2011 color ratio is evaluated as 5:5. On the other hand, the brightness feeling was evaluated by the participants at seven levels: Very bright to very dark. This experiment is a field experiment that is performed under natural light conditions by using the signboards actually placed under the open sky. The used signboards had different dimensions, and the visual distance to the signboards were determined by considering participant s observation easiness and safety. the horizontal illuminance (lx) at the observation point, the vertical axis on the right side indicates the ratio of the light source color mode to the object color mode, and the vertical axis on the left side indicates the degree of brightness feeling. The observation started at the highest horizontal illuminance (5000 lx), and terminated at 1 lx. Figure 3 General view of experimental system The experiment was performed as follows. First, the horizontal illuminance at the participant s eye level at the observation point was measured as the factor determining the participant s eye adaptation level. The experiment started when the horizontal illuminance at the observation point reached 5000 lx. Then, the experiment terminated when the illuminance in the visual environment reached the minimum level and did not decrease any more. The horizontal illuminance during the measurement period was divided into 14 levels, and the measured data was evaluated at respective levels. That is, at respective horizontal illuminance conditions, each participant evaluated the ratio of the light source color mode to the object color mode of the observed signboard, and also evaluated the brightness feeling. In parallel with these evaluations, also the signboard surface luminance and the ambient sky luminance were measured with a luminance meter. This experiment used six different signboards and 20 participants. 3. Experimental Results Figure 4 shows the signboard observation transition characteristics based on the average values of the data acquired by 20 participants. Figure 4 (a) shows the average measurement values of the four different signboards that have earlier lighting-up timing, and Figure 4 (b) shows the average measurement values of the two different signboards that have later lighting-up timing. In each of Figure 4 (a) and 4 (b), the horizontal axis indicates Figure 4 Transition characteristics of horizontal illuminance and degree of brightness feeling As shown in the ratio of the light source color mode to the object color mode in Figure 4 (a), the signboard status is clearly changed from the object color mode to the light source color mode by degrees with decreased amount of the horizontal illuminance. Especially, when the horizontal illuminance at the observation point reaches 1000 lx, the rate of the light source color mode obviously begins to increase. Next, when the horizontal illuminance decreases to 100 lx, the rate of the light source color mode exceeds the rate of the object color mode.

IJCSNS International Journal of Computer Science and Network Security, VOL.11 No.8, August 2011 69 Such tendencies are found in all of the four samples shown in Figure 4 (a). This proves that the horizontal illuminance is 1000 lx or less when the signboard surfaces that are perceived in object color mode in the daytime begin to be perceived in light source color mode. It can be said that this horizontal illuminance value provides evident lighting effects on advertising signboards. On the other hand, in the two signboards having later lighting-up timing (Figure 4 (b) ), the horizontal illuminance at the observation point is very low when the rate of the light source color mode begins to increase. This tendency may be created by two causes: (1) The lighting-up timing is later, that is, the lighting up occurs when the ambient illuminance is 100 lx or lower, and (2) one of these two signboards is almost black in the surface colors. These show that determining appropriate lighting-up timing of advertising signboards according to the illuminance level of the visual environment is extremely important in the design of effective advertising signboard lighting. The brightness feeling that is indicated on the vertical axis on the left in each of Figure 4 (a) and 4 (b) is divided into seven levels: Very bright (3) to very dark (-3). In Figure 4 (a), the brightness feeling value begins to increase when the horizontal illuminance at the observation point reaches 1000 lx. It is clear that most participants sense brightish state (brightness feeling value 1 ) when the horizontal illuminance reaches about 250 lx. It can be known that most participants evaluate that the signboard surfaces are quite bright when the horizontal illuminance reaches about 0 lx. On the other hand, in Figure 4 (b), the brightness feeling evaluation value has a tendency similar to that shown in Figure 4 (a) when the horizontal illuminance is in the range of 5000 to 3000 lx, but decreases considerably when the horizontal illuminance decreases to lower than 3000 lx. Especially, when the horizontal illuminance is about 250 lx, the average evaluation value is -1.8 (quite dark). Next, when the signboard is lighted up at a horizontal illuminance of about 100 lx, the brightness feeling evaluation value increases sharply. Therefore, it can be considered that the lighting-up timing at 100 lx is slightly later than the approximate timing. By considering this, it can be considered that the most appropriate lighting-up should occur when the horizontal illuminance at the observation point is in the range of 1000 to 250 lx. By the way, as the sunset time in Japan greatly depends on the season and month, the most appropriate lighting-up time depends on the season and month. Figure 5 shows how three levels of horizontal illuminance (250, 500, and 1000 lx) change depending on the season (month) and time. As shown in this figure, the earliest time for illuminance decreasing to each of these levels is about 16:30 in December, and the latest time is about 19:00 in July. It can be said that it is most important to construct the effective signboard lighting system by considering the optimal, variable lighting-up time and energy saving. Figure5 Horizontal illuminance change depending on season and time Figure 6 shows the ratio of the light source color mode to the object color mode and the brightness feeling that are replotted by considering the luminance difference between the sky and the signboards. In the same way as in Figure 4, the vertical axis on the left side indicates the degree of brightness feeling, and the vertical axis on the right side indicates the ratio of the light source color mode to the object color mode. However, in Figure 6, the horizontal axis indicates the difference between the sky luminance and the signboard luminance. Figure 6 Luminance difference between sky and signboards, ratio of light source color mode to object color mode, and brightness feeling On the other hand, in Figure 4, the perception status sharply changes when the horizontal illuminance on the

70 IJCSNS International Journal of Computer Science and Network Security, VOL.11 No.8, August 2011 observation point reaches 1000 lx or lower. However, in Figure 6, in which the luminance difference is plotted on the horizontal axis, the rate of the light source color mode hardly changes until the luminance difference reaches 200 cd/m 2, but the brightness feeling gradually decreases. As shown in Figure 6, when the luminance difference decreases to a value less than 200 cd/m 2, both the rate of light source color mode and the brightness feeling have a tendency to rapidly increase. Next, these values increase sharply when the luminance difference is less than 0 cd/m 2, that is, when the signboard luminance is higher than the sky luminance. Thus, it was found that the lighting-up effect was sharply increased at a luminance difference of 200 cd/m 2 or less. 4. Conclusion This study measured and examined the perception transition of the surfaces of advertising signboards (as lighted-up visual objects) in a dark adaptation process. As a result, it was found that the surfaces of advertising signboards, which were perceived in object color mode in daytime, were perceived in light source color mode in a specific visual environment. This tendency was found in a visual environment in which the horizontal illuminance at the observation point was 1000 lx or lower. When the horizontal illuminance at the observation point was 250 lx, the brightness feeling evaluation value was 1. The above results were acquired regarding the transition of the horizontal illuminance. Similarly, the signboards were perceived in the light source color mode when the luminance difference was 200 cd/m 2 or less. It can be said, from the above, that designing the lighting-up timing of outdoor advertising signboards is effective by considering the eye adaptation at an illuminance of about 250 lx (in which brightness feeling is 1). In addition, it can be considered that the most effective lighting up is performed when the difference between the sky luminance and the signboard luminance is less than 200 cd/m 2. By the way, as the sunset time depends on the season and month as explained before, the most appropriate lighting-up time depends on the season and month. It can be said that it is more effective to control the lighting-up time more carefully by using a luminance sensor as well as the lighting-up system that has a year timer adjusted according to the sunset time in each season. It is increasingly expected to construct the optimal outside-signboard lighting-up control system for superior lighting control by considering the results of our study about the difference between the sky luminance and the signboard luminance. In the meantime, there are amazing results in recent technological development for new light sources. Highly-efficient HID light sources, which will replace conventional mercury lamps, have been developed one after another and put to practical use for ordinary advertising signboard lighting. In addition, highly-efficient LED for providing a large light flux will be used practically. Therefore, it can be expected that the results of our study will affect the outdoor advertising signboard lighting design and will contribute to energy saving much more. After the Great East Japan Earthquake occurred, it has been announced that positive activities for further energy saving are demanded. In the conventional advertising signboard lighting design, only the lighting up and out functions have been simply considered. However, sophisticated lighting design for high efficiency and energy saving will be provided for appropriately changing the signboard surface luminance depending on the visual environment illuminance by optimizing lighting-up time and season conditions. Our study will contribute to developing such future design techniques. We hope that our study will advance the energy and power saving design for the advertising signboard lighting and other lightings. References [1] Kazuo Sendai, Shu Jiang, Mamoru Takamatsu, and Yoshio Nakashima: Appearance of Object Color when the Adaptation of Eyes Shifted from Light Adaptation to Dark Adaptation. 2006 Annual Conference of the Illuminating Engineering Institute of Japan, pp.144, 2006 [2] Shu Jiang, Kazuo Sendai, Yoshitaka Hirai, Mamoru Takamatsu, and Yoshio Nakashima: Appearance of Object Color when the Adaptation of Eyes Shifted from Light Adaptation to Dark Adaptation. Proceedings of 2005 Annual Conference of the Illuminating Engineering Institute of Japan, pp. 191-192, 2005 [3] Shu Jiang, Yoshio Nakashima, Mamoru Takamatsu, and Kazuo Sendai: Object Color Perception Transition in Dark Adaptation Process. Research Conference of Japan Ergonomics Society Tokai Branch, 2006 [4] Shu Jiang, Yoshio Nakashima, Mamoru Takamatsu, and Kazuo Sendai: Study of appropriate illumination on outdoor advertising signboard. Journal of the Color Science Association of Japan: Vol. 31, Supplement, pp. 66-67, 2007 [5] Shu Jiang, Yoshio Nakashima, Mamoru Takamatsu, Kazuo Sendai, Hiroshi Sawa, and Kazuhiro Sassa: Research on Observing the Transformation of Object Color Based on the Illumination. Japan Ergonomics Society: Vol.43, Supplement, pp.96-97, 2007

IJCSNS International Journal of Computer Science and Network Security, VOL.11 No.8, August 2011 71 Kazuo Sendai received bachelor's degree in Electronics from Toyama University in 1975. He joined Matsushita Electric Works CO.,Ltd. at the same year. In 1975, he was a Technical Staff at the Department of Lighting Products Manufacturing Division Company. He is currently a Ph.D. student in the Department of Intellectual Information Systems Engineering, Toyama University. He is a member of the Illuminating Engineering Institute of Japan and the Institute of Electrical Installation Engineers of Japan and the Institute of Professional Engineers of Japan. Hiroki Fujitareceived bachelor's degree in Applied Electronics from Tokyo University of Science in 2007. He joined YKK Corporation, at the same year. In 2009, he was a Technical Staff at the Department of Intellectual Information Systems Engineering, Toyama University. He is currently a Ph.D. student in the Department of Intellectual Information Systems Engineering, Toyama University. He is a member of the Illuminating Engineering Institute of Japan and the Institute of Electrical Installation Engineers of Japan. Mamoru Takamatsu received a Ph.D. in Engineering from Toyama University in 1999. He joined the Toyama University in 2000 and has been a lecture since 2002. He is a member of the Institute of Electrical Engineers of Japan, the Optical Society of Japan, the Illuminating Engineering Institute of Japan, the Institute of Image Information and Television Engineers and the Color Science Association of Japan. Yoshio Nakashima received a Ph.D. in Physical Information Engineering from the Tokyo Institute of Technology in 1980. He joined the Toyama University in 1993 and has been a professor since 1998. He is a member of the Institute of Electrical Engineers of Japan, the Optical Society of Japan, the Illuminating Engineering Institute of Japan, the Institute of Image Information and Television Engineersand the Color Science Association of Japan.