assumptions-usually of uniformity of the retinal areas concernedabout

Similar documents
suffices for vision, then at least 5 quanta must be absorbed within an

scotopic, or rod, vision, and precise information about the photochemical

The best retinal location"

QUANTITATIVE STUDY OF VISUAL AFTER-IMAGES*

We have already discussed retinal structure and organization, as well as the photochemical and electrophysiological basis for vision.

Recovery of Foveal Dark Adaptation

fringes were produced on the retina directly. Threshold contrasts optical aberrations in the eye. (Received 12 January 1967)

J. Physiol. (I952) i 6,

Vision. The eye. Image formation. Eye defects & corrective lenses. Visual acuity. Colour vision. Lecture 3.5

SMALL VOLUNTARY MOVEMENTS OF THE EYE*

J. Physiol. (I954) I23,

Optical, receptoral, and retinal constraints on foveal and peripheral vision in the human neonate

MAXWELL'S SPOT AND LOCAL DIFFERENCE OF COLOUR RESPONSE IN HUMAN RETINA. KOSAKU ISOBE* Department of Physiology, Tohoku University, Sendai

THRESHOLD INTENSITY OF ILLUMINATION AND FLICKER FREQUENCY FOR THE EYE OF THE SUN-FISH

Threshold measurements of spectral sensitivity in a blue monocone monochromat. Joel Pokorny, Vivianne C. Smith, and Richard Swartley

background, for a green grating on a red background, and for a red grating Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan, U.S.A. (Received 30 November 1967)

An analysis of retinal receptor orientation

[ 39 ] THE SHADOW REACTION OF DIADEMA ANTILLARUM PH1LIPPI

Introduction. Chapter Aim of the Thesis

Very short introduction to light microscopy and digital imaging

Physics 345 Pre-lab 1

BRIGHTNESS DISCRIMINATION AS A FUNCTION OF THE DURATION OF THE INCREMENT IN INTENSITY

Chapter Ray and Wave Optics

Chapter 6. Experiment 3. Motion sickness and vection with normal and blurred optokinetic stimuli

Effect of Stimulus Duration on the Perception of Red-Green and Yellow-Blue Mixtures*

porphyropsin) in crocodiles and some fish with white tapeta, while they were

This article reprinted from: Linsenmeier, R. A. and R. W. Ellington Visual sensory physiology.

Digital Image Processing

Simple method of determining the axial length of the eye

Simultaneous brightness contrast for flashes of light of different durations. Mathew Alpern

length from 10 to 60'. On the other hand, meridional amblyopes have substantial

PROCEEDINGS OF A SYMPOSIUM HELD AT THE CAVENDISH LABORATORY, CAMBRIDGE, Edited by

Lecture 8. Lecture 8. r 1

Visibility, Performance and Perception. Cooper Lighting

The Human Eye and a Camera 12.1

Appearance at the low-radiance end of HDR vision: Achromatic & Chromatic

Yokohama City University lecture INTRODUCTION TO HUMAN VISION Presentation notes 7/10/14

Light and Dark Adaptation

Fundamental Optics of the Eye and Rod and Cone vision

On spatial resolution

Evaluation of High Intensity Discharge Automotive Forward Lighting

2 The First Steps in Vision

THE DARK ADAPTATION OF THE EYE OF THE HONEY BEE

OPTICAL DEMONSTRATIONS ENTOPTIC PHENOMENA, VISION AND EYE ANATOMY

DOING PHYSICS WITH MATLAB COMPUTATIONAL OPTICS. GUI Simulation Diffraction: Focused Beams and Resolution for a lens system

Color Assimilation and Contrast near Absolute Threshold

Cone spacing and waveguide properties from cone directionality measurements

Simple reaction time as a function of luminance for various wavelengths*

Human Visual System. Prof. George Wolberg Dept. of Computer Science City College of New York

The eye* The eye is a slightly asymmetrical globe, about an inch in diameter. The front part of the eye (the part you see in the mirror) includes:

binocular projection by electrophysiological methods. An account of some METHODS

Chapter 25. Optical Instruments

TRAFFIC SIGN DETECTION AND IDENTIFICATION.

INVESTIGATIVE OPHTHALMOLOGY. Densitometry of pigments in rods and cones of normal and color defective subjects

Exam 4. Name: Class: Date: Multiple Choice Identify the choice that best completes the statement or answers the question.

A. A. Kishk and A. W. Glisson Department of Electrical Engineering The University of Mississippi, University, MS 38677, USA

themselves, and this is quite insufficient for such a purpose. For this exactly why they should be obtained, rather than other values.

Ron Liu OPTI521-Introductory Optomechanical Engineering December 7, 2009

COMMUNICATIONS THE ACCOMMODATION REFLEX AND ITS STIMULUS* powerful stimulus to this innervation is to be found in the disparity

Exp No.(8) Fourier optics Optical filtering

THE RECEPTIVE FIELDS OF OPTIC NERVE FIBERS

Visual Perception of Images

PMT tests at UMD. Vlasios Vasileiou Version st May 2006

Galbraith A Method for High-power

OPTICAL SYSTEMS OBJECTIVES

4th International Congress of Wavefront Sensing and Aberration-free Refractive Correction ADAPTIVE OPTICS FOR VISION: THE EYE S ADAPTATION TO ITS

VC 11/12 T2 Image Formation

Section 2 concludes that a glare meter based on a digital camera is probably too expensive to develop and produce, and may not be simple in use.

DIGITAL IMAGE PROCESSING LECTURE # 4 DIGITAL IMAGE FUNDAMENTALS-I

The Human Visual System. Lecture 1. The Human Visual System. The Human Eye. The Human Retina. cones. rods. horizontal. bipolar. amacrine.

Retinal contrast losses and visual resolution with obliquely incident light

Why is blue tinted backlight better?

VC 16/17 TP2 Image Formation

Psych 333, Winter 2008, Instructor Boynton, Exam 1

Retinal stray light originating from intraocular lenses and its effect on visual performance van der Mooren, Marie Huibert

Applications of Adaptive Optics for Vision Science

Analysis of retinal images for retinal projection type super multiview 3D head-mounted display

Estimation of spectral response of a consumer grade digital still camera and its application for temperature measurement

GROUPING BASED ON PHENOMENAL PROXIMITY

Macula centred, giving coverage of the temporal retinal. Disc centred. Giving coverage of the nasal retina.

Chapter 2: Digital Image Fundamentals. Digital image processing is based on. Mathematical and probabilistic models Human intuition and analysis

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I

Visual Effects of Light. Prof. Grega Bizjak, PhD Laboratory of Lighting and Photometry Faculty of Electrical Engineering University of Ljubljana

Appendix C: Graphing. How do I plot data and uncertainties? Another technique that makes data analysis easier is to record all your data in a table.

Design Description Document

GEOMETRICAL OPTICS Practical 1. Part I. BASIC ELEMENTS AND METHODS FOR CHARACTERIZATION OF OPTICAL SYSTEMS

Unit 8: Light and Optics

resolving power of the optics based on consideration of diffraction and the objective measurements of the quality of the optics of man in vivo have

VC 14/15 TP2 Image Formation

Köhler Illumination: A simple interpretation

dyed films of gelatin which transmitted a band in the extreme red King's College, Cambridge.

Digital Image Processing COSC 6380/4393

ECEN 4606, UNDERGRADUATE OPTICS LAB

Method for the characterization of Fresnel lens flux transfer performance

Limulus eye: a filter cascade. Limulus 9/23/2011. Dynamic Response to Step Increase in Light Intensity

Further reading. 1. Visual perception. Restricting the light. Forming an image. Angel, section 1.4

ISO INTERNATIONAL STANDARD. Photography Root mean square granularity of photographic films Method of measurement

OCULAR MEDIA* PHOTOGRAPHIC RECORDING OF OPACITIES OF THE. development by the control of diabetes, the supply of a deficient hormone

Reading. 1. Visual perception. Outline. Forming an image. Optional: Glassner, Principles of Digital Image Synthesis, sections

Optical design of a high resolution vision lens

Transcription:

364 J. Physiol. (1962), 16, pp. 364-373 With 5 text-figures Printed in Great Britain THE RELATIONSHIP OF VISUAL THRESHOLD TO RETINAL POSITION AND AREA By P. E. HALLETT,*, F. H. C. MARRIOTT AND F. C. RODGER From the University Laboratory of Physiology, Oxford (Received 16 August 1961) The absolute visual threshold of a field on zero background covering a given retinal area in the fully dark-adapted eye depends on at least three factors: the proportion of the incident light absorbed by the photosensitive pigment, the summation properties of the retinal area concerned, and the number of acting quanta necessary to evoke a visual response. This number may, of course, depend on the distribution, in space and time, of the quanta. The relationship between the areas and the thresholds of fields centred at a given retinal position has often been used as the basis for theories about the number of acting quanta required, but such deductions depend on assumptions-usually of uniformity of the retinal areas concernedabout the first two points. The present experiments were undertaken to test the validity of such assumptions, and to investigate the possibility of formulating a plausible theory of visual performance in threshold and simple acuity experiments on the basis of 'visual units', groups of receptors acting together. The experiments on acuity are described elsewhere (Hallett, 1961). METHODS Apparatus. Figure 1 is a diagram of the apparatus. The light source, B, is a 12 V, 48 W single-filament car headlamp bulb, under-run at 11 2 V from a 14 A-hr car battery. A wide-aperture condenser, C, forms a small image of the filament in the plane of a pendulum shutter, S, before illuminating an opal glass, (diameter 25 mm). The shutter gives exposures of 2-6 msec. A green filter, F, Ilford 64 with maximum transmission at about 52 m,u, and neutral wedges, W, are mounted in front of the opal. A battery of large lenses, L, about 3 cm in diameter, collecting over a wide angle, forms an image of the opal glass in the plane of an artificial pupil, P, 2 mm in diameter, in front of the eye, E. The image of the opal source is considerably larger than the artificial pupil. The front lens of the battery is then seen in Maxwellian view as a uniform field 46 in diameter. Metal test objects, diaphragms of known size, can be placed in front of this large field (in the plane X Y) to give smaller test fields of accurately known area and position. Near the centre of the field in the plane of the test objects, a length of 1 cm subtends 2 at the eye. The subject's head is fixed by biting on his dental impression in dentist's wax. He controls the shutter himself and can release it at a chosen moment. An assistant records his response * Present address: Department of Neurology, The Churchill Hospital, Oxford.

VISUAL THRESHOLD; POSITION AND AREA 365 to each flash ('seen' or 'not seen'), varies the intensity with neutral wedges, and changes the test object. A small red fixation point can be placed at any angle up to 25 with the axis of the apparatus; eccentricities up to 35 can be achieved by displacing the fields 1 from the axis of the apparatus. Calibration&. The spectral transmission curve of the filter Ilford 64 was measured by the National Physical Laboratory. The transmission of the neutral wedges was measured, for the light transmitted by the filter, by comparison with sector disks. The light flux transmitted by the colour filter through the artificial pupil, with the wedges out, was measured by comparison with a standard source (calibrated by the National Physical Laboratory) both visually with a Macbeth type of illuminometer, and photo-electrically with a Spectra Spot Meter. El-~ ~ x... i S 1 Fig. 1. Diagrammatic plan of the apparatus. An image of the single vertical filament of the electric bulb, B, is formed by the condenser, C, in the plane of the pendulum shutter, ss. The light then illuminates the opal glass disk,. An (imperfect) image of the opal disk,, is formed in the plane of the artificial pupil, P, by the large lenses, L. The spectral composition and intensity of the light are controlled by the colour filter, F, and the 'neutral' wedges, W. The eye, E, ofthe subject sees the lenses, L, in Maxwellian view. Test objects are placed in the plane XY in front ofl. The fixation point is not shown on the diagram. Light screening is also not represented. Experimental procedure. Each field was presented at a series of intensities, equally spaced on the logarithmic scale, ranging from levels at which the flash was never seen to levels at which it was always seen. The intensities were arranged in random order and usually covered a range of about 1 log. unit. In each series of about 1 exposures a 'blank' was included, in which, unknown to the subject, the light was completely cut off. The threshold was determined by the method described by Hartline & McDonald (1957). The intensity step used was usually 1 log. unit. Standard errors were estimated from separate determinations of the frequency-of-seeing curve. Determinations in which the blank was reported seen were ignored. This occurred twice in about 1 presentations of the blank. All experiments were carried out after the subject had been dark-adapted in total darkness for at least half an hour. 24 Physiol. 16

366 P. E. HALLETT, F. H. C. MARRIOTT AND F. C. RODGER RESULTS Thresholds for 1' field near 2 nasal Thresholds were measured for each of 117 positions (subject F.C.R.) in a square of side 6 centred at 2 nasal to the fixation point on the horizontal meridian, as shown in Fig. 2. In each experiment thresholds were measured in 13 positions along a horizontal line. The positions ranged from 17 to 23 eccentricity in the nasal field by j steps. Nine different horizontal lines were used, namely the meridian through the fixation point and positions 1, 2 21 and 3 above and below it. Two experiments, on different days, were done for each line. In one of these the thresholds were measured in order from 17 to 23, in the other in the reverse order. +3-+3- ~~~~~~~~~... - x... 3 ~~~~~~~~~... -3-... I I I I, I 5 1 15 17 2 23 Fig. 2. Diagram of the 117 positions for which thresholds were measured around 2 nasal field for subject F.C.R. Thirteen positions at I' intervals from 17 to 23 eccentricity were used for each of 9 horizontal lines, the horizontal meridian and positions 1, 2, 2j and 3 above and below it. The fixation point is marked by x. Analysis of the results gives the following conclusions: (1) The standard error of a single measurement was + -12 log. units. (2) No marked irregularities, such as small areas of very low sensitivity, were detected. (3) Variations from day to day were highly significant. (4) Differences between the horizontal lines were not significant. This comparison was not very accurate, since it involved differences between days; nevertheless, there was no indication of any systematic differences between lines. (5) Differences between positions on the lines were highly significant, and indicated a roughly linear rise in log threshold as the position moved from 17 to 23 from the fixation point. The mean values are given in Table 1.

VISUAL THRESHOLD; POSITION AND AREA 367 TABLE 1. Thresholds (means of 18 observations) for different eccentricities Eccentricity Threshold in nasal field Wedge energy (degrees) density (quanta) 17 1*78 84 17j 1-77 86 18 1-7 12 181 1*71 99 19 1*7 12 19j 1-71 99 2 1.71 99 2i 1*73 95 21 1-72 97 21j 1-68 16 22 1*64 117 221 1*64 117 23 1*63 119 Standard error of each value + -3 Thresholds for 1' field on horizontal meridian Thresholds for the 1' field (subject F.C.R.) were measured on the horizontal meridian at positions from 2 to 23 eccentricity in the nasal field, by I' steps. Each threshold is the mean of two observations at least. The range from 2 to 23 was covered in five overlapping experiments. so that allowance could be made for any consideirable day-to-day variations. The results are shown in Fig. 3. The threshold falls steadily up to 9 eccentricity. From 9 to 15 it is fairly constant. From 15 to 23 there is a slow rise in threshold; this is somewhat masked by random variations, and does not show up so clearly as in the first experiment. The standard error of each value is about + 9 log, units. Thresholds for fields of different sizes Thresholds were measured for fields of 1', 1 and 442 diameter (subject F.C.R.) at eccentricities 5, 1, 15, 2, 25, and 3 in the nasal field on the horizontal meridian. Each field size was used in four experiments, two covering the range 5-2 eccentricity and two 1-3. The results are shown in Fig. 4. The standard error of each value is + 9 log. units. For the 1' field the maximum sensitivity is at 15 eccentricity, thresholds at 1 and 2 both being higher. The difference is just significant (P <.5) for 1-15, but not quite significant for 15-2; the latter difference, however, is confirmed by the first experiment. For the 1 field thresholds at 15 and 2 are nearly equal. At 1 the threshold is just significantly higher (P <.5). For the 442 field the threshold at 2 is rather lower than that at 15 (not significant), while the threshold at 1 is definitely higher (P <.1). 24-2

368 P. E. HALLETT, F. H. C. MARRIOTT AND F. C. RODGER 2-5F _ C C" - 2-1-2C o *- 5 *---- * @ *- *. S *~~. * 5 1-5 I 5 1 15 Eccentricity Fig. 3. Thresholds for 1' field (subject F. C. R.) at different eccentricities along the horizontal meridian in the nasal field. I 2 25 3 C =3 4-6 C 2 5 1 15 2 Eccentricity 25 3 Fig. 4. Thresholds for fields of diameters 1', 1 and 412 at eccentricities between 5 and 3 on the horizontal meridian of the nasal field. The symbols show approximately the sizes of the 1 and 412 fields. Each point is the mean of determinations on 2 days, and has standard error + 9. The thresholds for 5, 1, 15 and 2 (continuous line) were measured on each of 2 days; those for 1, 15, 2, 25 and 3 (dotted line) on each of 2 different days.

VISUAL THRESHOLD; POSITION AND AREA 369 The relationship between threshold and area for five retinal positions Thresholds were measured (subject P. E. H.) for concentric circular fields of different areas in five different retinal positions. The results are shown in Fig. 5. The standard error of each point is + 7 log. units. Both scales are logarithmic. Log diameter is plotted at twice the magnification of the ordinate scale, so that log area (upper scale at the top of the figure), is the same as the ordinate scale. The initial horizontal part of each plot represents the area of complete summation. No summation (threshold intensity independent of area) would be represented 2 1 1 log area 6 A I ~~~~~~~~ 5- B 4C4., o4 - D 3 3 2 -E 4 1' 15' 3' 1 2 4 8 Diameter Fig. 5. Thresholds (subject P.E.H.) for concentric circular fields of different diameters in different retinal positions. The intensity and diameter scales are both logarithmic. The position of a curve depends on experimental accuracy (S.E. + -7 for a single point) and on day-to-day variations (as much as 3 log.unit). The average position of a curve can be determined only by very many experiments. Continuous lines are drawn through the average of days, given the same symbol, which by chance reasonably coincide. The broken lines represent the 8hape8 of the curves, not their mean positions, and are obtained by displacing one set of comparable points (continuous line) to agree with another set, or by averaging. The top four curves have been displaced from their true position by whole numbers of log.units. A, 5 nasal field displaced upwards by 4 log.energy units; 6 days. B, 7 temporal field displaced upwards by 3 log.units; 2 days. C, 7 nasal field displaced upwards by 2 log. energy units; 6 days. D, 2 nasal field displaced upwards by 1 log.unit; 4 days. E, 35 nasal field-correct locus; 8 days. Field diameters: 4', 1', 15', 21', 3', 42', 1, 1.41, 2, 2.82, 4, 5.46, 8.

37 P. E. HALLETT, F. H. C. MARRIOTT AND F. C. RODGER by a line of slope 1. Piper's law, energy proportional to the square root of area, corresponds to the line of slope 1. In curves, A, B, C (eccentricities 5, 7 nasal, 7 temporal field), summation is complete up to about 3' diameter. For D (eccentricity 2 nasal field) summation is complete to 1 diameter. For E (eccentricity 35 nasal field) some of the values are anomalous, but there appears to be complete summation up to 2 diameter. DISCUSSION The area-threshold relationship has been used as evidence for the twoquanta theory of visual thresholds (Bouman & van der Velden, 1947; Baumgardt, 1948). The argument depends on two things: first, that the retinal area considered is uniform, or effectively uniform; secondly, that Piper's law (threshold energy proportional to the square root of area) holds over a considerable range of areas. The present results cast doubt on both these points. Baumgardt (1959) has recently attempted to interpret the observed area-threshold relationship taking into account the non-uniformity of the retinal area concerned. The areas covered by the test field were corrected for retinal sensitivity, using the results for a 1 field obtained on different subjects by Pirenne & Marriott (1954). The author concluded that Piper's law was valid for the corrected, as well as the uncorrected, results. The calculation does not allow, however, for differences in size of summation area over the retinal area considered. Non-uniformity of the retinal area can arise in several ways. First, there may be local fluctuations in the sensitivity to smal fields; for example, areas of high threshold possibly associated with blood vessels. These have been observed in other subjects (Stiles & Crawford, 1937). The experiments on F.C.R. gave no indication of any such effect. Fundus photographs of this subject showed that the temporal retinal field investigated had only small blood vessels, and the thresholds measured with a 1' diameter field showed great dependence on distance from the fovea, but no marked local variations. The largest blood vessels in this region were estimated to be of the order of 3,u diameter. This figure is based on a comparison with the largest vessels in the region of the blind spot and on the results of de Margerie (196). Since the image of the 1' field is at least 6, in diameter (allowing for diffraction at the 2 mm artificial pupil), it is perhaps not surprising that they had little influence. In the case of P. E. H. the high threshold for a 21' field at 35 nasal might possibly be attributed to such variations, and there is also a suggestion that at 2 nasal the threshold for a 1 field is lower, in terms of

VISUAL THRESHOLD; POSITION AND AREA 371 total energy, than those for 4' and 1'. The reasons for these findings are not clear. Other authors have studied retinal heterogeneity; for a discussion of the literature see Pirenne (1956). The possible reasons for variation in threshold with retinal position are discussed in Cabello & Stiles (195); see also personal communication from Stiles cited in Pirenne (1956). Secondlv, sensitivity to a small field varies widely with retinal position, and in particular with distance from the fovea. The great range of this variation is shown in Fig. 3. Obviously no valid conclusions can be drawn if uniformity is assumed for parts of the retina which extend into regions near the fovea. Further, Table 1 shows that even for a restricted range of eccentricities in a region generally regarded as comparatively homogeneous there may be marked variations. Between 17 and 23, for subject F. C.R., the threshold for a 1' field rose by 4 %. Finally, even if an area of the retina is found which, for the subject used, shows little or no variation in threshold for a small field, the assumption ofuniformity may be invalidated by variations in the summation properties. The experiments with P. E. H. show clearly that the area within which total summation holds is different in different parts of the retina. The simple hypothesis of a retinal mosaic of identical visual units cannot be maintained. If such units exist they must vary in effective area according to retinal position, and further evidence (Pirenne, 1946; Hallett, 1961) shows that units of very different sizes must be active at different intensities in the same retinal area. The status of Piper's law has been the subject of considerable discussion. For very small areas threshold energy is independent of field size (Ricco's law). For very large areas threshold intensity is effectively independent of field size. There is an intermediate range, and for some experimental conditions a square-root relationship gives a good fit over a considerable range of areas (Baumgardt, 1959). Baumgardt (1959), and Bouman & van der Velden (1947) regard this as a fundamental law; Barlow (1958), on the other hand, concludes from a long series of experiments that 'temporal and spatial summation in human vision are complex, interrelated phenomena which are not adequately described by the simple empirical laws (Bunsen-Roscoe, Ricco, and Piper) except within certain rather narrow ranges.' The results of P. E. H. do not suggest that Piper's law has any general or fundamental validity. At 7 there is a fair approximation to the law between about 1 and 8 diameter (Fig. 2C), but at 5, 2, and 35 the slope of the curve is considerably less than -5. On the assumptions of uniformity and identical units the two-quanta hypothesis predicts a slope of -5 and a unit threshold higher than two

372 P."E. HALLETT, F. H. C. MARRIOTT AND F. C. RODGER quanta would lead to steeper slopes. Any break-down of the simple assumptions would reduce the slope of the curve. The present results, therefore, do not clearly contradict either hypothesis, although the slope of 5 for 7 eccentricity, for fields covering an area far from homogeneous, suggests that if a fundamental area-threshold relationship does exist for a hypothetical uniform retina the slope must be greater than 5. Upon the whole the results suggest that, in practice, the effect of area upon threshold depends on the extension of the field to more sensitive areas or units with greater collecting power, rather than on theoretical statistical considerations. The method of threshold measurement used in these experiments is that described by Hartline & McDonald (1957) and fully discussed in Pirenne, Marriott & O'Doherty (1957); it was independently developed by Stiles & Crawford (1934). It has the advantage that the subject is uninfluenced by any knowledge of the nominal intensity of the flash (cf. Pieron, 196) and at the same time it is quick and simple to use Ṫhe relevance of these results to experiments on acuity, and their interpretation in terms of visual units, is discussed elsewhere (Hallett, 1961). SUMMARY 1. The absolute threshold in energy units of blue-green light for detection of a brief (2-6 msec) light flash was measured for different field sizes and for different positions in the extrafoveal retina. 2. For a small test field, 1' in diameter, marked differences in threshold were found at different eccentricities. 3. For larger test fields there were also differences in threshold at different eccentricities. These did not, however, follow the same course as those for small fields; in particular, the position of the field for minimum threshold depended on the size of field. 4. The relationship between area and threshold was examined at different retinal positions. The form of the relationship, and in particular the area of complete spatial summation, varied according to the retinal position. 5. It was concluded that different retinal areas differ, not only in their sensitivity as measured with a small test field, but also in their properties as regards spatial summation. The relevance of this finding to theories concerning the visual threshold is discussed. We are grateful to Dr W. S. Stiles, of the National Physical Laboratory, for the loan of the Macbeth type illuminometer, and to Mr E. W. Allen of the Oxford Eye Hospital for taking photographs of the fundus. The work was supported by research grant B-181 from the National Institute of Neurological Diseases and Blindness, United States Public Health

VISUAL THRESHOLD: POSITION AND AREA 373 Service. The main apparatus, built by Messrs A. and H. Duncan of Aberdeen, had previously been acquired with the help of a grant from the Nuffield Foundation by Dr M. H. Pirenne, who allowed us to use it foi these experiments. One of the authors (F. H. C. M.) received a Medical Research Council personal grant. REFERENCES BARLOw, H. B. (1958). Temporal and spatial summation in background intensities. J. Phy8iol. 141, 337-35. human vision at different BAUMGARDT, E. (1948). Physiol. 31, 269-29. The quantic and statistical bases of visual excitation. J. gen. BAUMGARDT, E. (1959). Visual spatial and temporal summation. Nature, Lond., 184, 1951-1952. BOUMAN, M. A. & VAN DER VELDEN, H. A. (1947). The two-quanta explanation of the dependence of the threshold values and visual acuity on the visual angle and the time of observation. J. opt. Soc. Amer. 37, 98-919. CABELLO, J. & STILES, W. S. (195). Sensibilidad de bastones y conos en la parafovea. An. Soc. esp. Fis. Quim. 46, 251-282. DE MARGERIE, J.-M. (196). A study of the retinal blood vessels at different ages. D.Phil. thesis, University of Oxford. HALLETT, P. E. (1961). Area intensity relations in human dark-adapted vision. Progress in Photobiology, ed. BORGE, C. C. and BUCHMANN, B. Amsterdam: Elsevier. HARTLINE, H. K. & MCDONALD, R. (1957). The frequency of seeing at low illuminations. Published with PIRENNE, M. H., MARRIOTT, F. H. C. & O'DOHERTY, E. F. Spec. Rep. Ser. med. Res. Coun., Lond., 294. PI1aRON, H. (196). Allocution introductive du pr6sident. In Mechanisms of Colour Discrimination, ed. Galifret, Y. London: Pergamon Press. PIRENNE, M. H. (1946). On the variation of visual acuity with light intensity. Proc. Camb. phil. Soc. 42, 78-82. PIRENNE, M. H. (1956). Physiological mechanisms of vision and the quantum nature of light. Biol. Rev. 31, 194-241. PIRENNE, M. H. & MARRIOTT, F. H. C. (1954). Quantum theories of the absolute threshold: the influence of retinal position and area. Optica Acta, 1, 151-155. PIRENNE, M. H., MARRIOTT, F. H. C. & O'DOHERTY, E. F. (1957). Individual differences in night-vision efficiency. Spec. Rep. Ser. med. Res. Coun., Lond., 294. STILES, W. S. & CRAWFORD, B. H. (1934). The liminal brightness increment for white light for different conditions of the foveal and parafoveal retina. Proc. Roy. Soc. B, 116, 55-12. STILES, W. S. & CRAWFORD, B. H. (1937). The effect of a glaring light source on extrafoveal vision. Proc. Roy. Soc. B, 122, 255-28.