LEVELS OF VIGILANCE TRACK CHANGES IN FLOCK SIZE IN THE GREATER FLAMINGO (PHOENICOPTERUS RUBER RUBER)

Similar documents
Abstract The American Redstart is a wood warbler that is in population decline in northern Michigan.

Protocol for Censusing Yellow-billed Magpies (Pica nuttalli) at Communal Roosts

Effects of human activity on the foraging behavior of sanderlings Calidris alba

EEB 4260 Ornithology. Lecture Notes: Migration

LOUISIANA BIRD RECORDS COMMITTEE

MUD, BIRDS and POPPYCOCK*

Lesser Sandhill Cranes, Annual Summary Homer, Alaska, Summer By Kachemak Crane Watch

HOW THE OTHER HALF LIVES: MONARCH POPULATION TRENDS WEST OF THE GREAT DIVIDE SHAWNA STEVENS AND DENNIS FREY. Biological Sciences Department

Second Term Extra Credit: Bald Eagle Field Study America s most prestigious bird of prey

Project Number: H Project Title:

Philip C. Stouffer Jason A. Zoller. LSU School of Renewable Natural Resources Final Report 30 June 2006

alba) ) on the Susquehanna River in Harrisburg, Pennsylvania in Relation to Environmental Characteristics

Notes on a Breeding Population of Red-headed Woodpeckers in New York State. Jacob L. Berl and John W. Edwards

SOLAR RADIATION, LIGHT INTENSITY, AND ROOSTING

Steven SON 1,2, Frank CEZILLY 2 & Jean-Raphaël GROS-DESORMAUX 1

Susitna-Watana Hydroelectric Project (FERC No ) Waterbird Migration, Breeding, and Habitat Use Study Plan Section 10.15

Guidance note: Distribution of breeding birds in relation to upland wind farms

THE SPRING MIGRATION OF THE OVER EUROPE.

MPA Baseline Program. Annual Progress Report. Use of Estuarine, Intertidal, and Subtidal Habitats by Seabirds Within the MLPA South Coast Study Region

Evidence of a four-year population cycle for the Rusty Blackbird (Euphagus carolinus)

Sensing the World Around Us. Exploring Foundational Biology Concepts through Robotics & Programming

Are Horseshoe Crab Eggs a Limiting Resource for Red Knots?

Farr wind farm: A review of displacement disturbance on golden plover arising from operational turbines

Mallory NSHCF Report 2016 Field Season 1. Factors influencing population decline of marine birds. on Nova Scotia s Eastern Shore Islands

Florida Field Naturalist

Travel to Ecuador to Explore Potential Research and Field School Opportunities

3. Sound source location by difference of phase, on a hydrophone array with small dimensions. Abstract

Ecological Impacts of Australian Ravens on. Bush Bird Communities on Rottnest Island

The Effect of Human Disturbance on Foraging Behavior and Habitat Use in Piping Plover (Charadrius me/o&s)

ENR 2360: Ecology and Conservation of Birds

State of the Estuary Report 2015

Are pine martens the answer to grey squirrel control?

Owls & Turkeys. Literacy Centers For 2 nd & 3 rd Grades. FREE from The Curriculum Corner

Animal Behaviour- Assignment 2

American Kestrel. Appendix A: Birds. Falco sparverius. New Hampshire Wildlife Action Plan Appendix A Birds-183

GULLS WINTERING IN FLORIDA: CHRISTMAS BIRD COUNT ANALYSIS. Elizabeth Anne Schreiber and Ralph W. Schreiber. Introduction

A dynamic method to study the transmission of social foraging information in flocks using robots

Use of Estuarine, Intertidal, and Subtidal Habitats by Seabirds Within the MLPA South Coast Study Region. Final Plan of Work.

AERIAL SURVEY OF BIRDS AT MONO LAKE ON AUGUST 24, 1973

Bolinas Lagoon Heron and Egret Nesting Summary 2017

Farr wind farm: A review of displacement disturbance on dunlin arising from operational turbines

APPENDIX 11.2 BRENT GEESE SURVEY REPORT

MANAGEMENT OF COLONIAL WATERBIRDS AT TOMMY THOMPSON PARK

Seasonal changes in the response of oystercatchers Haematopus ostralegus to human disturbance

Birds Prefer to Feed near Predators than Feed near a Competitor. by Emerson Harris. Center for Teaching and Learning Glenn Powers 3o March 2018

Differential Foraging Patterns of Rodents and Birds in a Restored Prairie

PTT and Geolocator Case Studies. Long-billed Curlew Purple Martins and Wood Thrushes

Assessment of Photographs from Wildlife Monitoring Cameras in Drakes Estero, Point Reyes National Seashore, California

SakerGAP Questionnaire: To be compiled and submitted by National Information Coordinators from each Range State of the species.

IMPACTS ON BIOLOGICAL SYSTEMS: ANIMALS MIGRATORY BIRD ARRIVALS Spring and fall arrivals of some migratory birds are changing

California Least Tern & Western Snowy Plover Monitoring Project. Huntington State Beach Least Tern Natural Preserve A Partnership Since 2005

MONITORING PROGRAM FOR WATER BIRDS INHABITING THE SALT FLATS LOCATED ON NORTHWESTERN BONAIRE, DUTCH CARIBBEAN YEAR REPORT 2010

Wintering Corn Buntings

What Limits the Reproductive Success of Migratory Birds? Warbler Data Analysis (50 pts.)

American Crow (Corvus brachyrhynchos) flight behavior from communal foraging sites to communal roosting sites in Yolo County, California

GENERAL PROTOCOL CONTENTS

Branta leucopsis East Greenland/Scotland & Ireland

4/24/08. Behavioral Ecology / Evolutionary Ecology

Habitat Selection of Nesting and Migrating Birds in the Hortobágy. Ph.D Thesis. Zsolt Végvári

NUMBERS, MIGRATION CHRONOLOGY, AND ACTIVITY PATTERNS OF NONBREEDING CARIBBEAN FLAMINGOS IN YUCATAN, MEXICO

Journal of Avian Biology

Ornithology BIO 426 (W/O2) (Spring 2013; CRN 33963) (tentative, version 26th January 2013)

The effect of interspecific competition on the foraging behavior of the Eastern Gray Squirrel

Red-breasted Goose Monitoring Strategy for the Main Wintering Sites from Dobrogea, Romania

United States Department of the Interior

USING CHRISTMAS BIRD COUNT DATA TO DETERMINE POPULATION TRENDS OF FIVE BIRD SPECIES. by Thomas R. Hamilton

RAPTOR STUDY. raptor charts and posters

Temporal development of the coastal fish community in the Curonian Lagoon (Lithuania)

Sea Duck Joint Venture Annual Project Summary for Endorsed Projects FY08 (October 1, 2007 to September 30, 2008)

Conservation of the Andaman Serpent Eagle Spilornis elgini in the Andaman Islands: Phase I. SACON Technical Report - 192

The following protocols should begin as soon as feasible after identification of a diurnal roost (ideally that night):

NATIONAL RADIO ASTRONOMY OBSERVATORY SOCORRO, NEW MEXICO VLA PROJECT VLA TEST MEMO 114

SakerGAP Questionnaire: To be compiled and submitted by National Information Coordinators from each Range State of the species.

Population Patterns. Math 6.SP.B.4 6.SP.B.5 6.SP.B.5a 6.SP.B.5b 7.SP.B.3 7.SP.A.2 8.SP.A.1. Time: 45 minutes. Grade Level: 3rd to 8th

The Effects of Climate Change on the Breeding Behavior and Migration Patterns of Birds and Mammals. Dr. Susan Longest Colorado Mesa University

Branta leucopsis Russia/Germany & Netherlands

Greenlaw Mountain Hawk Watch Fall 2014

General report format, ref. Article 12 of the Birds Directive, for the report

Where are the Birds? Urban Birds in a Heat Island

Lecture Outline. Why Study Migration? Definitions

CORMORANT ADVISORY GROUP MEETING #13 Thursday, February 26, :30 p.m. to 9:00 p.m. Metro Hall, Room 303, 55 John Street, Toronto

PART FIVE: Grassland and Field Habitat Management

Breeding Curlew in Ireland

The importance of Port Stephens for shorebirds. Alan Stuart Hunter Bird Observers Club

Uptake of BirdLife South Africa/ EWT Best Practice Guidelines for Bird and Wind Energy

HERON AND EGRET MONITORING RESULTS AT WEST MARIN ISLAND: 2003 NESTING SEASON

Twilight ascents of Common Swifts: a comparative analysis

Project Title: Migration patterns, habitat use, and harvest characteristics of long-tailed ducks wintering on Lake Michigan.

E S P APPLICATION FOR SUPPORT OF AN EXPERIMENTAL INVESTIGATION OF MENDEL S PRINCIPLES OF HEREDITY IN ANIMALS AND PLANTS

Biodiversity Observations

International AEWA Single Species Action Planning Workshop for the management of. age e conservation status and possible actions in Germany

Ecological Impacts of Wind Farms: Global Studies. Are Wind Farms Hazardous to Birds and Bats? Stephen J. Ambrose

HUMAN ACTIVITY INFLUENCE AND DIURNAL AND NOCTURNAL FORAGING OF SANDERLINGS (CALIDRIS ALBA)

Determining Optimal Radio Collar Sample Sizes for Monitoring Barren-ground Caribou Populations

Nikon. Nikon Photo Quiz. Sponsored by Nikon Canada ONTARIO BIRDS DECEMBER 2005

Bolinas Lagoon Heron and Egret Nesting Summary 2014

Introduction. Previous and Ongoing Studies on This Issue

SHORT COMMUNICATIONS 169

SCRUB AND MEXICAN JAYS

Marbled Murrelet Effectiveness Monitoring, Northwest Forest Plan

Transcription:

SHORT COMMUNICATIONS ORNITOLOGIA NEOTROPICAL 15: 407 411, 2004 The Neotropical Ornithological Society LEVELS OF VIGILANCE TRACK CHANGES IN FLOCK SIZE IN THE GREATER FLAMINGO (PHOENICOPTERUS RUBER RUBER) Guy Beauchamp 1, & Raymond McNeil 2 1 Faculty of Veterinary Medicine, University of Montréal, P.O. Box 5000, St-Hyacinthe, Québec, Canada J2S 7C6. E-mail: Guy.Beauchamp@umontreal.ca 2 Department of Biological Sciences, University of Montréal, P.O. Box 6128, Succ. Centre- Ville, Montréal, Québec, Canada H3C 3J7. E-mail: Raymond.McNeil@umontreal.ca Los niveles de vigilancia reflejan cambios en el tamaño de las bandadas de Flamencos comunes (Phoenicopterus ruber ruber). Key words: Greater Flamingo, Phoenicopterus ruber, nocturnal foraging, diurnal foraging, flock size, Venezuela, vigilance. INTRODUCTION Numerous empirical studies in birds have demonstrated a decrease in the proportion of time allocated to vigilance as flock size increases (reviewed by Elgar 1989, Roberts 1996, Treves 2000). The negative correlation between vigilance and group size is usually ascribed to a perceived reduction in predation risk in larger groups and/or to an increase in the intensity of feeding competition (Beauchamp 2003). Because investigations of the group-size effect are often correlational, inferences cannot be made about the causal link between changes in flock size and vigilance. In such cases, several extraneous factors may explain why vigilance decreases in large flocks. The most obvious factor is food density. If large flocks aggregate over areas of high food density, vigilance may be expected to decrease as more time can be allocated to feeding thus making the inverse relationship between flock size and vigilance causally spurious (Elgar 1989). One possible way to establish a stronger causal link between vigilance and flock size might be to match changes in vigilance with concurrent changes in flock size in the field during a continuous time sequence. Adjustments in vigilance due to the arrival or departure of companions is more likely to be caused by changes in flock size than by changes in extraneous factors, such as food density, since birds are foraging at the same location at nearly the same time. Among the few studies that have used such a matched design, vigilance has been shown to increase after the departure of companions and to decrease after the arrival of companions in foraging Downy Woodpeckers (Picoides pubescens) (Sullivan 1984) and in preening Crested Terns (Sterna bergii) (Roberts 1995). Here, we examine changes in vigilance 407

BEAUCHAMP & MCNEIL subsequent to the arrival or departure of companions in flocks of Greater Flamingos (Phoenicopterus ruber ruber) foraging in a tropical lagoon complex in Venezuela. Previous correlational work has already documented a reduction in vigilance with variation in flock size in Greater Flamingos (Schmitz & Baldassarre 1992, Beauchamp & McNeil 2003) but it is not clear (i) to what extent variation in vigilance is strongly related to changes in flock size in this species and (ii) what the significance of such a relationship might be. We used the matched design to explore more fully the causal aspects of earlier correlational observations. METHODS Study area. We conducted the study at the Chacopata lagoon complex (10º40 N, 63º46 W) on the north side of the Araya Peninsula, Sucre State, northeastern Venezuela, from 20 December 2000 to 6 January 2001. One of us (GB) observed birds near the shore of two lagoons where foraging Greater Flamingos roosted and foraged in large numbers in the non-breeding season. We used 9X35 binoculars for daytime observations (between 15:30 and 17:30 daily) and a binocular light intensification module (ITT Night Quest 250, Monovision Night Vision, Laguna Beach, California) affixed with a 12X camera lens and an infra-red light beam for nighttime observations (between 18:45 and 21:00 daily). Observations were dictated into a portable cassette recorder. Sampling and statistical analyses. Feeding flocks of Greater Flamingos consisted of birds actively involved in feeding activities and located at least 30 m away from any other flock. We followed at most three different flocks at a single site at each observation period. Observations focused on a single individual in each flock. Since the population was not marked, it is possible that the same individuals may have been sampled more than once but this appears unlikely given the large number of birds in the area at the time (> 200). For each focal bird, sampling of behavior took place over at most 10 min or until the flock changed in size or was lost from sight. The number of birds departing or arriving was noted and the behavior of the same bird, if possible, was sampled one more time after the change in flock size once foraging activities resumed. Occasionally, the focal bird got lost in the shuffle as new birds arrived or left. In those cases, we selected a focal bird close to the area where the previous focal bird had foraged earlier. During each focal observation, we noted whether the focal bird foraged with the head underwater or scanned the surroundings with the head up. The timing of occurrence of all changes in behavior was obtained later from the taped observations. We calculated the time budget before and after the change in flock size. For each focal observation, mean scan duration refers to the average duration of all sequences with the head up, while mean feeding duration refers to the average duration of all sequences with the head underwater which preceded a sequence with the head up. The overall proportion of time spent with the head up is referred to as vigilance. We used linear regression models to relate changes in mean scan duration, mean feeding duration, and vigilance to changes in flock size. We also calculated the time budget of focal birds in focal observations that lasted at least 6 min and during which flock size remained the same. The time budget during the first and during the last 3 min of observation was compiled. We used the Wilcoxon signed ranks test to examine differences in mean scan duration, mean feeding duration, and vigilance within these flocks. Median values along with the interquartile range are presented. 408

SHORT COMMUNICATIONS RESULTS At the two lagoons, Greater Flamingos usually foraged in straight lines within 2 m of each other and moved forward with a scything action of the head. In most cases, individuals joined or left focal flocks by walking. We recorded behavior in flocks that experienced either a decrease (n = 4) or an increase (n = 15) in size. Median flock size before changes in membership was 4 (2.5 10). Behavior was also sampled in 32 flocks that failed to change in size during the observation period. Median flock size in these stable flocks was 16.5 (6.75 25.75). Mean scan duration decreased after an increase in flock size and increased after individuals left the flock (F 1,17 = 5.4, P = 0.03; Fig. 1A). After the arrival of new flock members, mean feeding duration increased but decreased when flock size became smaller (F 1,17 = 6.3, P = 0.02; Fig. 1B). Overall, vigilance decreased when flock size increased and increased when flock size decreased (F 1,17 = 7.3, P = 0.02; Fig. 1C). In sequences where flock size remained stable, we failed to document significant changes between the first and last 3 min of observations in mean scan duration [first: 4.8 s (3.3 7.8 s); last: 4.5 s (3.8 6.1 s); P = 0.99], in mean feeding duration [first: 20.3 s (13.6 37.5 s); last: 16.9 s (13.1 29.8 s); P = 0.08] and in vigilance [first: 17% (7 29%); last: 21% (15 25%); P = 0.24]. DISCUSSION Levels of vigilance observed during timed behavior sequences clearly tracked changes in flock size in foraging Greater Flamingos. As flock size increased, birds foraged for longer bouts and reduced scan duration. Vigilance decreased in these newly enlarged flocks. In contrast, birds foraged to a lesser extent as flock size decreased and scanned for longer FIG. 1. Changes in mean scan duration (A), mean feeding duration (B) and vigilance (C) as a function of changes in flock size in foraging Greater Flamingos (n = 19). Positive changes in flock size indicate that the focal flock experienced a decrease in size. Similarly, positive changes in mean scan duration, mean feeding duration or vigilance also indicate a decrease as flock size changed during the observation period. periods. Vigilance thus increased in these newly reduced flocks. The results are consistent with the findings of several earlier studies 409

BEAUCHAMP & MCNEIL that correlated changes in vigilance with flock size (Elgar 1989, Roberts 1996, Treves 2000). Here, we demonstrate a strong link between changes in flock size and vigilance in a wild foraging bird species. In addition, we show that vigilance in the Greater Flamingo is a dynamic process whereby birds adjust their level of vigilance in response to arrivals to, and departures from, the flock (Broom & Ruxton 1998). One potential confounding factor in the matched design used in this study is the passage of time which necessarily accompanies any changes in flock size. As time goes by, individuals in a flock may become less wary since sufficient time may have elapsed to assess potential risk factors (Carbone et al. 2003) such as disturbance by people (Yosef 2000). In this case, vigilance may be expected to decrease with time thus masking any reduction due to a subsequent increase in flock size. Similarly, food density may decrease with time or birds may become less hungry. In this case, vigilance may be expected to increase with time thus confounding any increase due to a subsequent decrease in flock size. However, there was no indication that our measures of vigilance varied at the time scale used in the study. The earlier study with Downy Woodpeckers in a foraging context did not control for the potential effects of wariness, food density, or satiation (Sullivan 1984). Greater Flamingos reacted to the number of birds added to or subtracted from the flock as indicated by significant slopes in the regression models. It remains to be seen over which range of flock sizes Greater Flamingos will still respond to changes in flock size. In addition, it would be interesting to examine whether flocks of different sizes react differently to arrivals and departures. One would assume that changes in behavior may be more pronounced in small flocks which maintain a high level of vigilance and where the scope for adjustments in vigilance is highest. In preening Crested Terns, however, the magnitude of change in vigilance was not related to flock size (Roberts 1995). The matched design provides a powerful means to examine the effect of flock size on behavior when the effect of extraneous factors, such as food density, is not expected to vary to a large extent over the duration of observations. Once a strong link is established, it becomes possible to examine more clearly how ecological factors, such as predation risk and feeding competition, will influence the relationship between vigilance and flock size. We did not investigate these ecological factors but suggest that this should be the next step in further studies of vigilance behavior. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We wish to thank J. Rodriguez for use of the field station and F. Vuilleumier for useful comments on the paper. This study was partly funded by grants from NSERC to RM, the University of Montréal, and the Universidad de Oriente in Venezuela through the collaboration agreement between the two universities. REFERENCES Beauchamp, G. 2003. Group-size effects on vigilance: a search for mechanisms. Behav. Proc. 63: 111 121. Beauchamp, G., & R. McNeil. 2003. Vigilance in Greater Flamingos foraging at night. Ethology 109: 511 520. Broom, M., & G. D. Ruxton. 1998. Modelling responses in vigilance rates to arrivals and departures from a group of foragers. IMA J. Math. Appl. Med. Biol. 15: 387 400. Carbone, C., W. A. Thompson, L. Zadorina, & J. M. Rowcliffe. 2003. Competition, predation risk and patterns of flock expansion in Barnacle Geese (Branta leucopsis). J. Zool. 259: 301 410

SHORT COMMUNICATIONS 308. Elgar, M. A. 1989. Predator vigilance and group size in mammals and birds. Biol. Rev. 64: 13 33. Roberts, G. 1995. A real-time response of vigilance behaviour to changes in group size. Anim. Behav. 50: 1371 1374. Roberts, G. 1996. Why individual vigilance declines as group size increases. Anim. Behav. 51: 1077 1086. Schmitz, R. A., & G. A. Baldassarre. 1992. Correlates of flock size and behavior of foraging American Flamingos following hurricane Gilbert in Yucatan, Mexico. Condor 93: 260 264. Sullivan, K. A. 1984. The advantages of social foraging in downy woodpeckers. Anim. Behav. 32: 16 22. Treves, A. 2000. Theory and method in studies of vigilance and aggregation. Anim. Behav. 60: 711 722. Yosef, R. 2000. Individual distances among Greater Flamingos as indicators of tourism pressure. Waterbirds 23: 26 31. Accepted 4 March 2004. 411