A Three-Channel Model for Generating the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex in Each Eye

Similar documents
Foveal Versus Full-Field Visual Stabilization Strategies for Translational and Rotational Head Movements

A Vestibular Sensation: Probabilistic Approaches to Spatial Perception (II) Presented by Shunan Zhang

Lecture IV. Sensory processing during active versus passive movements

Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion

An Analog VLSI Model of Adaptation in the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex

Experiment HM-2: Electroculogram Activity (EOG)

Quiz 2, Thursday, February 28 Chapter 5: orbital geometry (all the Laws for ocular motility, muscle planes) Chapter 6: muscle force mechanics- Hooke

The Persistence of Vision in Spatio-Temporal Illusory Contours formed by Dynamically-Changing LED Arrays

5R01EY Page 1 of 1. Progress Report Scanning Cover Sheet. PI Name: Org: Start Date: Snap:

Signal Processing of Semicircular Canal and Otolith Signals in the Vestibular Nuclei during Passive and Active Head Movements

Contribution of Head Movement to Gaze Command Coding in Monkey Frontal Cortex and Superior Colliculus

Rotational Vestibular Chair

Lecture 4 Foundations and Cognitive Processes in Visual Perception From the Retina to the Visual Cortex

Interference in stimuli employed to assess masking by substitution. Bernt Christian Skottun. Ullevaalsalleen 4C Oslo. Norway

Differential Processing of Semicircular Canal Signals in the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex

TSBB15 Computer Vision

How Actions Alter Sensory Processing

The attenuation of perceived motion smear during combined eye and head movements

NOISE REDUCTION IN SCREW COMPRESSORS BY THE CONTROL OF ROTOR TRANSMISSION ERROR

Chapter 73. Two-Stroke Apparent Motion. George Mather

Experiments on the locus of induced motion

Where s the Floor? L. R. Harris 1,2,, M. R. M. Jenkin 1,3, H. L. M. Jenkin 1,2, R. T. Dyde 1 and C. M. Oman 4

Appendix C: Graphing. How do I plot data and uncertainties? Another technique that makes data analysis easier is to record all your data in a table.

VISUAL-VESTIBULAR INTERACTION DURING OFF-VERTICAL AXIS ROTATION

Sixth Quarterly Progress Report

CLINICAL SCIENCES. Effects of Earth-Fixed vs Head-Fixed Targets on Static Ocular Counterroll

VISUAL VESTIBULAR INTERACTIONS FOR SELF MOTION ESTIMATION

Lecture 3 Complex Exponential Signals

Rapid Motor Learning in the Translational Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex

Psych 333, Winter 2008, Instructor Boynton, Exam 1

IOC, Vector sum, and squaring: three different motion effects or one?

A Novel Galvanic Vestibular Stimulation Based Navigator For The Blind

Anticipatory eye movements stabilize gaze during self-generated head movements

Measurement of oscillopsia induced by vestibular Coriolis stimulation

Advancing Simulation as a Safety Research Tool

Insights into High-level Visual Perception

TAKING A WALK IN THE NEUROSCIENCE LABORATORIES

Spatial Judgments from Different Vantage Points: A Different Perspective

The Neuronal Basis of Visual Self-motion Estimation

Interventions for vision impairments post brain injury: Use of prisms and exercises. Dr Kevin Houston Talia Mouldovan

Behavioural Realism as a metric of Presence

Computation of Inertial Motion: Neural Strategies to Resolve Ambiguous Otolith Information

Eighth Quarterly Progress Report

The role of sensory systems in directional perception of the fiddler crab, Uca pugilator

MULTIPLE INPUT MULTIPLE OUTPUT (MIMO) VIBRATION CONTROL SYSTEM

AC phase. Resources and methods for learning about these subjects (list a few here, in preparation for your research):

Discrimination of Virtual Haptic Textures Rendered with Different Update Rates

Goals. Introduction. To understand the use of root mean square (rms) voltages and currents.

SHORT COMMUNICATION INTRACELLULAR RECORDINGS FROM INTACT LOCUSTS FLYING UNDER CLOSED-LOOP VISUAL CONDITIONS

A new test bench for examinations of the pantograph-catenary interaction

A Fraser illusion without local cues?

From Neuroscience to Mechatronics

Journal of Vestibular Research, Vol. 2, pp , 1992 Printed in the USA. All rights reserved.

State of the Science Symposium

VNG/ENG. To Preserve and Improve Balance VISUALEYES BINOCULAR GOGGLES

Experiment 8: An AC Circuit

Adaptation of the vestibulo-ocular reflex, subjective tilt, and motion sickness to head movements during short-radius centrifugation

In an unmagnetized piece of iron, the atoms are arranged in domains. In each domain the atoms are aligned, but the domains themselves are random.

Visual perception of motion in depth: Application ofa vector model to three-dot motion patterns*

Sensing self motion. Key points: Why robots need self-sensing Sensors for proprioception in biological systems in robot systems

Detection of external stimuli Response to the stimuli Transmission of the response to the brain

The Shape-Weight Illusion

Determining the effectiveness of a vibrotactile balance prosthesis

Takeharu Seno 1,3,4, Akiyoshi Kitaoka 2, Stephen Palmisano 5 1

MULTI-LAYERED HYBRID ARCHITECTURE TO SOLVE COMPLEX TASKS OF AN AUTONOMOUS MOBILE ROBOT

Vestibular cues and virtual environments: choosing the magnitude of the vestibular cue Laurence Harris 1;3 Michael Jenkin 2;3 Daniel C. Zikovitz 3 Dep

Motion Perception II Chapter 8

Supporting Online Material for

Magnetic sensor signal analysis by means of the image processing technique

Predictive Gaze Stabilization During Periodic Locomotion Based On Adaptive Frequency Oscillators

SCATTERING POLARIMETRY PART 1. Dr. A. Bhattacharya (Slide courtesy Prof. E. Pottier and Prof. L. Ferro-Famil)

(12) Patent Application Publication (10) Pub. No.: US 2011/ A1

binocular projection by electrophysiological methods. An account of some METHODS

Eccentric gaze dynamics enhance vection in depth

THE SINUSOIDAL WAVEFORM

Sensation and Perception. What We Will Cover in This Section. Sensation

Perceiving a stable world during active rotational and translational head movements

8.2 IMAGE PROCESSING VERSUS IMAGE ANALYSIS Image processing: The collection of routines and

Does the Middle Temporal Area Carry Vestibular Signals Related to Self-Motion?

An Auditory Localization and Coordinate Transform Chip

6.1 - Introduction to Periodic Functions

CHAPTER 5 CONCEPTS OF ALTERNATING CURRENT

Maps in the Brain Introduction

APP NOTE. Acceleration Sensitivity Characteristics of Quartz Crystal Oscillators

EMMA Software Quick Start Guide

The Haptic Perception of Spatial Orientations studied with an Haptic Display

Polarization Experiments Using Jones Calculus

Introduction to DSP ECE-S352 Fall Quarter 2000 Matlab Project 1

Thirteenth Quarterly Progress Report

ENGINEERING GRAPHICS ESSENTIALS

Assessing the perceptual consequences of non Earth environments

SEE++: A Biomechanical Model of the Oculomotor Plant

Chapter 8: Perceiving Motion

Binaural hearing. Prof. Dan Tollin on the Hearing Throne, Oldenburg Hearing Garden

ORTHOGRAPHIC PROJECTIONS. Ms. Sicola

How to define Graph in HDSME

Visual Rules. Why are they necessary?

Using Line and Ellipse Features for Rectification of Broadcast Hockey Video

Concentric Spatial Maps for Neural Network Based Navigation

ensory System III Eye Reflexes

Transcription:

A Three-Channel Model for Generating the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex in Each Eye LAURENCE R. HARRIS, a KARL A. BEYKIRCH, b AND MICHAEL FETTER c a Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, Canada b Department of Neurology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany c Department of Neurologie, Klinikum Karlsbad-Langensteinbach, Karlsbad-Langensteinbach, Germany KEYWORDS: vestibulo-ocular reflex; three-dimensional eye movements; channels; passive rotation Coding head movement involves representing the head s velocity and axis of rotation. The neural representation can then be used to inform perceptual and motor processes. An important motor response to head movement is the compensatory eye movements evoked, one component of which is the vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR). Historically a three-neuron arc has been described as the core of the neural mechanism underlying the generation of the VOR. 1,2 Such a direct line between sensor (the canals) and effector (the eye muscles) implies independent processing of the geometric components of the three-dimensional VOR. 3 A more flexible and robust representation of the movement involves an interactive process in which the activity coding movement in each direction is interpreted in the context of the activity of the others. Many sensory attributes are coded by the activity of a small set of channels, 4 and the closely constrained three-dimensional movement of the head could be efficiently represented by such a system. Psychophysical methods have been developed to investigate channel systems among which is adaptation. After adapting the response to a particular stimulus, the effect on the responses to closely related stimuli can often reveal a channel-coding system. 5,6 Here we use an adaptation technique to provide evidence for a three-channel model underlying the representation of head rotation and generating the vestibulo-ocular reflex of each eye. These channels are conceptually different from those proposed for coding head velocity ranges, 7 as discussed elsewhere. 8 Address for correspondence: Laurence R. Harris, Department of Psychology, York University, 4700 Keele Street, Toronto, Ontario M3J 1P3, Canada. Voice: 416-736-2100, ext. 66108; fax: 416-736-5814. harris@yorku.ca Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 956: 537 542 (2002). 2002 New York Academy of Sciences. 537

538 ANNALS NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES FIGURE 1. The VOR measured before and after adaptation. A shows the axes tested and the Tübingen Vestibular Testing Facility. B-D show the amplitude and orientation in the horizontal plane of the slow-phase component of the VOR evoked in response to each of three frequencies of rotation around four axes in the horizontal plane before and after adaptation around the axis shown by the fat black arrow. The top row plots the amplitude and direction of the responses before (solid circles) and after (open circles) adaptation plotted on a linear scale. The lines are best-fit sines. The next row plots the best-fit sines in polar coordinates. The bottom row plots the differences between the conditions before and after. The largest effects are around the roll axis, some 45 degrees from the adapted axis.

HARRIS et al.: CHANNEL MODEL OF THE VOR 539 METHODS The experiment was run in two sessions separated by at least two days. The first session evaluated the eye movements evoked by rotation around eight test axes. Using the Tübingen Vestibular Research Stimulator and starting from upright, we rotated seven subjects in yaw, pitch, roll and axes 45 degrees in between (FIG. 1A) in the dark and measured the evoked eye movements with 3-D scleral search coils on the left eye. We used 100 sec of sum-of-sines (0.032, 0.065, 0.13, 0.26 Hz, ± 20 degrees). In the second session, subjects were adapted to subject-stationary vision viewed at 1 m during physical rotation (0.13 Hz; ± 100 degrees) in the plane of the right-anterior, left-posterior (RALP) canal-pair. Under these conditions the vestibulo-ocular reflex is suppressed 9 and the reduced gain persists when performance is measured subsequently in darkness. 10 After the 30-minute adaptation procedure, eye coils were placed on the left eye, and the VOR evoked by rotation about the test axes measured in the dark. The data analysis method has been described elsewhere. 11 RESULTS The slow-phase component of the VOR induced by rotation before adaptation was not always aligned with the stimulating axis and showed a variation in amplitude and deviation that varied from axis to axis 11 (FIG. 1B-D [solid circle and solid lines] and FIG. 2 [ pre ]). After visually driven VOR gain reduction by stabilized vision around the RALP axis, the response in the dark to rotation around the adapting axis (indicated by thick arrows in FIG. 1) was reduced. However, changes in both amplitude and alignment were also seen in response to rotation around other axes, most notably roll. The differences between the before and after conditions are illustrated in the bottom row of FIG. 1 B-D: clearly the largest effect is in response to roll rotation. Notice that the response to pitch rotation, which, like roll, was only 45 degrees away from the adaptation axis, was not significantly altered. MODELING The data were modeled with a three-channel model to code the orientation and velocity of head rotation. Each of the three channels of this model represents a component of the movement. They are not based on any anatomical features but are proposed simply as an information-processing system. The head rotation is represented as a three-dimensional vector of a length representing velocity. This vector is then projected orthogonally onto each of the channels. Each channel has a gain by which the projected component is multiplied and an orientation. To recover the head movement, the activity in the three channels are taken as the coordinates. The output is the vector sum of the three channels activity. In order to model the VOR before any adaptation, the orientations and gains of the three channels were configured as free variables and the output of the model, with a random initial configuration, was compared to the actual response for each subject around each axis. Gain and orientations were varied systematically and the process repeated until the output of the model best matched the data. The orientation

540 ANNALS NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES FIGURE 2. Simulating the VOR for the left eye. The orientation and relative length of the hypothetical channels are shown in the center column seen from the side (top row), back (middle row) and above (bottom row). For clarity the results are divided into those obtained from rotation around axes in the sagittal plane and the horizontal plane. The average data are shown with standard deviations as symbols (use FIG. 1A as key). Superimposed are the output of the three-channel model for the pre- and post-adaptation conditions (large open circles). All the major features of the data are reproduced by this model.

HARRIS et al.: CHANNEL MODEL OF THE VOR 541 and gains of the set of three channels that optimally reproduced all the major features of the pre-adapt VOR, are shown in FIGURE 2. The prediction of this model of the response to the eight axes used are shown as large open symbols on either side in the columns labelled pre. There is a good correspondence. Next the model was adapted. First the relative hypothetical activity of each of the channels expected in response to the physical motion component of the adapting stimulus was calculated given the gains and orientations of the channels obtained from the best fit to the pre-adapted data.the relative gains of each channel were then adjusted by an amount proportional to their relative activity during the adaptation experience to produce an adapted model. The response of the adapted model was simulated by projecting each head rotation onto the adapted channels (FIG. 2). The output of the adapted model showed an excellent fit to the adapted data, reproducing all the major features. The properties of the proposed channels (FIG. 2, center) do not correspond to canal or roll/pitch/yaw coordinates. They are far away from the planes of individual canals, with one close to roll and the others forming an X approximately in Listing s plane,which can be expected to be tilted outwards since the eyes were likely verged in dark at a distance roughly corresponding to the screen. 12 A separate set of channels is needed for each eye since they are not in a plane orthogonal to the straight ahead but are instead tilted outwards by about 20 degrees. This study shows that the vestibulo-ocular reflex can be elegantly modeled by a three-channel system. The proposed location of the channels correspond to emerging studies indicating a neural coordinate system involving an axis close to roll 13 and Listing s plane. 14 16 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This work was supported by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft; Natural Science & Engineering Council (NSERC), Canada; and the Centre for Research in Earth and Space Technology (CRESTech), Ontario. REFERENCES 1. LORENTE DE NÓ, R. 1933. Vestibulo-ocular reflex arc. Ann. Neurol. Psychiat. 30: 245 291. 2. SZENTÁGOTHAI, J. 1950. The elementary vestibulo-ocular reflex arc. J. Neurophysiol. 13: 395 407. 3. VILIS, T. & D. TWEED.1988. A matrix analysis for a conjugate vestibulo-ocular reflex. Biol. Cybernet. 59: 237 245. 4. BLUM, B. 1991. Channels in the Visual Nervous System: Neurophysiology, Psychophysics and Models. Freund. London. 5. CAMPBELL, F.W. & R.W. TEGEDER. 1991. A survey of channels and challenges, of information and meaning. In Channels in the Visual Nervous System: Neurophysiology, Psychophysics and Models. B. Blum, Ed.: 1 10. Freund. London. 6. GRAHAM, N. 1989. Visual Pattern Analyzers. Oxford University Press. Oxford. 7. LISBERGER, S.G., F.A. MILEs, et al. 1983. Frequency-selective adaptation: evidence for channels in the vestibulo-ocular reflex? J. Neurosci. 3: 1234 1244. 8. HARRIS, L.R. 1997. The coding of self motion. In Computational and Psychophysical Mechanisms of Visual Coding. L.R. Harris & M. Jenkin, Eds.: 157 183. Cambridge University Press. Cambridge.

542 ANNALS NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES 9. BARNES, G.R. 1982. Visual factors affecting suppression of the vestibulo-ocular reflex. In Functional Basis of Ocular Motility Disorders. G. Lennerstrand et al. Eds.: 387 389. Pergamon. Oxford and New York. 10. BERTHOZ, A. & G. MELVILL JONES. 1985. Adaptive Mechanisms in Gaze Control. Elsevier. New York. 11. HARRIS, L.R. & K. BEYKIRCH, et al. 2001. The visual consequences of deviations in the orientation of the axis of rotation of the human vestibulo-ocular reflex. Vision Res. 41: 3271 3281. 12. MOK, D. & A. RO, et al. 1992. Rotation of Listing s plane during vergence. Vision Res. 32: 2055 2064. 13. CRAWFORD, J.D. & W. CADERA, et al. 1991. Generation of torsional and vertical eye position signals by the interstitial nucleus of Cajal. Science 252: 1551 1553. 14. CRAWFORD, J.D. & T. VILIS. 1991. Axes of eye rotation and Listing s law during rotations of the head. J. Neurophysiol. 65: 407 423. 15. SMITH, M.A. & J.D. CRAWFORD. 1998. Neural control of rotational kinematics within realistic vestibuloocular coordinate systems. J. Neurophysiol. 80: 2295 2315. 16. SMITH, M.A. & J.D. CRAWFORD. 2001. Self-organizing task modules and explicit coordinate systems in a neural network model for 3-D saccades. J. Comput. Neurosci. 10: 127 150.