VISUALLY INDUCED MOTION SICKNESS: AN INSIGHT FROM NEUROSCIENCE

Similar documents
COMPUTATIONAL ERGONOMICS A POSSIBLE EXTENSION OF COMPUTATIONAL NEUROSCIENCE? DEFINITIONS, POTENTIAL BENEFITS, AND A CASE STUDY ON CYBERSICKNESS

Effects of foveal retinal slip on visually induced motion sickness: a pilot study

Cybersickness, Console Video Games, & Head Mounted Displays

CAN GALVANIC VESTIBULAR STIMULATION REDUCE SIMULATOR ADAPTATION SYNDROME? University of Guelph Guelph, Ontario, Canada

Mitigating Visually Induced Motion Sickness: A Virtual Hand-Eye Coordination Task

Effects of Navigation Speed on Motion Sickness Caused by an lmmersive Virtual Environment

Multi variable strategy reduces symptoms of simulator sickness

VISUALLY INDUCED MOTION SICKNESS DURING COMPUTER GAME PLAYING

Combined Pitch and Roll and Cybersickness in a Virtual Environment

Vertical display oscillation effects on forward vection and simulator sickness

Analysis of Driver s Head Tilt Using a Mathematical Model of Motion. Sickness

Cybersickness in the presence of scene rotational movements along di!erent axes

Frequency Characteristics of Visually Induced Motion Sickness

Defense Technical Information Center Compilation Part Notice

Frequency characteristics of visually induced motion sickness

Chapter 6. Experiment 3. Motion sickness and vection with normal and blurred optokinetic stimuli

Experimental Setup of Motion Sickness and Situation Awareness in Automated Vehicle Riding Experience

COMPARING TECHNIQUES TO REDUCE SIMULATOR ADAPTATION SYNDROME AND IMPROVE NATURALISTIC BEHAVIOUR DURING SIMULATED DRIVING

Amy D. Wesley UGS Corporation Bronx, New York, USA Tina Brunetti Sayer Van Buren Township, Michigan, USA

arxiv: v1 [cs.hc] 6 Oct 2017

Postural instability precedes motion sickness

Effects of Duration of Immersion in a Virtual Reality Environment on Postural Stability

DEMOGRAPHIC AND DRIVING PERFORMANCE FACTORS IN SIMULATOR ADAPTATION SYNDROME

MANUAL CONTROL WITH TIME DELAYS IN AN IMMERSIVE VIRTUAL ENVIRONMENT

Factors Associated with Simulator Sickness in a High-Fidelity Simulator

Visually Induced Motion Sickness Predicted by Postural Instability

A Mathematical Model of Motion Sickness in 6DOF Motion and Its Application to Vehicle Passengers

Vection and cybersickness generated by head-anddisplay motion in the Oculus Rift

THE EFFECTS OF 0.2 HZ VARYING LATENCY WITH MS VARYING AMPLITUDE ON SIMULATOR SICKNESS IN A HELMET MOUNTED DISPLAY

Motion sickness issues in VR content

Motion Sickness from Combined Lateral and Roll Oscillation: Effect of Varying Phase Relationships

Vection change exacerbates simulator sickness in virtual environments

A Wearable Device Providing a Visual Fixation Point for the Alleviation of Motion Sickness Symptoms

Motion Sickness: Effect of the Magnitude of Roll and Pitch Oscillation

2/3/2016. How We Move... Ecological View. Ecological View. Ecological View. Ecological View. Ecological View. Sensory Processing.

Design and Fabrication of a System to Reduce Motion Sickness

What do I need to know about multisensory interactions?

Human Vision. Human Vision - Perception

Viewpoint Snapping to Reduce Cybersickness in Virtual Reality

The Special Senses: Vision

Lecture 4 Foundations and Cognitive Processes in Visual Perception From the Retina to the Visual Cortex

Effect of passengers active head tilt and opening/closure of eyes on motion sickness in lateral acceleration environment of cars

The relationship between postural stability and virtual environment adaptation

Vision V Perceiving Movement

Vision V Perceiving Movement

Preliminary findings on the Virtual Environment for Radiotherapy Training (VERT) system: simulator sickness and presence

A Vestibular Sensation: Probabilistic Approaches to Spatial Perception (II) Presented by Shunan Zhang

MULTIPLE EXPOSITION TO A DRIVING SIMULATOR REDUCES SIMULATOR SYMPTOMS FOR ELDERLY DRIVERS. Centre de recherche FRSQ du CHA de Québec

Psychophysiological and Performance Aspects on Motion Sickness

State of the Science Symposium

PERCEIVING MOTION CHAPTER 8

Neurovestibular/Ocular Physiology

FINAL PROJECT BEST WAYS OF PRODUCING CYBERSICKNESS IN VR

Lecture 5. The Visual Cortex. Cortical Visual Processing

Effects of Visual-Vestibular Interactions on Navigation Tasks in Virtual Environments

Visual Occlusion Decreases Motion Sickness in a Flight Simulator

Critical Significance of Human Factors in Ship Design

Chapter 8: Perceiving Motion

Simulator Sickness Questionnaire: Twenty Years Later

COGS 101A: Sensation and Perception

David Jones President, Quantified Design

Parvocellular layers (3-6) Magnocellular layers (1 & 2)

Expanding and contracting optic-flow patterns and vection

CyberPsychology and Behavior. A theory for treating visual vertigo due to optical flow. Virtual Reality and Rehabilitation

WCRR2001 ID NUMBER: 169. STUDIES ABOUT MOTION SICKNESS Effect of combined lateral and roll oscillations

Human Factors Consideration in Clinical Applications of Virtual Reality

Effect of Visual Realism on Cybersickness in Virtual Reality

An Auditory Localization and Coordinate Transform Chip

Investigation of visually induced motion sickness: a comparison of mitigation techniques in real and virtual environments

Postural stability and sex differences in visually induced motion sickness A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF UNIVERSITY OF MINNESOTA BY

Regression models for provoking motion sickness in tilting trains

iris pupil cornea ciliary muscles accommodation Retina Fovea blind spot

CORE RESEARCH AREA 1: SENSORY PHYSIOLOGY AND MOTION PERCEPTION

Chapter 2: Starting from the very beginning

HW- Finish your vision book!

Lecture IV. Sensory processing during active versus passive movements

7Motion Perception. 7 Motion Perception. 7 Computation of Visual Motion. Chapter 7

A Three-Dimensional Evaluation of Body Representation Change of Human Upper Limb Focused on Sense of Ownership and Sense of Agency

Review, the visual and oculomotor systems

TSBB15 Computer Vision

Spatial coding: scaling, magnification & sampling

Optical Illusions and Human Visual System: Can we reveal more? Imaging Science Innovative Student Micro-Grant Proposal 2011

Changes in Reaction Times and Executive Decision-Making following Exposure to Waterborne Motion

Do Stereo Display Deficiencies Affect 3D Pointing?

XOR at a Single Vertex -- Artificial Dendrites

Spatial Vision: Primary Visual Cortex (Chapter 3, part 1)

Quiz 2, Thursday, February 28 Chapter 5: orbital geometry (all the Laws for ocular motility, muscle planes) Chapter 6: muscle force mechanics- Hooke

Outline 2/21/2013. The Retina

Running head: THE INFLUENCE OF EXPECTATION ON MOTION SICKNESS 1. The Influence of Self-Motion Expectation on Motion Sickness Severity

Are There Side Effects to Watching 3D Movies? A Prospective Crossover Observational Study on Visually Induced Motion Sickness

Enhancing the Visually Induced Self-Motion Illusion (Vection) under Natural Viewing Conditions in Virtual Reality

Cortical sensory systems

The eye* The eye is a slightly asymmetrical globe, about an inch in diameter. The front part of the eye (the part you see in the mirror) includes:

Accelerating self-motion displays produce more compelling vection in depth

Virtual Guiding Avatar: An Effective Procedure to Reduce Simulator Sickness in Virtual Environments

MECHANICAL DESIGN LEARNING ENVIRONMENTS BASED ON VIRTUAL REALITY TECHNOLOGIES

THE EFFECT OF 0.2 HZ AND 1.0 HZ FREQUENCY AND 100 MS AND MS AMPLITUDE OF LATENCY ON SIMULATORY SICKNESS IN A HEAD MOUNTED DISPLAY

Sensation and Perception

The Effects of Filters on Colour Vision

The shape of luminance increments at the intersection alters the magnitude of the scintillating grid illusion

Transcription:

VISUALLY INDUCED MOTION SICKNESS: AN INSIGHT FROM NEUROSCIENCE Ji, Jennifer Ting Ting Department of Industrial Engineering and Engineering Management / Hong Kong University of Science & Technology / Clear Water Bay/ Kowloon, Hong Kong SAR, PRC +852 2358 8637 / ieemjtt@ust.hk So, Richard Hau Yue Department of Industrial Engineering and Engineering Management / Hong Kong University of Science & Technology / Clear Water Bay/ Kowloon, Hong Kong SAR, PRC +852 2358 7105 / rhyso@ust.hk ABSTRACT Most empirical studies on visually induced motion sickness (VIMS) have cited the sensory rearrangement theory to explain the generation of VIMS. However, since 1990, there has been a surge of competing theories for VIMS (e.g., postural stability theory; nystagmus theory; and subjective vertical conflict theory). This has sparked off much intellectual debate on the possible mechanisms and etiologies of VIMS. On the other hands, neuroscience experiments have advanced our understanding of the neural mechanisms behind vection perception and vertigo sensation. This paper seeks to identify the possible neural pathways concerning the etiologies of VIMS. Keywords Motion sickness, cybersickness, neuro-ergonomics, computational ergonomics INTRODUCTION Visually induced motion sickness Lo and So (2001) reviewed 37 empirical studies on VIMS caused by viewing virtual reality displays published in or before 1999. Those 37 studies examined effects of 17 independent variables including variables related to the content of VR simulation (e.g., types of scene background and scene movement), variables related to how the VR simulation is being presented (e.g., exposure duration, types of VR display, display s field-of-view, image delays, use of stereoscopic, inter-pupillary-distance mismatch, method of navigation), variables related to the physical movement of participants (e.g., postures during simulation, amounts of head movement during simulation), and variables related to individual characteristics (e.g., age, gender, pre-exposure posture stability, habituation, menstrual phase, drug treatment). A further review of the literature indicates that between 1999 and 2005, another 36 empirical studies were

published. Given the ample amount of published empirical data, the authors are convinced that the timing is appropriate to determine the fundamental mechanism responsible for generating VIMS in human. Visually induced motion sickness (VIMS) has been the subject of many empirical studies in the past 100 years. As the display technology evolved over the years, the apparatus used to produce the visual stimuli also changed (telescope: Stratton, 1896, 1897; rotating drums: Stern et al., 1985, 1989, 1993; simulators: Kennedy and Frank, 1995, Kennedy and Massey, 1995, Kennedy et al., 1989, 1995; virtual reality displays: Ji et al., 2004; Lo and So, 2001, McCauley and Sharkey, 1992, Stanney et al., 1998, Stoffregen and Smart, 1998, and Wilson, 1996). Although it has been reported that the profiles of symptoms can change as the display technology changes (e.g., Kennedy et al., 1997), most empirical studies on VIMS have cited the sensory rearrangement theory (Reason, 1967; Reason and Brand, 1975) to explain the generation of VIMS. Since 1990, there has been a surge of competing theories for VIMS (e.g., reflexresponse theory: Griffin, 1990; postural stability theory: Riccio and Stoffregen, 1992; nystagmus theory: Ebenhotz et al., 1994; and subjective vertical conflict theory: Bles et al., 1998). This has sparked off much intellectual debate on the possible mechanisms and etiologies of VIMS. A review of literature indicates that there are conflicting empirical findings. For examples, Stern et al. (1990) and Flanagan and May et al. (2002) reported an association between significant reductions of sickness symptoms with significant reductions of circular vection ratings. However, both Webb and Griffin (2002, 2003) reported that circular vection ratings were not correlated with sickness ratings. Instead, they were correlated with eye-movements. The former finding is more consistent with the sensory rearrangement theory while the latter is more consistent with the nystagmus theory. One obvious way to resolve these conflicts is to conduct more empirical studies. This paper, however, proposes an additional approach that can go hand-in-hand with the empirical study approach. With the recent advances in our understanding of human brain functions, the authors propose to utilize the knowledge accumulated in the field of neuroscience to hypothesize a set of possible neural pathways leading to the generation of VIMS. These hypothetical pathways can then be tested with future empirical studies. Advances in neuroscience A review of literature indicates that there has been much development in (i) signal processing models to account for sensory rearrangement and visual-vestibular interactions (e.g., Oman, 1982, 1990, 1991, 1998; Zupan et al., 2002; and Telban et al., 2001, 2002) as well as (ii) understanding of the neuro-mechanism of self-motion perceptions, vertigo, and motion sickness (e.g., Brandt, 1999; Lappe, 2000; Van Essen et al., 1992; Takedo et al., 2001). The understanding of human brain functions provides an opportunity to identify hypothetical neural pathways to explain the generation of VIMS (Ji et al., 2005). The benefits of determining hypothetical neural pathways of VIMS Once the hypothetical neural pathways of VIMS are established, software or hardware simulation can be developed to simulate the functions of those pathways. The simulated results can then be compared to empirical results so that the pathways themselves can be tested and verified. This process is consistent with the concept of studying an ergonomics process by building an ergonomics process proposed in So and Lor (2004) and referred to as computational ergonomics approach. Under this approach, empirical studies are tools to refine a biological inspired algorithm or hardware that simulates an ergonomics process until the simulation systems can accurately simulate the corresponding ergonomics process.

To further illustrate the benefits of studying an ergonomics process by building an ergonomics process. Let us use an imaginative example. If scientists who had been studying simulator sickness in the 80 s also developed a biological inspired computational algorithm that could simulate the generation of simulator sickness, when VIMS caused by viewing virtual reality displays (i.e., cybersickness) became a cause for concerns, scientists can refine or modify the algorithm developed for simulator sickness to simulate the generation of cybersickness. As this algorithm is biologically inspired, it should have a functional structure similar to the real biological process of cybersickness generation. In other words, the algorithm should reflect the true neural mechanism behind the generation of cybersickness. Furthermore, scientists from all over the world can work on the same algorithm and shared their refined codes via the Internet. In 2005, the authors were funded by the Hong Kong Research Grants Council to develop a biological inspired algorithm to simulate the VIMS produced after viewing virtual reality displays. This paper presents selected preliminary findings of this study. THE ROLE OF VECTION IN VIMS: AN INSIGHT FROM NEUROSCIENCE As mentioned in the introduction section, there have been conflicting reports on the relationships between vection sensation and VIMS (e.g., Stern et al., 1990; Flanagan and May et al., 2002; Webb and Griffin, 2002, 2003). Establishing an hypothetical neural pathway for vection perception and nausea symptoms can be an important guide to future studies in this area. A review of literature indicates that the magnocellular pathways within the cells in the retina are responsible for the perception of visual motion (Kandel, 1991). The m-type ganglion cells along the pathways are less sensitive to color information and visual patterns of high spatial frequencies. The insensitive to visual color information is consistent with the empirical findings that change of color of a virtual reality simulation did not significantly change the rated levels of nausea ratings and scores of simulator sickness questionnaire (Yuen, 2001). The magnocellular pathway projects onto the primary visual cortex (V1 area) via the thalamus. Cells in V1 area process the visual information within their respective field-of-views (known as receptive fields) according to the spatial frequency characteristics, orientation of spatial patterns, as well as the temporal movements of these patterns. The processed signals are then fed to the middle temporal area (MT area or V5 area) (Rolls and Deco, 2002) and the parietooccipatal area PO(V6) (Brandt, 1999). Both MT area and the area PO(V6) project signals to the medial superior temporal area (MST area) (Rolls and Deco, 2002). Longstaff (2000) reported that patients with lesions in area V5 could lose their ability to perceive motion and, hence, vection. At the MST area, signal representing the global optic flow are formed and passed to the parietal-insular vestibular cortex (PIVC) area via the inferior parietal area 7a. It has been known that the area PO(V6) and the PIVC are related to the visually induced circular vection and the vestibular-induced circular vection respectively and reciprocally (Brandt, 1999). A review of literature shows that nausea sensation can be triggered by signal projects from the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and vomiting actions are associated with nucleus tractus solitarius (NTS) and lateral medullary reticular formation (LMRF) in the brain stem (Miller et al., 1996; Yates et al., 1998). However, no direct neural pathway was found between area PO(V6) and these three areas (IFG, NTS, and LMRF). Indirect

pathways were found via area V6A, inferior parietal area 7a, PIVC, vestibular nuclei, and thalamus. Although the PIVC is not oxygenated during visually induced circular vection (Brandt, 1999), it is not clear whether the reciprocal actions between PIVC and PO(V6) can play a push-pull role in linking the visually-induced circular vection associated with area PO(V6) and the nausea sensation associated with the inferior frontal gyrus. FINAL REMARKS This paper presents selected preliminary findings of a study to identify the possible neural pathways for VIMS. So far, the identified neural pathways support that vection is generated in association with sickness symptoms of VIMS but does not have a clear evidence for a direct cause-and-effect link between the vection signals associated with the parieto-occipital area PO(V6) and nausea and vomiting signals associated with the inferior frontal gyrus, the nucleus tractus solitarius, and the lateral medullary reticular formation, respectively. At the conference, the pathways concerning the generation of optokinetic nystagmus and their relationships with vomiting and other sickness symptoms of VIMS will also be presented. In addition, the interior structure and the role of vestibular nuclei on VIMS will also be discussed. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The authors would like to thank the Hong Kong Research Grant Council for the earmarked grant HKUST6154/04E. REFERENCES Bles, W.; Bos. J.E.; De Graaf, B.; Groen, E.; Wertheim, A.H. (1998) Motion sickness: only one provocative conflict? Brain Res. Bull. 47: pp.481-487. Brandt, T. (1999) Vertigo: its multisensory syndromes. Springer (2nd edition). Ebenholtz, S.M.; Cohen, M.M. and Linder, B.J. (1994) The possible role of nystagmus in motion sickness: a hypothesis. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 65, pp.1032-5. Flanagan, M.B.; May, J.G. and Dobie, T.G. (2002) Optokinetic nystagmus, vection, and motion sickness. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 73, pp.1067-1073. Griffin, M.J. (1990) Handbook of human vibration. Academic press. Ji, J.; So, R.H.Y.; Lor, F.; Cheung, R.T.F.; Howarth, P. and Stanney, K. (2005) A search for possible neural pathways leading to visually induced motion sickness. Vision, 17: pp.131-134. Ji, J.T.T., Lor, F.W.K. and So. R.H.Y. (2004) Integrating a computational model of optical flow into the cybersickness dose value prediction model. Proceedings of the 48th annual meeting of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 20-24 September, New Orleans, LA, USA. Kandel, E.R. (1991) Perception of motion, depth, and form. Chapter 30 in: Principles of Neural Science by Kanpel, E.R., Schwartz, J.H. and Jessell, T.M. Kennedy, R.S. and Frank, L.H. (1985) A review of motion sickness with special reference to simulator sickness. Technical report TA1001.5, August 1985, Transportation Research Board, National research Council, pp. 1-70. Kennedy, R.S. and Massey, C.J. (1995) Incidences of Fatigue and Drowsiness Reports from Three Dozen Simulators: Relevance for the sopite syndrome. Proceedings of the first workshop on simulation and interaction in virtual environments, 13-15 July,

University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA, USA. Kennedy, R.S.; Drexler, J.M.; Lanham, D.S. and Massey, C.J. (1995) Gender differences in simulator sickness incidence: Implications for military virtual reality systems. Safe Journal, 25, pp. 69-76. Kennedy, R.S.; Lanham, S. and Drexler, J.M. et al. (1997) A comparison of incidences, symptom profiles, measurement techniques and suggestions for research. Presence, 6, pp. 638-644. Kennedy, R.S.; Lilienthal, M.G.; Berbaum, K.S.; Baltzley, D.R. and McCauley, M.E. (1989) Simulator sickness in U.S. Navy fight simulators. Aviation, Space, and Environment Medicine, 60, p.106. Lappe, M. (2000) Computational mechanisms for optic flow analysis in primate cortex. Int.Rev. Neurobiology. 235-68. Lo, W.T. and So, R.H.Y. (2001) Cybersickness in the presence of scene rotational movements along different axes. App. Erg. 32, 1-14. Longstaff, A. (2000) Eye and visual pathway in vision. In: neuroscience BIOS Scientific, pp. 138-140. McCauley, M.E. and Sharkey, T.J. (1992) Cybersickness: perception of self-motion in VEs. PRESENCE, 1, 311-318. Miller, A.D.; Rowley, H.A.; Roberts, T.P.L.; Kucharczyk, J. (1996) Human cortical activity during vestibular- and drug-induced nausea detected using MSI. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 781: pp.670-672. Oman, C.M. (1982) A herristic mathematical model for the dynamic of sensory conflict and motion sickness. Acta Oto. Supp. 392, pp.6-44. Oman,C.M.(1990) Motion sickness: a synthesis and evaluation of the sensory conflict theory. Can. J. of Phys and Pharm. 68:294-03. Oman, C.M. (1991) Sensory conflict in motion sickness: an Observer Theory approach, chapter 24, Pictorial Communication in Virtual and Real Environments, 362-367, Taylor & Francis, London. Oman, C.M. (1998) Sensory Conflict Theory and Space Sickness: Our Changing Perspective, J. of Vestibular Research, 8, pp.51-56. Reason, J.T. (1967) Relationships between motion sickness susceptibility, motion aftereffects, and receptivity. Ph.D. thesis. University of Leicester, England. Reason, J.T. and Brand, J.J. (1975) Motion sickness. Academic press. Riccio, G.E. and Stoffregen, T.A. (1991) An ecological theory of Motion Sickness and Postural Instability. Ecological psychology, 3, pp.195-240. Rolls, E.T. and Deco, G. (2002) Computational neuroscience of vision. Oxford University press. So, R.H.Y., Finney, C.M. and Goonetilleke, R.S. (1999) Motion sickness susceptibility and occurrence in Hong Kong Chinese. Contemporary Ergonomics 1999, Taylor & Francis. Stanney, K.M. and Salvendy, G. et al. (1998) Aftereffects and sense of presence in virtual environments: formulation of a research and development agenda. Int. J of HCI, 10, pp. 135-187. Stern, R.M.; Hu, S.; Leblanc, B.S. and Koch, K.L. (1993) Chinese hyper-susceptibility to vection-induced motion sickness. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine. Sept. pp. 827-832. Stern, R.M.; Hu,S.; Vasey, M.W. and Koch, K.L. (1989) Adaptation to vection-induced sympyoms of motion sickness. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine.60: pp 566-72.

Stern, R.M.; Koch,K.L.; Leibowitz, H.W.; Shupart, C.L. and Stewart, W.R. (1985) tachygastria and motion sickness. Aviation, Space and Environmental Medicine. 56, pp. 1074-1077. Stratton, G.M. (1897) Vision without inversion of the retinal image. Psychol. Rev., 4, pp.341-346. Webb, N.A. and Griffin, M.J. (2002) Optokinetic stimuli: motion sickness, visual acuity, and eye movements. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 73, pp.351-8. Webb, N.A. and Griffin, M.J. (2003) Eye movement, vection, and motion sickness with foveal and peripheral vision. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 73, pp.622-5. Takeda, N. et al. (2001) Neural mechanisms of motion sickness. J. Med. Invest. 48, 44-59. Telban, R.J. and Cardullo, F.M. (2001) An Integrated Model of Human Motion Perception with Visual-Vestibular Interaction. AIAA Mod. Simu. Tech.Conf. and Exhibit, Montreal, Canada, Aug 6-9, 2001. Telban, R.J.; Cardullo, F.M. and Houck, J.A. (2002) A Nonlinear, Human-Centered Approach to Motioin Cueing with a Neurocomputing Solver. AIAA Mod. Simu.Tech. Conf. and Exhibit, Monterey, Canada, Aug 5-8, 2002 Van Essen, D.C.; Anderson, C.H. and Felleman, D.J. (1992) Information Processing in the Primate Visual System: An Integrated Syst.Perspective. Sci., New Series. 255: 419-423. Wilson, J.R. (1996) Effects of participating in virtual environments: a review of current knowledge. Safety Science, 23, pp. 39-51. Yates B.J.; Miller A.D. and Lucot J.B. (1998) Physiological Basis and Pharmacology of Motion Sickness: an Update, Brain Res. Bulletin, 47(5), 395-406. Yuen, S.L. (2002) Effects of scene complexity in virtual environments on levels of cybersickness. MPhil thesis, HKUST Zupal, L.H.; Merfeld, D.M. and Darlot (2002) Using sensory weighting to model the influence of canal, otolith, and visual cues on spatial orientation and eye movements. Biol. Cybern, 86, 209-230.