Häkkinen, Jukka; Gröhn, Lauri Turning water into rock

Similar documents
Turbine Blade Illusion

Directional Bias in the Perception of Cast Shadows

Limitations of the Oriented Difference of Gaussian Filter in Special Cases of Brightness Perception Illusions

Psychophysics of night vision device halo

Spatial Judgments from Different Vantage Points: A Different Perspective

Computational Vision and Picture. Plan. Computational Vision and Picture. Distal vs. proximal stimulus. Vision as an inverse problem

Plan. Vision Solves Problems. Distal vs. proximal stimulus. Vision as an inverse problem. Unconscious inference (Helmholtz)

Occlusion. Atmospheric Perspective. Height in the Field of View. Seeing Depth The Cue Approach. Monocular/Pictorial

Human Vision. Human Vision - Perception

The Anne Boleyn Illusion is a six-fingered salute to sensory remapping

This is a repository copy of Thatcher s Britain: : a new take on an old illusion.

CAN WE BELIEVE OUR OWN EYES?

Perceptual Organization

Discriminating direction of motion trajectories from angular speed and background information

Perception of scene layout from optical contact, shadows, and motion

COPYRIGHTED MATERIAL. Overview

COPYRIGHTED MATERIAL OVERVIEW 1

Vision. Definition. Sensing of objects by the light reflected off the objects into our eyes

Beau Lotto: Optical Illusions Show How We See

Face Perception. The Thatcher Illusion. The Thatcher Illusion. Can you recognize these upside-down faces? The Face Inversion Effect

Perceived depth is enhanced with parallax scanning

Virtual Reality Technology and Convergence. NBA 6120 February 14, 2018 Donald P. Greenberg Lecture 7

Blindness to Curvature and Blindness to Illusory Curvature

First-order structure induces the 3-D curvature contrast effect

Ala-Laurinaho, Juha; Zheng, Jianfang; Räisänen, Antti One-antenna gain measurement in a probe station

Perception. What We Will Cover in This Section. Perception. How we interpret the information our senses receive. Overview Perception

The Shape-Weight Illusion

EC-433 Digital Image Processing

Virtual Reality Technology and Convergence. NBAY 6120 March 20, 2018 Donald P. Greenberg Lecture 7

Limitations of the Medium, compensation or accentuation

Limitations of the medium

Peripheral Color Demo

Non-Photorealistic Rendering

GLOSSARY for National Core Arts: Media Arts STANDARDS

Non-Photorealistic Rendering

Perception: From Biology to Psychology

Virtual Reality. NBAY 6120 April 4, 2016 Donald P. Greenberg Lecture 9

Optics Practice. Version #: 0. Name: Date: 07/01/2010

Image Extraction using Image Mining Technique

Chapter 18 Optical Elements

Simple Figures and Perceptions in Depth (2): Stereo Capture

You ve heard about the different types of lines that can appear in line drawings. Now we re ready to talk about how people perceive line drawings.

Patronen, J.; Stenroos, Christian; Virkkunen, Mikko; Papula, Suvi; Sarikka, Teemu Inspection of Carbon Fibre Titanium Carbon Fibre Stepped-Lap Joint

ISO INTERNATIONAL STANDARD. Photography Electronic still-picture cameras Resolution measurements

doi: /

Enhancement of Perceived Sharpness by Chroma Contrast

Development of sensitivity to static pictorial depth information*

P rcep e t p i t on n a s a s u n u c n ons n c s ious u s i nf n e f renc n e L ctur u e 4 : Recogni n t i io i n

The eye, displays and visual effects

Introduction. Visual data acquisition devices. The goal of computer vision. The goal of computer vision. Vision as measurement device

Unit IV: Sensation & Perception. Module 19 Vision Organization & Interpretation

Research on visual physiological characteristics via virtual driving platform

Object Perception. 23 August PSY Object & Scene 1

CB Database: A change blindness database for objects in natural indoor scenes

Inverting an Image Does Not Improve Drawing Accuracy

Virtual Reality. Lecture #11 NBA 6120 Donald P. Greenberg September 30, 2015

The reference frame of figure ground assignment

NON UNIFORM BACKGROUND REMOVAL FOR PARTICLE ANALYSIS BASED ON MORPHOLOGICAL STRUCTURING ELEMENT:

The Lady's not for turning: Rotation of the Thatcher illusion

Perceptual Organization. Unit 3 RG 4e

Effect of camera separation on the viewing experience of stereoscopic photographs

Image Quality Assessment for Defocused Blur Images

Different Mirror Surfaces

IOC, Vector sum, and squaring: three different motion effects or one?

Slide 4 Now we have the same components that we find in our eye. The analogy is made clear in this slide. Slide 5 Important structures in the eye

Reference Free Image Quality Evaluation

Resolving Perceptual Ambiguity Visual Rules & Other Factors

This document is a preview generated by EVS

Moon Illusion. (McCready, ; 1. What is Moon Illusion and what it is not

III. Publication III. c 2005 Toni Hirvonen.

On the Monty Hall Dilemma and Some Related Variations

Geometric Optics Practice Problems. Ray Tracing - Draw at least two principle rays and show the image created by the lens or mirror.

Perceived Image Quality and Acceptability of Photographic Prints Originating from Different Resolution Digital Capture Devices

2. GOALS OF THE STUDY 3. EXPERIMENT Method Procedure

Human Vision and Human-Computer Interaction. Much content from Jeff Johnson, UI Wizards, Inc.

Orientation-sensitivity to facial features explains the Thatcher illusion

Hannula, Jari-Matti & Viikari, Ville Uncertainty analysis of intermodulation-based antenna measurements

Maaspuro, Mika PIR sensor modeling and simulating using Comsol Multiphysics and its Ray Optics Module

The fragile edges of. block averaged portraits

The Quantitative Aspects of Color Rendering for Memory Colors

The shape of luminance increments at the intersection alters the magnitude of the scintillating grid illusion

Homework #2 Color Science

Articulation: brightness, apparent illumination, and contrast ratios

Prof. Riyadh Al_Azzawi F.R.C.Psych

Intuitive Color Mixing and Compositing for Visualization

Limitations of the Medium, compensation or accentuation

Limitations of the Medium, compensation or accentuation

Visual computation of surface lightness: Local contrast vs. frames of reference

Nonuniform multi level crossing for signal reconstruction

Perception. The process of organizing and interpreting information, enabling us to recognize meaningful objects and events.

Light and Optical Systems

Exploring body holistic processing investigated with composite illusion

Salient features make a search easy

Gestalt Principles of Visual Perception

Image Quality Evaluation for Smart- Phone Displays at Lighting Levels of Indoor and Outdoor Conditions

THE RELATIVE IMPORTANCE OF PICTORIAL AND NONPICTORIAL DISTANCE CUES FOR DRIVER VISION. Michael J. Flannagan Michael Sivak Julie K.

Sensation & Perception

A test of size-scaling and relative-size hypotheses for the moon illusion

Sensation and Perception

Bayesian Method for Recovering Surface and Illuminant Properties from Photosensor Responses

Transcription:

Powered by TCPDF (www.tcpdf.org) This is an electronic reprint of the original article. This reprint may differ from the original in pagination and typographic detail. Häkkinen, Jukka; Gröhn, Lauri Turning water into rock Published in: Perception DOI: 10.1177/2041669515627951 Published: 01/02/2016 Document Version Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Please cite the original version: Häkkinen, J., & Gröhn, L. (2016). Turning water into rock: The inverted waves effect. Perception, 7(1), 1-6. https://doi.org/10.1177/2041669515627951 This material is protected by copyright and other intellectual property rights, and duplication or sale of all or part of any of the repository collections is not permitted, except that material may be duplicated by you for your research use or educational purposes in electronic or print form. You must obtain permission for any other use. Electronic or print copies may not be offered, whether for sale or otherwise to anyone who is not an authorised user.

Short and Sweet Turning water into rock: The inverted waves effect i-perception January-February 2016: 1 6! The Author(s) 2016 DOI: 10.1177/2041669515627951 ipe.sagepub.com Jukka Häkkinen Institute of Behavioural Sciences, University of Helsinki, Finland Lauri Gröhn Synestesia Software Music, Finland Abstract Humans perceive shape in two-dimensional shaded images, and turning such an image upside down can result in inversion of the relief of this image. Previous research indicates that this inversion is caused by assumptions related to overhead illumination, global convexity and viewpoint above the surface. In our article, we describe the inverted waves effect, in which turning an image of waves upside down changes its relief and also its perceived material properties. Keywords shape from shading, materials, surface perception, perception of water If there is a limited amount of information about scene structure, the visual system makes assumptions to reach an interpretation of a visual scene. For example, when circular patches containing a shading gradient, such as that in Figure 1(a), are viewed, the observer perceives the surrounding patches as convex bulges and the central patch as a concave indentation. If the image is turned upside down, as in Figure 1(b), the three-dimensional (3D) interpretation of the image changes: the concave areas become convex, and the convex areas become concave. This is a well-known effect (Brewster, 1826; Rittenhouse, 1786; von Fieandt, 1949) that is related to the visual system s assumption that the light source is above the image (Ramachandran, 1988). Such assumptions about the light source are also made when viewing more complex grayscale images such as in Figure 1(c) and (d). Turning Figure 1(c) upside down, as shown in Figure 1(d), results in a perception where the convex and concave parts of the image change. However, Reichel and Todd (1990) showed that shading is not necessary for the inversion effect. For example, when Figure 1(e) is turned upside down, as shown in Figure 1(f), the image relief changes. The change occurs because the visual system makes the assumption that the image is being viewed from overhead, which means that we perceive images as being backwardly slanted away from us. Because of this, we assume that depth increases with height in the visual field (Reichel and Todd, 1990). Further assumptions were Corresponding author: Jukka Häkkinen, Institute of Behavioural Sciences, PO Box 9, 00014 University of Helsinki, Finland. Email: jukka.hakkinen@helsinki.fi Creative Commons CC-BY: This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License (http://www.creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/) which permits any use, reproduction and distribution of the work without further permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access pages (http://www. us.sagepub.com/aboutus/openaccess.htm).

2 i-perception 7(1) Figure 1. (a) A circular patch with a gradient is perceived as 3D. The surrounding patches are observed as bulges, and the central patch is perceived as flat or concave. (b) When the image is turned upside down, the depth interpretation changes: the concave areas are observed as convex, and the convex areas are observed as concave. (c) A shaded surface. (d) When the surface in c is turned upside down, the depth relief is inverted. This is most easily visible in the center of the image, which has changed from convex to concave. (e) A surface where shading has been replaced with line texture. (f) When the textured image in e is turned upside down, the depth relief changes.

Häkkinen and Gröhn 3 Figure 2. (a) A photograph of waves. (b) Turning the photograph upside down changes both the 3D structure of the scene and the interpretation of its material properties. The upside-down version of the image is most commonly described as a rocky surface or a microscopic image of human tissue. demonstrated by Langer and Bu lthoff (2001), whose experiments showed that the visual system assumes global convexity in images. This effect was later confirmed with photorealistic images by Liu and Todd (2004). Related to the previous depth inversion phenomena, in this study, we observed an image for which inversion causes a change not only in the relief but also in the perceived material properties of the image. Figure 2(a) shows a grayscale photograph of waves. When the image is turned upside down, in Figure 2(b), the 3D properties of the image change. In other words, the convex parts of the image become concave, and the concave parts become convex. Interestingly, the material properties of the surface also change, as the upside-down waves now appear to be completely different. Observations within our research group suggested that some people perceived the inverted image as a rocky surface, whereas others observed it as a microscopic image of human tissue. This effect can also be produced by turning one s head upside down, as in other similar demonstrations. To explore the differences between interpretations of upright and inverted images, we conducted an experiment in which observers were asked to describe both images. The experiment was conducted with 67 observers (51 females and 16 males) who were students at the Institute of Behavioural Sciences, University of Helsinki. The stimuli were displayed on an Eizo ColorEdge CG241W display with a resolution of 1920 pixels in width and 1200 pixels in height. A separate, smaller display was on the table, and it was used for showing the text box used for answering. The viewing distance was 80 cm, and the size of the images was 9.6 7.4 of visual angle. Observers viewed the two images in a randomized order and answered the question What does this image depict? by writing a free answer with a computer keyboard. The presentation time was not limited. The results show that inverting the image changed the viewers interpretation of the surface material. The leftmost blue bar in Figure 3 shows that all participants recognized the upright image as water. In contrast, there was more variation in the descriptions of the inverted image, as shown by the rightmost bars in Figure 3. Most of the participants (frequency (f) ¼ 37 of 67) described the image as a solid material such as rock (f ¼ 28) or as a microscopic image of human tissue (f ¼ 8). Several participants described the surface as water (f ¼ 26), but most of these (f ¼ 19 of 26) described it as rapids, not as waves. There were

4 i-perception 7(1) Figure 3. Frequency of answers belonging to the water or solid categories. Blue bars indicate the frequency of water category answers, and black bars indicate the frequency of solid category answers. The leftmost bars show the results for the upright image, and the rightmost bars show the results for the inverted image. The numbers above the bars indicate the frequency of answers. Note that in the inverted image condition, four answers have been removed because the participants indicated both categories. Figure 4. (a) A photograph of waves. (b) Turning this photograph upside down does not change the 3D interpretation or the interpretation of the surface material. (Images are in the public domain.) also four participants who said that the surface could be either water or a solid surface. These four answers were omitted from further analysis. Most likely, a number of variables affected the result. First, in one-half of the experimental cases, the participants observed the waves image first. This initial exposure might have primed them to interpret the inverted image as water. Second, the foam at the top left part of Figure 2(a) is a cue for water that stays invariant in the inverted image (bottom left area of Figure 2(b)). Using a stimulus without this additional cue might reduce the number of people

Häkkinen and Gröhn 5 who interpret the inverted image as water. Third, the method of giving free answers increased the variability in the results. The effect needs to be quantified with a more exact methodology. This phenomenon, which we call the inverted waves effect, is probably related to the visual characteristics of fluids. Paulun, Kawabe, Nishida, & Fleming (2015) noticed that the perceived viscosity of fluids varies systematically with the 2D shape statistics of images. Their experimental stimuli consisted of computer simulations of fluid pouring from a source into a reservoir, so the stimulus properties were different from our wave images, which are interpreted as having specific visual structures (Kung & Richards, 1988). It would be interesting to further investigate what these visual properties are and how they are used for fluid categorization in images with waves. We noted that the effect is difficult to reproduce with wave images found from Internet searches. An example of this is shown in Figure 4(a), which does not change its material properties when turned upside down, as shown in Figure 4(b). This suggests that there are specific conditions for the effect, and these need to be defined in further research. Acknowledgements I would like to thank Mikko Nuutinen for his help with Figure 1(c) (f). Declaration of Conflicting Interests The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Funding The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This research was supported by funding from the Academy of Finland (Project number 267061). References Brewster, D. (1826). On the optical illusion of the conversion of cameos into intaglios, and of intaglios into cameos, with an account of other analogous phenomena. Edinburgh Journal of Science, 4, 99 108. Kung, T. J., & Richards, W. A. (1988). Inferring Water from images. In W. Richards (Ed.), Natural computation (pp. 224 233). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Langer, M. S., & Bu lthoff, H. H. (2001). A prior for global convexity in local shape-from-shading. Perception, 30, 403 410. Liu, B., & Todd, J. T. (2004). Perceptual biases in the interpretation of 3D shape from shading. Vision Research, 44, 2135 2145. Paulun, V. C., Kawabe, T., Nishida, S., & Fleming, R. W. (2015). Seeing liquids from static snapshots. Vision Research, 115, 1 12. Ramachandran, V. S. (1988). Perception of shape from shading. Nature, 331, 163 166. Reichel, F. D., & Todd, J. T. (1990). Perceived depth inversion of smoothly curved surfaces due to image orientation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 653 664. Rittenhouse, D. (1786). Explanation of an optical deception. Transactions of the American Philosophical Society, 2, 37 42. von Fieandt, K. (1949). The phenomenological problem of light and shadow. Acta Psychologica, 6, 337 357.

6 i-perception 7(1) Author Biographies Jukka Ha kkinen earned his doctorate in psychology (PhD) from the University of Helsinki. Currently, he is a university researcher in the Visual Cognition Research Group, Institute of Behavioural Sciences, University of Helsinki, and an adjunct professor in the Department of Computer Science, Aalto University School of Science. His current research interests include reality-likeness cues in images, hand-eye coordination in real and virtual object manipulation and perception of image quality. Lauri Gro hn has a Master of Science in theoretical physics from the University of Helsinki. He has worked at Kone Ltd., Nokia Ltd., and the Finnish Funding Agency for Technology and Innovation and with the European Commission on various managerial and expert tasks. Currently, he owns a company called Synestesia Software Music, which generates music from images (www.synestesia.fi).