Thresholds for Dynamic Changes in a Rotary Switch

Similar documents
THIS study focuses on a group of common haptic objects

Discrimination of Virtual Haptic Textures Rendered with Different Update Rates

A Pilot Study: Introduction of Time-domain Segment to Intensity-based Perception Model of High-frequency Vibration

A Perceptual Study on Haptic Rendering of Surface Topography when Both Surface Height and Stiffness Vary

Speech, Hearing and Language: work in progress. Volume 12

Haptic Identification of Stiffness and Force Magnitude

Haptic Cueing of a Visual Change-Detection Task: Implications for Multimodal Interfaces

Here I present more details about the methods of the experiments which are. described in the main text, and describe two additional examinations which

Rendering Moving Tactile Stroke on the Palm Using a Sparse 2D Array

Haptic Models of an Automotive Turn-Signal Switch: Identification and Playback Results

A Study of Perceptual Performance in Haptic Virtual Environments

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Perception of Haptic Force Magnitude during Hand Movements

Increasing the Impedance Range of a Haptic Display by Adding Electrical Damping

The Shape-Weight Illusion

Vibrotactile Apparent Movement by DC Motors and Voice-coil Tactors

2920 J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 102 (5), Pt. 1, November /97/102(5)/2920/5/$ Acoustical Society of America 2920

Comparison of Human Haptic Size Discrimination Performance in Simulated Environments with Varying Levels of Force and Stiffness

Exploring Surround Haptics Displays

Chapter II. Development of a Multi-Finger Positional Display

Tone-in-noise detection: Observed discrepancies in spectral integration. Nicolas Le Goff a) Technische Universiteit Eindhoven, P.O.

Lecture 7: Human haptics

Differences in Fitts Law Task Performance Based on Environment Scaling

Haptic Perception & Human Response to Vibrations

Absolute and Discrimination Thresholds of a Flexible Texture Display*

The Haptic Perception of Spatial Orientations studied with an Haptic Display

Computer Haptics and Applications

Effects of Longitudinal Skin Stretch on the Perception of Friction

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

Salient features make a search easy

The Effect of Force Saturation on the Haptic Perception of Detail

Chapter 2 Introduction to Haptics 2.1 Definition of Haptics

Haplug: A Haptic Plug for Dynamic VR Interactions

Elements of Haptic Interfaces

Perception of pitch. Definitions. Why is pitch important? BSc Audiology/MSc SHS Psychoacoustics wk 4: 7 Feb A. Faulkner.

Spatial Low Pass Filters for Pin Actuated Tactile Displays

The Effect of Opponent Noise on Image Quality

Haptic Abilities of Freshman Engineers as Measured by the Haptic Visual Discrimination Test

Abstract. 2. Related Work. 1. Introduction Icon Design

Touch & Haptics. Touch & High Information Transfer Rate. Modern Haptics. Human. Haptics

Passive and Active Kinesthetic Perception Just-noticeable-difference for Natural Frequency of Virtual Dynamic Systems

Haptic Camera Manipulation: Extending the Camera In Hand Metaphor

Comparison of Three Eye Tracking Devices in Psychology of Programming Research

Tactile Actuators Using SMA Micro-wires and the Generation of Texture Sensation from Images

Perception of pitch. Definitions. Why is pitch important? BSc Audiology/MSc SHS Psychoacoustics wk 5: 12 Feb A. Faulkner.

Texture recognition using force sensitive resistors

The Perception of Optical Flow in Driving Simulators

Lecture on Angular Vibration Measurements Based on Phase Demodulation

A Tactile Display using Ultrasound Linear Phased Array

Speed Feedback and Current Control in PWM DC Motor Drives

The Effect of Frequency Shifting on Audio-Tactile Conversion for Enriching Musical Experience

Perception of Curvature and Object Motion Via Contact Location Feedback

phri: specialization groups HS PRELIMINARY

HAPTIC interactions have become increasingly popular in

Haptic control in a virtual environment

Proprioception & force sensing

Perceptual Overlays for Teaching Advanced Driving Skills

Behavioural Realism as a metric of Presence

Gear Transmission Error Measurements based on the Phase Demodulation

Haptic Display of Multiple Scalar Fields on a Surface

MEM 01 DC MOTOR-BASED SERVOMECHANISM WITH TACHOMETER FEEDBACK

From Encoding Sound to Encoding Touch

The influence of changing haptic refresh-rate on subjective user experiences - lessons for effective touchbased applications.

332:223 Principles of Electrical Engineering I Laboratory Experiment #2 Title: Function Generators and Oscilloscopes Suggested Equipment:

CONTACT FORCE PERCEPTION WITH AN UNGROUNDED HAPTIC INTERFACE

2. Introduction to Computer Haptics

PHASE DEMODULATION OF IMPULSE SIGNALS IN MACHINE SHAFT ANGULAR VIBRATION MEASUREMENTS

Research Article Perception-Based Tactile Soft Keyboard for the Touchscreen of Tablets

Spatial Judgments from Different Vantage Points: A Different Perspective

Arbitrating Multimodal Outputs: Using Ambient Displays as Interruptions

Simultaneous presentation of tactile and auditory motion on the abdomen to realize the experience of being cut by a sword

DC motor control using arduino

Effects of Magnitude and Phase Cues on Human Motor Adaptation

Shape Memory Alloy Actuator Controller Design for Tactile Displays

Jane Li. Assistant Professor Mechanical Engineering Department, Robotic Engineering Program Worcester Polytechnic Institute

Does Judgement of Haptic Virtual Texture Roughness Scale Monotonically With Lateral Force Modulation?

702. Investigation of attraction force and vibration of a slipper in a tactile device with electromagnet

Periodic Tactile Feedback for Accelerator Pedal Control

TOUCH screens are an indispensable part of our lives.

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007

A CLOSER LOOK AT THE REPRESENTATION OF INTERAURAL DIFFERENCES IN A BINAURAL MODEL

Electro-hydraulic Servo Valve Systems

Perceptual Design of Haptic Icons

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007

CHAPTER 2. RELATED WORK 9 similar study, Gillespie (1996) built a one-octave force-feedback piano keyboard to convey forces derived from this model to

The role of intrinsic masker fluctuations on the spectral spread of masking

Decomposing the Performance of Admittance and Series Elastic Haptic Rendering Architectures

MAE106 Laboratory Exercises Lab # 5 - PD Control of DC motor position

Motomatic Servo Control

Psychophysical Characterization and Testbed Validation of a Wearable Vibrotactile Glove for Telemanipulation

the human chapter 1 Traffic lights the human User-centred Design Light Vision part 1 (modified extract for AISD 2005) Information i/o

Active Vibration Isolation of an Unbalanced Machine Tool Spindle

Perception of pitch. Importance of pitch: 2. mother hemp horse. scold. Definitions. Why is pitch important? AUDL4007: 11 Feb A. Faulkner.

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Design and evaluation of Hapticons for enriched Instant Messaging

Redundant Coding of Simulated Tactile Key Clicks with Audio Signals

Vibration Feedback Models for Virtual Environments

Sound rendering in Interactive Multimodal Systems. Federico Avanzini

This is a postprint of. The influence of material cues on early grasping force. Bergmann Tiest, W.M., Kappers, A.M.L.

Evaluation of Five-finger Haptic Communication with Network Delay

Transcription:

Proceedings of EuroHaptics 2003, Dublin, Ireland, pp. 343-350, July 6-9, 2003. Thresholds for Dynamic Changes in a Rotary Switch Shuo Yang 1, Hong Z. Tan 1, Pietro Buttolo 2, Matthew Johnston 2, and Zygmunt Pizlo 3 1 Haptic Interface Research Laboratory, Purdue University 1285 Electrical Engineering Building, West Lafayette, IN 47907-1285 {yang22, hongtan}@purdue.edu 2 Ford Motor Company, 2101 Village Road, Dearborn, MI 48121-2053, {pbuttolo, mjohn223}@ford.com 3 Department of Psychological Sciences, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN 47907-1364 pizlo@psych.purdue.edu Abstract. Switches are everyday objects that interface humans to many functionalities and influence our perception of the machinery that they are attached to. This study is aimed at a better understanding of the perceptual attributes of switches. Specifically, the perceptual thresholds for dynamic changes in a rotary switch were evaluated in two experiments using an adaptive procedure. Exp. I measured human s ability to detect the presence of a random noise superimposed on a sinusoidal torque vs. angular position profile. The detection thresholds were found to be in the range 1 3% of the peak torque. This high sensitivity was interpreted as being consistent with the existing literature in that humans are more sensitive to stimulation at high frequencies than that at low frequencies. Having established the importance of switch dynamics in its perception, Exp. II measured detection thresholds for sinusoidal torque variations with spatial frequencies ranging from 2.8 to 180. Average thresholds varied from 0.37 to 26.17% of the average torque (30 N mm) over the aforementioned spatial frequency range. These experiments provide quantitative results on our ability to detect dynamic events in a rotary switch. They shed new insight on the design and specification of rotary switches. 1 Introduction This study focuses on a group of common haptic objects known as switches. Switches come in many shapes and forms in our everyday life. They not only serve important functions but also color our daily experience. The way a headlight switch looks, feels and sounds inside an automobile, for example, influences our perception of not only the quality of the switch but the car as well. What then, are the perceptual attributes of switches? What makes one switch feel different from another? What engineering metrics help to predict the haptic percept of a switch? How can a target percept for a switch be translated into engineering design specifications for mass production? Many studies have investigated the feel of switches, and ways of emulating switches with electromechanical devices. One of the earliest studies on torque sensi- 343

tivity was conducted by Woodruff and Helson [1]. Using a steel rod mounted on two ball-bearing assemblies, they measured Weber fractions for torque discrimination using different knob sizes and varying weights suspended from the rod. MacLean proposed the idea of using a haptic interface to capture the force-distance characteristics of a switch (e.g., a toggle switch), and use the same haptic device to emulate the switch [2, 3]. An instrumented haptic object was used as an expressive media for multimodal communication [4] and for manipulating digital media [5]. Recently, Hasser and Cutkosky modeled the dynamics of human hands holding a haptic knob in a simple pinch grasp with a second-order system [6]. Such a model can lead to better algorithms for the stable control of an active switch. Realizing the importance of haptic interfaces in an automobile, car manufacturers have embraced the idea of a control dial with active haptic feedback. Examples include the idrive system codeveloped by Immersion Corporation and BMW, and the Haptic Scroll Wheel in a Nissan concept car. Despite all these efforts, much work still remains to fully understand the perceptual attributes of a switch. Our work is motivated by the desire to find a parsimonious set of engineering parameters that uniquely determines the percept of a switch. Traditionally, switch performance has been characterized by torque vs. travel (angular position) profiles for rotary switches. Manufacturers of switches are often given such profiles with fixed, say 10-30%, tolerance. We argue that these profiles are not sufficient to describe the perception of the resultant switches. We show that the dynamic behavior of a switch plays an important role in the way a switch feels to the fingers. Two experiments are reported in this paper. In the first experiment, we demonstrate the extraordinary sensitivity of human fingers to the high-frequency noise in switch dynamics. In the second experiment, we characterize the thresholds for switch dynamics as a function of spatial frequency. 2 Experiment I: Torque Profile Roughness Threshold Informal observations indicated that humans are extremely sensitive to minute buzzing noise during an otherwise smooth turn of a rotary switch. To quantify this perceptual phenomenon, we investigated the detection thresholds for random variations in the torque profile of a rotary switch. 2.1 Method The experimental apparatus consisted of a rotary motor (Maxon RE25 118752), an optical encoder (Computer Optical Product, CP950), a power amplifier (Trust Automation 115), and a transformer (CUI MPS100-24). As shown in Fig. 1, a rotary knob is attached to the end of the motor shaft. The subject grasped the knob with the thumb and the index finger of the left hand, and turned the switch to the left (i.e., counter-clockwise). The torque resisting the turn was programmed to follow a chosen profile as a function of angular position. 344

Fig. 1. Experimental apparatus used in both Exp. I and II. A rotary motor applied the torque profile for a stimulated rotary switch. Subject grasped a rotary knob that interfaced the thumb and the index finger to the motor shaft The stimuli used in Exp. I were characterized by the typical sinusoidal torque vs. angle profiles for a rotary switch (Fig. 2a). Fig. 2b shows a low-frequency sinusoidal profile superimposed with a white noise signal. Each noise sample was generated from a uniform distribution centered at 0 peak-torque level. The signal was then filtered in the spatial domain by a 4 th -order Butterworth low-pass filter with a cutoff frequency that was 32 times that of the original low-frequency signal. The subject s task was to detect the presence of the noise, or equivalently, to discriminate between a smooth turn (Fig. 2a) and a rough turn (Fig. 2b). Nine subjects (3 females and 6 males) participated in Exp. I. The subject sat in front of a table with the apparatus on the left and a computer monitor and a keyboard on the right. During the experiments, the subject was instructed to rest the left elbow on the table in a comfortable position. A black cloth was placed between the apparatus and the keyboard to prevent the subject from seeing the hand or the apparatus. A noise-reduction earphone was worn by the subject to eliminate any possible auditory cues emanating from the apparatus. A brief training procedure was conducted at the beginning of each run. After entering the experimental parameters for the run, the subject was given an opportunity to feel either stimulus alternative by clicking on the corresponding button. The training was self paced and lasted as long as the subject wished. Data collection began when the subject terminated the training. 345

(a) (b) Fig. 2. Stimuli used in Exp. I. Subject felt either a smooth sinusoidal torque profile (Fig. 2a) or one superimposed with random noise (Fig. 2b). Magnitude of the superimposed noise varied from trial to trial depending on the subject s response to the previous trial A one-up two-down adaptive procedure was employed to estimate the threshold corresponding to the 70.7% level on the psychometric function [7]. On each trial, the subject felt, in random order, the two rotary switches shown in Fig. 2. The subject s task was to indicate which switch felt rougher. The noise level (defined by the width of the random noise distribution) increased after each incorrect response ( oneup ) and decreased after two consecutive correct responses ( two-down ). The noise level at which stimulus intensity changed from increasing to decreasing (or vice versa) was called a reversal. The average value of the noise levels from the last 3 reversals was recorded as the estimated threshold. 2.2 Results Thresholds ranged 0.5 to 1.5 N mm for a peak torque value of 50 N mm. The data correspond to a noise sensitivity of 1 to 3% of the peak torque. These values are considerably lower than the force thresholds (Weber fraction) of 5-10% reported in the literature [8] [9], or the torque threshold of 4-12% [1]. This is mainly due to the difference in the way stimuli were generated. Most studies on force or torque discrimination threshold require the subject to discriminate between two constant or slowly-varying force profiles. An equivalent construction would be to have a subject discriminate between the profile shown in Fig. 2a and a similar sinusoidal profile with a larger amplitude. The pair of stimuli we used in Exp. I differ in the high-frequency noise component. Therefore, our results are detection thresholds for high-frequency noises, rather than discrimination thresholds. The fact that our thresholds are much lower than reported force/torque discrimination thresholds is consistent with the general knowledge that the somatosensory system is more sensitive to high frequency vibrations (especially in the range 200-300 Hz, mediated by Pacinian corpuscles) than to low frequency motions [10]. 346

3 Experiment II: Detection of Spatial Torque Variations The results of Exp. I indicated that humans are extremely sensitive to high-frequency dynamic behavior of a switch. Exp. II was designed to investigate this sensitivity further by measuring the detection thresholds for spatial torque variations at seven spatial frequencies over a 64-fold range. 3.1 Method The same experimental apparatus shown in Fig. 1 was used in Exp. II. Five subjects (1 female and 4 males) participated in these experiments. The stimuli consisted of either a constant torque or one superimposed with a sinusoidal torque variation (Fig. 3). Visual and auditory shields were used throughout the experiments to eliminate possible extraneous cues. Before each run, the subject could feel either the constant or the sinusoidal torque profile for as many times as they wished. Data collection began after the subject terminated the training. A three-interval forced-choice paradigm combined with a one-up three-down adaptive procedure was employed because it has been shown to be most efficient by a number of investigators (see [11] for a review). On each trial, the subject turned the rotary knob three times ( three intervals ). A mechanical hard stop was used to ensure that each turn started from the same position, and a small torque in the clockwise direction kept the knob at the hard stop when the subject was not applying any force to the knob (see Fig. 1). As the subject turned the switch in a counter-clockwise direction, resistive torque in the clockwise direction was applied to the knob according to the torque vs. angle profile such as the one shown in Fig. 4. The total travel was limited to 90. At the end of the travel, applied torque dropped to zero. As the subject brought the knob back to its resting position, a small restoring torque was applied to help bring the knob to the mechanical stop. This completed one turn. Of the three turns the subject had to execute for each trial, two of them (randomly selected) contained a constant torque profile, and the remaining turn contained a sinusoidally-varying torque profile. The subject s task was to indicate which of the three intervals contained the sinusoidal profile. The response was then used by the adaptive procedure to determine the amplitude of the sinusoidal signal for the next trial. No feedback was provided. Amplitudes were changed initially by 4 db and then by 1 db after the first three reversals. An experimental run was terminated after 12 reversals at the 1-dB step size. Each run typically lasted 70-90 trials. The threshold was estimated as the average of those last 12 reversal amplitudes. Detection thresholds obtained this way correspond to 79.4% correct responses [7]. To estimate the standard error of the estimate of the threshold, 6 estimates of the threshold were calculated from the 6 pairs of the 12 reversals, and the corresponding standard error was obtained (see [12] for a description of a similar experimental procedure and data analysis technique). 347

Fig. 3. Torque profiles used in Exp. II, shown with a spatial period of 45. Offset torque was kept at 30 N mm. Subject either felt a constant torque (dashed line) or a variable torque (solid line) as a function of angular position. Amplitude of the superimposed sinusoidal signal varied from trial to trial depending on the subject s response to the previous trial. Spatial period varied from run to run 3.2 Results Thresholds obtained from the five subjects are shown in Fig. 4. For each subject, thresholds increased as the spatial period of the sinusoidal variation increased. Thresholds averaged over the five subjects varied from 0.11 to 7.85 N mm, corresponding to 0.37 to 26.17% of the average torque value. This represents a 71-fold increase in threshold over a 64-fold increase in the spatial frequencies of the torque variations. This trend indicates an almost linear growth of threshold as a function of spatial period. It shows that it is much easier for humans to detect a fast-varying dynamic behavior in the switch than to detect a slowly-varying one. Subject SY and PB s data are quite similar (p=0.9894) and appear to be the lowest in Fig. 4. These two subjects have had the most experience with the rotary switch emulation apparatus from debugging experimental procedures, and from participating in other psychophysical experiments involving the same apparatus. A Tukey s HSD procedure revealed three groups of subjects: (1) SY, PB and HT, (2) SC, and (3) ZP, with increasing detection thresholds [critical value q(0.32148; 175) = 3.89818, minimum significant difference = 0.341]. The mean thresholds for subjects SY, PB, HT, SC and ZP were 1.5303, 1.5314, 1.7334, 3.1189 and 3.9316, respectively. 348

Fig. 4. Detection thresholds for sinusoidal torque variations for all five subjects. Data from each subject are plotted with the same symbol. Threshold is plotted as the amplitude of the sinusoidal torque variation (see Fig. 3) against the spatial period of the same. For clarity, only the ±1 standard error bars for subject ZP are shown. These error bars are representative of other subjects data. Solid lines are used for subject SY and PB whose thresholds are significantly lower than those of the other subjects 4 Discussion Several factors might have affected the detection thresholds obtained in Exp. II. First, there is the question of whether detection thresholds depended on the speed with which a subject turned the rotary knob. One alternative interpretation of the results in Fig. 4 would be that thresholds increased as temporal frequency decreased. We define temporal frequency as the ratio of rotating velocity (degree/sec) over spatial period (degree/cycle). Some subjects commented on the impression of an optimal velocity with which they performed the best. It would therefore be interesting to investigate the relationship between detection threshold and temporal frequency. Such results can be directly compared to similar data in literature where stimuli are characterized in position but not force or torque (e.g., [10]). Second, there is also the question of whether detection thresholds depended on the average torque value. According to [1], torque discrimination threshold decreased from 12.6 to 4.4% when the 349

reference torque increased from 8.35 to 100 e-gm (equivalent gram-meter). It is therefore necessary to investigate whether the thresholds obtained in Exp. II would change if the average torque was different. These issues will be topics for future studies. Acknowledgment This work has been partially supported by the Ford Motor Company through a University Research Program grant, and by the National Science Foundation under Grant No. 9984991. References 1. B. Woodruff and H. Helson, Torque Sensitivity as a Function of Knob Radius and Load, American Journal of Psychology, vol. 80, pp. 558-571, 1967. 2. K. E. MacLean, Emulation of Haptic Feedback for Manual Interfaces, doctoral dissertation, Dept. of Mechanical Engineering, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, 1996. 3. K. E. MacLean, The Haptic Camera: A Technique for Characterizing and Playing Back Haptic Environments, Proceedings of the 5th International Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environments and Teleoperator Systems, vol. DSC-Vol. 58, 1996. 4. K. E. MacLean and J. B. Roderick, Aladdin: Exploring Language with a Haptic Door Knob, Interval Research Corp. Technical Report # 1999-058, 1999. 5. S. S. Snibble and K. E. Maclean, Haptic Technique for Media Control, Proceedings of the 14th Annual ACM Symposium on User Interface Software and Technology, 2001. 6. C. J. Hasser and M. R. Cutkosky, System identification of the human hand grasping a haptic knob, Proceedings of the 10th Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment and Teleoperator Systems, pp. 180-189, 2002. 7. H. Levitt, Transformed Up-Down Methods in Psychoacoustics, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, vol. 49, pp. 467-477, 1971. 8. L. A. Jones, Matching forces: constant errors and differential thresholds, Perception, vol. 18, pp. 681-687, 1989. 9. X. D. Pang, H. Z. Tan, and N. I. Durlach, Manual discrimination of force using active finger motion, Perception & Psychophysics, vol. 49, pp. 531-540, 1991. 10. S. J. Bolanowski Jr., G. A. Gescheider, R. T. Verrillo, and C. M. Checkosky, Four channels mediate the mechanical aspects of touch, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, vol. 84, pp. 1680-1694, 1988. 11. M. R. Leek, Adaptive procedures in psychophysical research, Perception & Psychophysics, vol. 63, pp. 1279-1292, 2001. 12. A. J. Brisben, S. S. Hsiao, and K. O. Johnson, Detection of vibration transmitted through an object grasped in the hand, Journal of Neurophysiology, vol. 81, pp. 1548-1558, 1999. 350