The Haptic Perception of Spatial Orientations studied with an Haptic Display

Similar documents
Role of gravitational cues in the haptic perception of orientation

Haptic perception of spatial relations

Influence of Shape Elements on Performance during Haptic Rotation

Egocentric reference frame bias in the palmar haptic perception of surface orientation. Allison Coleman and Frank H. Durgin. Swarthmore College

The influence of exploration mode, orientation, and configuration on the haptic Mu«ller-Lyer illusion

Here I present more details about the methods of the experiments which are. described in the main text, and describe two additional examinations which

Path completion after haptic exploration without vision: Implications for haptic spatial representations

Spatial Judgments from Different Vantage Points: A Different Perspective

Muscular Torque Can Explain Biases in Haptic Length Perception: A Model Study on the Radial-Tangential Illusion

The Shape-Weight Illusion

Haptic perception of linear extent

A triangulation method for determining the perceptual center of the head for auditory stimuli

Haptic control in a virtual environment

The Mona Lisa Effect: Perception of Gaze Direction in Real and Pictured Faces

The influence of changing haptic refresh-rate on subjective user experiences - lessons for effective touchbased applications.

Perception of Haptic Force Magnitude during Hand Movements

Discrimination of Virtual Haptic Textures Rendered with Different Update Rates

The Bourdon illusion in haptic space

THE POGGENDORFF ILLUSION WITH ANOMALOUS SURFACES: MANAGING PAC-MANS, PARALLELS LENGTH AND TYPE OF TRANSVERSAL.

Journal of Experimental Psychology

THE INTERACTION BETWEEN HEAD-TRACKER LATENCY, SOURCE DURATION, AND RESPONSE TIME IN THE LOCALIZATION OF VIRTUAL SOUND SOURCES

Representing Spatial Location and Layout From Sparse Kinesthetic Contacts

The Haptic Impendance Control through Virtual Environment Force Compensation

Evaluation of Five-finger Haptic Communication with Network Delay

A Three-Dimensional Evaluation of Body Representation Change of Human Upper Limb Focused on Sense of Ownership and Sense of Agency

Balance and Tension 1

Acta Psychologica 134 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Acta Psychologica. journal homepage:

Analysis of Frontal Localization in Double Layered Loudspeaker Array System

AGING AND STEERING CONTROL UNDER REDUCED VISIBILITY CONDITIONS. Wichita State University, Wichita, Kansas, USA

Attenuating the haptic horizontal vertical curvature illusion

THE POGGENDORFF ILLUSION: THE PRESENCE OF ANOMALOUS FIGURE IN GENERATING THE EFFECT. Department of General Psychology, University of Padua, Italy

NAVIGATIONAL CONTROL EFFECT ON REPRESENTING VIRTUAL ENVIRONMENTS

HRIR Customization in the Median Plane via Principal Components Analysis

Haptic Space Processing Allocentric and Egocentric Reference Frames

Can a haptic force feedback display provide visually impaired people with useful information about texture roughness and 3D form of virtual objects?

Visual perception of motion in depth: Application ofa vector model to three-dot motion patterns*

Virtual Sound Source Positioning and Mixing in 5.1 Implementation on the Real-Time System Genesis

Development of a Finger Mounted Type Haptic Device Using a Plane Approximated to Tangent Plane

Salient features make a search easy

Haptic Camera Manipulation: Extending the Camera In Hand Metaphor

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

Perception of room size and the ability of self localization in a virtual environment. Loudspeaker experiment

Running an HCI Experiment in Multiple Parallel Universes

Spatial Low Pass Filters for Pin Actuated Tactile Displays

Perception of the horizontal and vertical in tangible displays: minimal gender differences

Thresholds for Dynamic Changes in a Rotary Switch

Scene layout from ground contact, occlusion, and motion parallax

Evaluating Effect of Sense of Ownership and Sense of Agency on Body Representation Change of Human Upper Limb

The vertical-horizontal illusion: Assessing the contributions of anisotropy, abutting, and crossing to the misperception of simple line stimuli

Algebraic functions describing the Zöllner illusion

Haptic Discrimination of Perturbing Fields and Object Boundaries

GROUPING BASED ON PHENOMENAL PROXIMITY

Size Illusion on an Asymmetrically Divided Circle

EXPERIMENTAL ERROR AND DATA ANALYSIS

Depth adjacency and the rod-and-frame illusion

Optimizing color reproduction of natural images

Simultaneous Perception of Forces and Motions Using Bimanual Interactions

Psych 333, Winter 2008, Instructor Boynton, Exam 1

Müller-Lyer Illusion Effect on a Reaching Movement in Simultaneous Presentation of Visual and Haptic/Kinesthetic Cues

The relation between perceived apparent source width and interaural cross-correlation in sound reproduction spaces with low reverberation

The effect of illumination on gray color

Haptic Identification of Stiffness and Force Magnitude

Graphing Techniques. Figure 1. c 2011 Advanced Instructional Systems, Inc. and the University of North Carolina 1

Vision Research 48 (2008) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage:

The roles of axes of symmetry in orientation illusions

Reducing the motor response in haptic parallel matching eliminates the typically observed gender difference

AD-A lji llllllllllii l

Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion

technical drawing

Development of A Finger Mounted Type Haptic Device Using A Plane Approximated to Tangent Plane

Vertical Localization Performance in a Practical 3-D WFS Formulation

Cutaneous Force Feedback as a Sensory Subtraction Technique in Haptics

The effect of two types of induced-motion displays on perceived location of the induced target

ECEN 4606, UNDERGRADUATE OPTICS LAB

Assessing Measurement System Variation

Audio Engineering Society. Convention Paper. Presented at the 119th Convention 2005 October 7 10 New York, New York USA

Influence of stimulus symmetry on visual scanning patterns*

GAZE AS A MEASURE OF SOUND SOURCE LOCALIZATION

SAT pickup arms - discussions on some design aspects

DOSELAB TOMOTHERAPY TG-148 QA QUICK GUIDE TG-148 RECOMMENDED TESTS 1. V.B.1.C. - Y-JAW DIVERGENCE/BEAM CENTERING

Universities of Leeds, Sheffield and York

Experiments on the locus of induced motion

No symmetry advantage when object matching involves accidental viewpoints

Distance perception from motion parallax and ground contact. Rui Ni and Myron L. Braunstein. University of California, Irvine, California

Dissociating Ideomotor and Spatial Compatibility: Empirical Evidence and Connectionist Models

IOC, Vector sum, and squaring: three different motion effects or one?

A Pilot Study: Introduction of Time-domain Segment to Intensity-based Perception Model of High-frequency Vibration

Rendering Moving Tactile Stroke on the Palm Using a Sparse 2D Array

Chapter 3. Adaptation to disparity but not to perceived depth

Object Perception. 23 August PSY Object & Scene 1

Automated measurement of cylinder volume by vision

Visual computation of surface lightness: Local contrast vs. frames of reference

Assessing Measurement System Variation

Keywords: cylindrical near-field acquisition, mechanical and electrical errors, uncertainty, directivity.

The Representational Effect in Complex Systems: A Distributed Representation Approach

The use of size matching to demonstrate the effectiveness of accommodation and convergence as cues for distance*

Statistics 101: Section L Laboratory 10

Running an HCI Experiment in Multiple Parallel Universes

REPORT ITU-R BO Multiple-feed BSS receiving antennas

Color appearance in image displays

Transcription:

The Haptic Perception of Spatial Orientations studied with an Haptic Display Gabriel Baud-Bovy 1 and Edouard Gentaz 2 1 Faculty of Psychology, UHSR University, Milan, Italy gabriel@shaker.med.umn.edu 2 CNRS, Laboratory "Cognition & Development" (UMP 8605 CNRS), University of Paris 5, France gentaz@psycho.univ-paris5.fr Abstract. Blindfolded adults explored the orientation of a virtual rod generated by an haptic display in the frontal plane and reproduced it in the 2D (movements constrained to the frontal plane) or in the 3D (movements free in space) reproduction conditions. We found that the haptic perception of orientations in the frontal plane was similar in both conditions, indicating that the haptic perception of orientations generated by an haptic display is similar to the results of classical haptic studies. 1 Introduction The vertical and horizontal orientations are perceived more accurately than the oblique orientations. Appelle called this anisotropy the "oblique effect"[1]. In most studies of the oblique effect in the haptic modality, the set of possible orientations was limited to a plane by the experimental apparatus (i.e., a metallic rod that rotated around a fixed axis in space) [2,3,4]. The orientation of a stimulus in a plane is defined by a single parameter such as the angle with the horizontal or vertical axis. In contrast, an orientation in space is defined by two parameters such as its azimuth and elevation. The objective of this study is to find out whether the oblique effect is still present in the absence of any spatial constraint when the participant needs to focus only on more than a single parameter to perform the task. In this study, we used a haptic display to present the stimulus and record the response in space. In the exploration phase, the haptic display constrained the participant s fingertip s to-and-fro movements along a "virtual rod" in space. In the reproduction phase, the participant reproduced the same movements in two different conditions. In the 2D reproduction condition, the finger movements were constrained to the frontal plane. Like in the previous studies, we expected to find an oblique effect in this condition. In the 3D reproduction condition, the movements of the finger were unconstrained in space. Because the participant needed to pay attention to the orientation of his or her movements inside as well as outside the frontal plane to perform the task in this condition, we did not know whether the oblique effect would still be present.

462 Gabriel Baud-Bovy and Edouard Gentaz 2 Method Ten right-handed undergraduate students participated in the study. Each participant was blindfolded and wore a brace that immobilized the wrist joint. The participant sat in front of the haptic display (PHANTOM 1.5) and put the right index finger into a thimble that was mounted on the extremity of the haptic device. At the beginning of each trial, the haptic device produced a central force field that guided the participant's finger to the center of the workspace 35 cm in front of the sternum. A beep indicated the beginning of the exploration phase. During this phase, the haptic device allowed the participant to move the fingertip freely during 7.5 seconds along a 14 cm-long line segment which defined the target orientation. Then, a central force field brought the fingertip back to the center of the workspace (5 s). Finally, a second beep indicated the beginning of the reproduction phase during which the participant had to reproduce the exploratory to-and-fro movements as accurately as possible during 7.5 seconds. In the 2D condition, the movements were constrained to the frontal plane during the reproduction phase (Fig. 1). In the 3D condition, the participant could move his or her finger freely in space. The experiment included four orientations which all intersected in the workspace center. Inside each condition, each orientation was presented six times in a random order. Both reproduction conditions were presented in two successive blocks of 24 trials. Their order of presentation was reversed for half the participants. Participants rehearsed the procedure until proficient before starting the experiment. The position of the fingertip was recorded by the haptic device every 50 ms during both phases. To compute the orientation of the movement recorded during the reproduction phase, we fitted a straight line to the trajectory by computing the main eigenvector of the covariance matrix: S = n 1 n 1 i= 1 Fig. 1. The four target orientations positioned in the participant's fronto-parallel plane. ( X x)( X x) where X is an n x 3 matrix with the coordinates of the trajectory in space, n is the number of samples, and x is the mean position. The direction of the main eigenvector constituted the response of the subject to a trial. The angular error in the 3D reproduction condition, i.e. the angular difference between the orientation of the stimulus and of the response in space, was split into two components: the in-plane and out-of-plane error. The in-plane error corresponds to the angle between the target orientation and the projection of the response in the frontal plane while the out-of-plane error corresponds to the angle between the response and the frontal plane. In the 2D reproduction condition, the out-of-plane error is null by definition.

The Haptic Perception of Spatial Orientations studied with an Haptic Display 463 For each participant, target and reproduction condition, we computed the mean and standard deviation (SD) of the in- and out-of-plane errors. Then, we computed the systematic error (across-subject signed average), the between-subject variability (SD of the subject means), and the within-subject variability (mean of the participants SDs ). Fig. 2. In-plane and out-of-plane angular error. Left column: Mean error for each subject and orientation. Each line corresponds to a different participant. Middle column: Grand mean across all subjects (solid line). The bars denote the between-subject variability for each orientations. Right column: Each bar denotes the across- subject average of the standard deviations computed for each orientation and subject separately. 3 Results Errors inside the frontal plane were very similar in the 2D and 3D reproduction conditions (Fig. 2). The vertical orientation was reproduced very accurately by all participants in both conditions (mean ± between-subject variability of the in-plane error: -0.1 ± 0.4 and -0.2 ± 0.6 in the 2D and 3D reproduction conditions respectively). The within-subject variability was also the smallest for the vertical orientation (0.4 and 0.3 for the 2D and 3D conditions, respectively). The second best orientation was the horizontal orientation. The between-subject as well as within-subject variability was the largest for the two obliques (>4.0 ). These results show the presence of an oblique effect in both conditions and are similar to those observed in previous studies. In the 3D reproduction condition, the out-of-plane error was on average smaller for the vertical and horizontal than for the two oblique orientations. The differences across orientations were however less marked than for the in-plane errors. In particular, there was almost no difference across orientations with respect to the withinsubject variability of the out-of-plane error. Participants reproduced the vertical orientation slightly less accurately outside than inside the frontal plane. The within-subject variability was also the larger outside than inside the frontal plane (2.2 vs. 0.3 ). In

464 Gabriel Baud-Bovy and Edouard Gentaz other words, this pattern of errors indicates that the variability of the responses is larger in the sagittal plane than in the frontal plane for the vertical orientation. A two-way repeated-measure ANOVA of the in-plane error with the reproduction condition and the orientation as within-subject revealed that the orientation factor as well as the interaction were statistically significant (orientation: F(3,27)=6.07, ε=.407, P=0.027, interaction: F(3,27)=11.4, P<0.001). Figure 2 shows indeed that errors are larger in the 2D than in the 3D reproduction condition for the two oblique orientations. In order to test whether the variability of the responses depended on the orientation, we analyzed the SDs computed for each participant, orientation and reproduction condition in a similar manner. The results of the two-way repeated measure ANOVA indicated the orientation factor was the only statistically significant effect (F(3,27)=33.11, ε =.915, P 0). Finally, one-way repeated-measure ANOVAs of the out-of-plane errors in the 3D reproduction condition indicated that the orientation factor was not statistically significant (P>.05). 4 Discussion Like in the classical studies, we found an oblique effect inside the frontal plane in both reproduction conditions. However, unlike previous studies, the vertical orientation was better reproduced than the horizontal orientation. A possible explanation may be that the haptic display permitted the participants to make exactly the same movements in the exploratory and reproduction phases. Thus, it is possible that the need in the classical haptic studies to grasp the rod in order to rotate it back to its initial orientation interfered with the perception of the vertical. The relative size of the in-plane and the out-plane errors in the 3D reproduction condition depended on the orientation of the stimulus. While the participants very accurately and consistently reproduced the vertical orientation inside the frontal plane, they produced out-of-plane errors of the same order of magnitude for all other orientations. In particular, we did not find any significant oblique effect. The similarity of the in-plane errors in both conditions and the uniformity of the out-of-plane errors across orientations in the 3D condition suggest that the participants focused their attention on the orientation of the stimulus inside the frontal plane in the 3D and 2D reproduction conditions alike. Thus, the processes involved in reproducing the orientation in the frontal plane could be different from those that permitted the participants to keep their response close to the frontal plane in the 3D reproduction condition. References 1. Appelle, S.: Perception and discrimination as a function of stimulus orientation: The "oblique effect" in man and animals, Vol 78. Psychological Bulletin (1972) 266-278 2. Appelle, S., Countryman, M.: Eliminating the haptic oblique effect: Influence of scanning incongruity and prior knowledge of the standards, Vol 15. Perception (1986) 365 369

The Haptic Perception of Spatial Orientations studied with an Haptic Display 465 3. Gentaz, E., Hatwell, Y.: Role of gravitational cues in the haptic perception of orientation, Vol 58. Perception & Psychophysics (1996) 1278 1292 4. Lechelt, E. C., Verenka, A.: Spatial anisotropy in intramodal and cross modal judgements of stimulus orientations: The stability of the oblique effect, Vol 9. Perception (1980) 581 589