Assessing the contribution of binaural cues for apparent source width perception via a functional model

Similar documents
The relation between perceived apparent source width and interaural cross-correlation in sound reproduction spaces with low reverberation

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007

The psychoacoustics of reverberation

A binaural auditory model and applications to spatial sound evaluation

Tone-in-noise detection: Observed discrepancies in spectral integration. Nicolas Le Goff a) Technische Universiteit Eindhoven, P.O.

A CLOSER LOOK AT THE REPRESENTATION OF INTERAURAL DIFFERENCES IN A BINAURAL MODEL

Binaural Hearing. Reading: Yost Ch. 12

Evaluation of a new stereophonic reproduction method with moving sweet spot using a binaural localization model

THE MATLAB IMPLEMENTATION OF BINAURAL PROCESSING MODEL SIMULATING LATERAL POSITION OF TONES WITH INTERAURAL TIME DIFFERENCES

IMPLEMENTATION AND APPLICATION OF A BINAURAL HEARING MODEL TO THE OBJECTIVE EVALUATION OF SPATIAL IMPRESSION

III. Publication III. c 2005 Toni Hirvonen.

Envelopment and Small Room Acoustics

Spectral and temporal processing in the human auditory system

Perception of pitch. Importance of pitch: 2. mother hemp horse. scold. Definitions. Why is pitch important? AUDL4007: 11 Feb A. Faulkner.

AN AUDITORILY MOTIVATED ANALYSIS METHOD FOR ROOM IMPULSE RESPONSES

Perception of pitch. Definitions. Why is pitch important? BSc Audiology/MSc SHS Psychoacoustics wk 4: 7 Feb A. Faulkner.

Intensity Discrimination and Binaural Interaction

The Human Auditory System

THE TEMPORAL and spectral structure of a sound signal

Sound Radiation Characteristic of a Shakuhachi with different Playing Techniques

Predicting localization accuracy for stereophonic downmixes in Wave Field Synthesis

Interaction of Object Binding Cues in Binaural Masking Pattern Experiments

Monaural and Binaural Speech Separation

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

I. INTRODUCTION. NL-5656 AA Eindhoven, The Netherlands. Electronic mail:

Perception of pitch. Definitions. Why is pitch important? BSc Audiology/MSc SHS Psychoacoustics wk 5: 12 Feb A. Faulkner.

Speaker placement, externalization, and envelopment in home listening rooms

Psychoacoustic Cues in Room Size Perception

Auditory Localization

Computational Perception. Sound localization 2

Psycho-acoustics (Sound characteristics, Masking, and Loudness)

QUALITY ASSESSMENT OF MULTI-CHANNEL AUDIO PROCESSING SCHEMES BASED ON A BINAURAL AUDITORY MODEL

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Effects of Reverberation on Pitch, Onset/Offset, and Binaural Cues

Exploiting envelope fluctuations to achieve robust extraction and intelligent integration of binaural cues

Introduction. 1.1 Surround sound

DECORRELATION TECHNIQUES FOR THE RENDERING OF APPARENT SOUND SOURCE WIDTH IN 3D AUDIO DISPLAYS. Guillaume Potard, Ian Burnett

The role of intrinsic masker fluctuations on the spectral spread of masking

QoE model software, first version

Auditory modelling for speech processing in the perceptual domain

A triangulation method for determining the perceptual center of the head for auditory stimuli

A Comparative Study of the Performance of Spatialization Techniques for a Distributed Audience in a Concert Hall Environment

Enhancing 3D Audio Using Blind Bandwidth Extension

The effect of 3D audio and other audio techniques on virtual reality experience

Testing of Objective Audio Quality Assessment Models on Archive Recordings Artifacts

INVESTIGATING BINAURAL LOCALISATION ABILITIES FOR PROPOSING A STANDARDISED TESTING ENVIRONMENT FOR BINAURAL SYSTEMS

Robust Speech Recognition Based on Binaural Auditory Processing

MULTICHANNEL CONTROL OF SPATIAL EXTENT THROUGH SINUSOIDAL PARTIAL MODULATION (SPM)

Audio Engineering Society. Convention Paper. Presented at the 124th Convention 2008 May Amsterdam, The Netherlands

Analysis of Frontal Localization in Double Layered Loudspeaker Array System

Auditory Based Feature Vectors for Speech Recognition Systems

COM325 Computer Speech and Hearing

Binaural auralization based on spherical-harmonics beamforming

Results of Egan and Hake using a single sinusoidal masker [reprinted with permission from J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 22, 622 (1950)].

Perceptual Band Allocation (PBA) for the Rendering of Vertical Image Spread with a Vertical 2D Loudspeaker Array

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Validation of lateral fraction results in room acoustic measurements

Validation of a Virtual Sound Environment System for Testing Hearing Aids

Convention Paper Presented at the 126th Convention 2009 May 7 10 Munich, Germany

FFT 1 /n octave analysis wavelet

HRTF adaptation and pattern learning

Acoustics Research Institute

Hearing and Deafness 2. Ear as a frequency analyzer. Chris Darwin

You know about adding up waves, e.g. from two loudspeakers. AUDL 4007 Auditory Perception. Week 2½. Mathematical prelude: Adding up levels

Capturing 360 Audio Using an Equal Segment Microphone Array (ESMA)

Development and Validation of an Unintrusive Model for Predicting the Sensation of Envelopment Arising from Surround Sound Recordings

Effect of Harmonicity on the Detection of a Signal in a Complex Masker and on Spatial Release from Masking

BIOLOGICALLY INSPIRED BINAURAL ANALOGUE SIGNAL PROCESSING

Spatial audio is a field that

I R UNDERGRADUATE REPORT. Stereausis: A Binaural Processing Model. by Samuel Jiawei Ng Advisor: P.S. Krishnaprasad UG

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Recording and analysis of head movements, interaural level and time differences in rooms and real-world listening scenarios

THE PERCEPTION OF ALL-PASS COMPONENTS IN TRANSFER FUNCTIONS

A classification-based cocktail-party processor

Robust Speech Recognition Based on Binaural Auditory Processing

Technical University of Denmark

Distortion products and the perceived pitch of harmonic complex tones

University of Huddersfield Repository

General overview of spatial impression, envelopment, localization, and externalization

VIRTUAL ACOUSTICS: OPPORTUNITIES AND LIMITS OF SPATIAL SOUND REPRODUCTION

THE PAST ten years have seen the extension of multichannel

Improving room acoustics at low frequencies with multiple loudspeakers and time based room correction

Distance Estimation and Localization of Sound Sources in Reverberant Conditions using Deep Neural Networks

Tara J. Martin Boston University Hearing Research Center, 677 Beacon Street, Boston, Massachusetts 02215

HCS 7367 Speech Perception

SOUND QUALITY EVALUATION OF FAN NOISE BASED ON HEARING-RELATED PARAMETERS SUMMARY INTRODUCTION

Exploiting deep neural networks and head movements for binaural localisation of multiple speakers in reverberant conditions

A study on sound source apparent shape and wideness

6-channel recording/reproduction system for 3-dimensional auralization of sound fields

University of Huddersfield Repository

Convention Paper Presented at the 128th Convention 2010 May London, UK

Sonnet. we think differently!

Multichannel Audio Technologies. More on Surround Sound Microphone Techniques:

Perceptual Distortion Maps for Room Reverberation

ANALYSIS AND EVALUATION OF IRREGULARITY IN PITCH VIBRATO FOR STRING-INSTRUMENT TONES

Interior Noise Characteristics in Japanese, Korean and Chinese Subways

A cat's cocktail party: Psychophysical, neurophysiological, and computational studies of spatial release from masking

Exploiting deep neural networks and head movements for binaural localisation of multiple speakers in reverberant conditions

Listening with Headphones

1856 IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON AUDIO, SPEECH, AND LANGUAGE PROCESSING, VOL. 18, NO. 7, SEPTEMBER /$ IEEE

Transcription:

Virtual Acoustics: Paper ICA06-768 Assessing the contribution of binaural cues for apparent source width perception via a functional model Johannes Käsbach (a), Manuel Hahmann (a), Tobias May (a) and Torsten Dau (a) (a) Hearing Systems Group, Technical University of Denmark, 800 Kgs. Lyngby, Denmark, johk@elektro.dtu.dk Abstract In echoic conditions, sound sources are not perceived as point sources but appear to be expanded. The expansion in the horizontal dimension is referred to as apparent source width (ASW). To elicit this perception, the auditory system has access to fluctuations of binaural cues, the interaural time differences (ITDs), interaural level differences (ILDs) and the interaural coherence (IC). To quantify their contribution to ASW, a functional model of ASW perception was exploited using the TWO!EARS auditory-front-end (AFE) toolbox. The model determines the leftand right-most boundary of a sound source using a statistical representation of ITDs and ILDs based on percentiles integrated over time and frequency. The model s performance was evaluated against psychoacoustic data obtained with noise, speech and music signals in loudspeakerbased experiments. A robust model prediction of ASW was achieved using a cross-correlation based estimation with either IC or ITDs, in contrast to a combination of ITDs and ILDs where the performance slightly decreased. Keywords: Binaural listening, spatial perception, auditory modeling, room acoustics, virtual acoustics

Assessing the contribution of binaural cues for apparent source width perception via a functional model Introduction Spatial perception is a primary function of the human auditory system. It is essential in decoding the auditory scene surrounding a listener. Each sound source in such a scene has a certain location and distance with respect to the listener. This spatial separation helps the listener in distinguishing concurrent sources from each other, e.g. a target speaker from interfering noise sources. The perceived horizontal extent of sound sources is typically described by the apparent source width (ASW). A reduced sensitivity to ASW as, e.g., found in hearing-impaired listeners [7] may have consequences for the ability to spatially separate sound sources. Therefore, it is important to understand the contributing cues to ASW perception. According to literature, three binaural cues are mainly contributing to ASW: The interaural time differences (ITDs) and the interaural level differences (ILDs) which are also important for determining the location of a sound source in the horizontal plane, and the interaural coherence (IC). Due to reflections in rooms and from the head and torso of the listener, all three cues fluctuate over time. With increasing amount of room reflections, the IC decreases and larger variations in ITDs and ILDs occur, leading to an increased ASW. The psychophysical relation between these three binaural cues and ASW can be exploited by binaural auditory models. Traditional models of ASW have been used to evaluate the quality of concert halls by analyzing the interaural cross-correlation (IACC) function []. Based on the IACC, the interaural coherence (IC) is extracted as the absolute maximum value normalized by the root-mean-square (RMS) value of the left- and right-ear signals. Hereby, an inverse relation between IC and ASW exists. Okano et al. (99) [0] proposed a frequency-specific weighting of the IC, termed IACC E, that averages the IC in three octave bands 0., and khz. The IACC E is calculated for the first 80 ms of the binaural impulse recordings (BRIRs) since early reflections are known to contribute mostly to ASW []. Zotter et al. (0) [] observed a high correlation of r = 0.97 between the IACC E and the perceptual data obtained in a stereo loudspeaker measurement setup. Similar ideas as the ones provided by Okano et al. (99) were implemented in a complex binaural auditory model by van Dorp Schuitman et al. (0) [] which splits the input signal in a direct and a reverberant stream. From the direct stream, the model extracts ITDs up to khz as the time-lag at the maximum IC and estimates the ASW by averaging their standard deviation. In contrast to the traditional IC-based measures, this model is applied on binaural recordings. The model showed higher correlations with perceptual data compared to the IACC E. Blauert and Lindemann (986) [] suggested that both, ITD and ILD fluctuations, contribute to ASW. They combined the standard deviation of both cues with equal weights and reported a higher correlation with perceptual data (r = 0.7) as opposed to an IC-based model (r = 0.6). Later, Mason et al. (00) [8] developed an ASW model that combined both ITDs and ILDs according to the duplex theory, by using ITDs at low frequencies and ILDs at high

frequencies [9]. Furthermore, the loudness of the stimuli was estimated and integrated in the model. Also, Okano et al. (99) and van Dorp Schuitman et al. (0) considered the monaural sound pressure level (SPL) as an additional cue for ASW. Thus, several models of ASW have been suggested in the literature, each validated on different perceptual datasets. The present study investigated the generalizability of such models by evaluating their performance across two experimental datasets that were obtained for band limited and broadband noise, as well as speech and music signals (Käsbach et al. 0, 0 [6], [7]). Here, it was investigated whether (i) correlation-based approaches, i.e. using ICs or ITDs (as suggested by Okano et al. (99) and van Dorp Schuitman et al. (0), respectively) are sufficient for the estimation of ASW, (ii) their suggested frequency regions, i.e. three octave bands at 0., and khz or below khz, are optimal in such approaches or whether high-frequency ICs or ITDs also contribute to ASW and (iii) a model combining ITDs and ILDs (as suggested by Blauert and Lindemann, 986, and Mason et al., 00) is feasible. Summary of the perceptual studies Two previously conducted studies on ASW perception (Käsbach et al. 0, 0 [6], [7]), in the following referred to as Exp. A and B, were considered here to develop and evaluate models of ASW perception. Distinct sensations of ASW were generated by using stereo loudspeaker setups. In such a setup, the listener perceives a phantom sound image in the center of the two loudspeakers. The ASW was measured as a function of the physical source width (PSW) which was controlled by two experiment-specific settings, the loudspeaker layout and applied signal processing. In the measurement procedure, listeners indicated the perceived ASW on a degree scale as illustrated in Figure. In Exp. B, listeners could indicate the left and right most boundary of the sound source separately, whereas in Exp A, the response had to be given symmetrically. In the present study, only source signals per experiment were used. In Exp. A, the stereo setup at an angle of ±0 degrees was used indicated by the red dashed rectangles in Figure. Five distinct PSW values, denoted by PSW # to PSW #, were generated by varying the coherence between the two loudspeaker channels accordingly to IC LS =,0.8,0.6,0. and 0. The source signal was either Gaussian white noise, band-pass filtered with a bandwidth of octaves at a center frequency of 0. khz or high-pass (HP) filtered at 8 khz. The stimuli had a duration of s and were presented at 70 db SPL. In Exp. B, the PSW was controlled by varying the angle between the stereo speakers. In addition, a source widening algorithm was applied as described in Zotter et al. (0) []. Specifically, a line-array of stereo loudspeaker pairs (Type Dynaudio BM6) plus an additional loudspeaker in the center of the array was used as indicated by the gray rectangles in Figure. In total, five distinct PSW values were generated. The source signals were pink noise, male speech and a guitar sample. The stimuli had a duration of 6 s and were presented at 70 db SPL. In Figure, the perceived ASW as a function of PSW averaged across listeners is shown for Exp. A (left panel) and Exp. B (right panel). The error bars represent the standard deviation across listeners. It can be seen that ASW increases with increasing PSW. In Exp. A (left panel), the different signal types (represented by the different symbols and line styles) show similar results with a tendency that the bandpass-filtered signal at 0 Hz and the white noise signal were

perceived with larger ASW than the HP filtered signal at 8 khz. In a statistical analysis with a linear mixed-effects model, the factor PSW showed a similar effect size (F(, 8) =.6, p < 0.00) compared to the factor source signal (F(, ) = 97., p < 0.00) which was larger than the interaction of both (F(6, 6) =, p < 0.00). In Exp. B (right panel), it can be seen that ASW increases as well with PSW in a similar manner as in Exp. A. Small differences can be seen between the source signals, such that the noise source was generally perceived to have a larger ASW than the speech and guitar signals. In a statistical analysis with a linear mixed-effects model, the factor PSW showed a dominating effect size (F(, 0) = 0, p < 0.00) compared to the factor source signal (F(, 79) =.8, p < 0.00) and the interaction of both (F(8, 78) = 9., p < 0.00). Figure : Sketch of the experimental set-up. The loudspeaker pairs generate a phantom source at 0 degree. Listeners were asked to indicate the ASW in degree, for both boundaries of the source image. For further details, see [6] and [7]. The ASW model Figure shows a schematic diagram of the model. Binaural recordings were obtained with a head and torso simulator (HATS) that was placed at the listener s position. The functional model consisted of various processing stages, including gammatone filtering, inner haircell transduction (IHC) and absolute threshold of hearing (ATH). Given the binaural signal, the model extracted ITDs, ILDs and IC, in order to predict ASW.. Front-end The auditory processing was based on the auditory-front-end (AFE) developed by the TWO!EARS consortium []. The binaural signals were first analyzed by a gammatone filterbank to represent the frequency selectivity of the basilar membrane. The filters were set to a bandwidth of one equivalent rectangular bandwidth (ERB) in the frequency range between 80 to 89 Hz. In the second stage, the IHC transduction was simulated, i.e. the loss of phase locking to the stimulus fine structure at high frequencies. The IHC processing was performed according to Bernstein et al. (999) [], suggesting a cut-off frequency of Hz and simulating basilar-membrane compression, which Faller and Merimaa (00) [] also applied in an auditory model of localization perception. In a following stage, the activity in each frequency band was estimated. The signals had been calibrated to a root-mean-square (RMS) value corresponding to the 70 db SPL of the experimental stimuli. Frequency bands with an SPL below the ATH as defined in Terhardt (979) [] were not considered further in the processing. In

the last stage, ITDs, ILDs and ICs were calculated per time-frequency units. The signals of both ears were analyzed in short-time hanning windows of 0 ms duration, with an overlap of 0%, which resulted in a time-frequency representation of each ear signal. The IC and ITD were extracted from the normalized interaural cross-correlation function per time-frame. The IC was equal to the maximal coherence and the ITD corresponded to the time-lag at this value. Time-lags were limited to a range of ±. ms. The ILDs were defined as the energy difference in db between the two ear signals. L R Front-end f f IHC ATH IACC f ILD t f ITD t f IC t Back-end Figure : Schematic diagram of the binaural ASW model.. Back-end The ASW estimation was based on the statistical distribution of the binaural cues. The width of this distribution was represented by percentiles and resembled the ASW. Hereby, the leftand right-most boundary of the sound source corresponded to the lower and upper percentile from the distribution s median. Figure shows an example of the percentiles [0 70]% (left and right pointing triangles, respectively) per frequency channel for ITDs (left panel) and ILDs (third panel) in the case of the noise source in Exp. B. The percentiles increase from PSW # (narrow distribution in gray) to PSW # (wide distribution in red), especially for the ITDs. Choosing percentiles that are further away from the median, here illustrated for percentiles [0 90]% (squares and circles, respectively), the values of the ITDs (second panel) and ILDs (fourth panel) increase, but their dynamic range, i.e. the difference between PSW # and PSW #, is similar. For the following analysis, the [0 70]% percentiles were chosen to obtain a higher outlier rejection. The first back-end, termed DUPLEX, combined the percentiles of the ITDs and ILDs according to the duplex theory [9] which was motivated by Blauert and Lindemann (986) and Mason et al. (00). The combination of both binaural cues required the normalization of each cue. ITDs were normalized by. ms and ILDs by db SPL, which corresponded to the observed maxima, respectively, in the percentiles across stimuli. According to the duplex theory, ITDs contribute up to. khz and ILDs contribute above this frequency value. The final prediction of the left and right boundaries was then obtained by calculating the mean value across all frequency channels of the lower and upper percentile, respectively. In a second back-end, termed ITD low, only the ITD-percentiles were analyzed with an upper frequency limit of khz according to van Dorp Schuitman et al. (0). The third back-end used the IC for the ASW prediction, termed IC E, resembling a short-term analysis of the IACC E. In total, 6 gammatone filters of the front-end were selected corresponding to

the frequency range between 0. to.8 khz, defined by the octave-wide filters in IACC E at 0., and khz. The frame-based values of IC were averaged with equal weights across all frames and frequency channels. The IACC E according to Okano et al. (99) served as a reference. A calibration stage was required to map the output of each model to ASW in degrees. Using a linear fitting approach, the calibrated model output was y cal = ay + b, where a is a sensitivity parameter, b an offset and y the uncalibrated model output. For the calibration two data points were used, PSW # and PSW # of the white noise stimulus in Exp. A. 8 8 8 8 Frequency [khz] 0. 0. 0. - -0. 0 0. ITD [s] 0 - Frequency [khz] 0. 0. 0. - -0. 0 0. ITD [s] 0 - Frequency [khz] 0. 0. 0. - - 0 ILD [db] Frequency [khz] 0. 0. 0. - - 0 ILD [db] Figure : ITD-percentiles (left panels) and ILD-percentiles (right panels) as a function of frequency in case of the pink noise source in Exp. B. Shown are the [0 70]% percentiles (left and right pointing triangles, respectively) and the the [0 90]% percentiles (squares and circles, respectively) for PSW # (gray) and PSW # (red). Modeling results The individual model performance was accessed by calculating Pearson s correlation coefficient r and the RMS-error between the calibrated model outputs and all experimental data (left and right boundaries), i.e. for Exps. A and B including all source signals. The corresponding values are displayed in Table. In general, all four models provided a high correlation with the perceptual data (ranging from r = 0.9 to r = 0.97). This is due to the fact that PSW is the dominating factor compared to the source stimulus which is captured correctly by all models. 0. khz white 8 khz noise speech guitar -60-0 -0 0 0 0 60-60 -0-0 0 0 0 60 Figure : Perceptual results of ASW for Exp. A (left panel) and Exp. B (right panel) in degrees. ASW is shown as a function of the physical source width (PSW), denoted by PSW # (narrow) to # (wide). Plotted are the mean and standard deviation. The different symbols and line styles represent the different source signals. In Figure, the outputs of the four tested models, IACC E, IC E, ITD low and DUPLEX are presented for Experiment A (left panels) and for Exp B (right panels). Note that the first two 6

0. khz white 8 khz noise speech guitar -IACC E -IC E -IACC E -IC E ITD low DUPLEX ITD low DUPLEX -60-0 -0 0 0 0 60-60 -0-0 0 0 0 60-60 -0-0 0 0 0 60-60 -0-0 0 0 0 60 Figure : Modeling results of ASW for Exp A (left panels) and Exp B (right panels) in degrees. From top to bottom: IACC E, IC E, ITD low and DUPLEX. ASW is shown as a function of the physical source width (PSW), denoted by PSW # (narrow) to # (wide). The different symbols and line styles represent the different source signals. models are inversely proportional to ASW and are therefore shown as IACC E and IC E, respectively. Further, both models produced a single output value and are therefore shown with a symmetric ASW. It can be seen that all models are able to predict the general trend in the data, i.e. that the perceived ASW increases with PSW. Differences occur with respect to the slopes of the predicted boundaries of the ASW and between source signals. The IACC E model achieves the highest correlation of the considered models with r = 0.97 (r = 0.98 which corresponds to the findings in []) due to the fact that it captures the dynamic range in ASW correctly, i.e. the difference between smallest and largest ASW, for both experiments. However, the model does not capture the increase in ASW for PSW # in Exp. B and does only reveal minor differences between the source signals. Considering the model denoted by IC E, the performance decreases to r = 0.9. This indicates that a short-term analysis of the IC (including the IHC and ATH model stages) and a higher frequency resolution (6 gammatone filters as opposed to octave-wide filters in IACC E ) are not required to account for the perceptual data. The IC E ) predictor has a reduced sensitivity, i.e. a more shallow slope of the boundaries. However, it partially captures source signal differences in Exp. A, e.g. larger ASWs for low frequencies (blue circles) compared to high frequencies (green diamonds), but contradicts the data for the noise source (black rectangles). The ITD low model s performance is with r = 0.9 between the IC E and IACC E models. Since both the IC-based models and ITD low are extracted from the IACC, this result is plausible. Its output shows a dynamic range similar to that in the data and is also more asymmetric due to the fact that the boundaries are estimated separately by the corresponding percentiles, such that a potentially asymmetric HATS positioning becomes more crucial. Hence, prediction errors are caused by the asymmetric output and an overestimation in case of the speech and guitar source signals in Exp B. In Table, the performance of the IACC E, IC E and ITD low models is also shown for the case when including the entire bandwidth for the analysis (denoted with the subscript broad ). The corre- 7

sponding performance is decreased compared to their low frequency estimates. Interestingly, the IACC broad and IC broad result both in r = 0.88, indicating that it becomes irrelevant whether a long- or short-term analysis is performed in this case. The ITD broad model results in r = 0.9. This suggests that high-frequency components in IACC-based measures do not provide useful information for ASW. The DUPLEX model provides a similar output as the ITD low model, but performance decreases to r = 0.9. Therefore, adding ILDs in the analysis does not provide a further benefit. Table : Model performances in terms of correlation coefficient r, r, RMS-error and the Akaike information criterion (AIC). Model r r RMS-error [ ] AIC (dof = ) IACC E 0.97 0.98. 9 IACC broad 0.88 0.9 8.7 - IC E 0.9 0.9 0. 8 IC broad 0.88 0.9.7 - ITD low 0.9 0.96 6. 6 ITD broad 0.9 0.9 7.9 - DUPLEX 0.9 0.9 7.9 7 DUPLEX short 0.87 0.9. - ILD 0.77 0.88.7 - Discussion. Statistical analysis of the ASW models The presented ASW models, IACC E, IC E, ITD low and DUPLEX were compared in a statistical analysis. A -way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed using the model type, PSW and source signal as factors. In contrast to the correlation coefficient r, this allowed for a more detailed model analysis across both factors PSW and source signal. The evaluation was based on the Akaike information criterion (AIC) (using degrees of freedom, dof = ) which is a relative criterion, whereby a lower AIC indicates a better model performance. In such an analysis, listed in Table, the IC E model performed best (AIC = 8), the ITD low and the DUPLEX provided similar performance (AIC = 6 and 7, respectively) and the IACC E (AIC = 9) model performed less well. However, in a post-hoc analysis with Bonferroni correction (correction factor of ), no significant differences (p posthoc < 0.0) between the models could be revealed.. The contribution of ILDs It was shown that including ILDs in the model predictions did not improve the model performance compared to the ITD low model. In Figure, an analysis of the [0 70]% percentiles (marked by the opposite pointing triangles) for the ITDs (left panel) and ILDs (third panel) 8

across frequency is presented for PSW # and PSW # in case of the noise source in Exp. B. While the percentiles of the ITDs increase substantially (roughly by 00 ms) from PSW # to #, the percentiles of the ILDs only increase by less than db for frequencies below khz and, thereby, exploit a small dynamic range of ILD fluctuations. However, this dynamic range is sufficient for a pure ILD model resulting in a correlation of r = 0.77 (see Table ). The analysis window duration and shape play a role in the analysis of the ILDs. A shorter analysis window captures larger instantaneous ILDs which might improve the dynamic range of the ILDs in the percentiles. This was tested for a ms window duration with a 0. ms overlap for the ILD analysis while maintaining the 0 ms window and the 0. ms overlap for the ITD analysis. While for this approach, called DUPLEX short, the dynamic range of ILDs was doubled to db, the correlation further decreased to r = 0.87 (see Table ). Even though the contribution of ILDs to ASW cannot be supported in the current study with the considered stationary stimuli (even for the speech and music signals, the variations across PSW were small), ILDs might become more relevant for the ASW estimation of sound sources in real rooms. 6 Summary and conclusions In this study, two experiments were presented where the ASW was measured as a function of the PSW. The stimuli were analyzed by four binaural functional models to predict ASW. A model that combines ITDs and ILDs according to the duplex theory (DUPLEX) was compared to other existing approaches in the literature, i.e. IACC E, IC E, and ITD low. Models based on the interaural cross-correlation function (either extracting IC or ITD) produced similar results for the estimation of ASW. The best performance was obtained by a long-term analysis of the binaural signals using the IACC E. Apparently, the signals were stationary enough such that a long-term analysis was sufficient. The previously suggested frequency regions for the analysis with cross-correlation based models seems optimal, i.e. averaging across three octave bands at 0., and khz for the IACC E and IC E models and considering frequencies only below khz for the ITD low model. Adding higher frequency components deteriorated the ASW estimation in all models. The DUPLEX model that also included ILDs could not provide any further benefit in the ASW estimation, possibly due to the stationary character of the chosen stimuli. 7 Acknowledgement This work was supported by EU FET grant TWO!EARS (ICT-6807) and by the Centre for Applied Hearing Research which is a research consortium with Oticon, Widex and GNResound. The binaural models were implemented together with Manuel Hahmann and are based on the auditory front-end (AFE) of the TWO!EARS consortium []. References [] Ando, Y. (007): Concert hall acoustics based on subjective preference theory. The Springer Handbook of Acoustics (Springer Science + Business Media, New York), pp. -86. [] Bernstein, L., R., van de Par, S. and Trahiotis, C. (999): The normalized interaural correlation: Accounting for 9

NoSπ thresholds obtained with Gaussian and "low-noise" masking noise. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 06 (), pp. 870-876. [] Blauert, J. and Lindemann, W. (986): Auditory spaciousness: Some further psychoacoustic analyses. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 80 (), pp. -. [] Bradley, J. S. (0): Review of objective room acoustics measures and future needs. Elsevier Applied Acoustics 7, 7-70. [] Faller, C. and Merimaa, J. (00): Source localization in complex listening situations: Selection of binaural cues based on interaural coherence. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 6 (), pp. 07?089. [6] Käsbach, J., May, T., Le Goff, N. and Dau, T. (0): The importance of binaural cues for the perception of ASW at different sound pressure levels. DAGA, Oldenburg. [7] Käsbach, J., Wiinberg, A., May, T., Løve Jepsen, M. and Dau, T. (0): Apparent source width perception in normal-hearing, hearing-impaired and aided listeners. DAGA, Nürnberg. [8] Mason, R., Brookes, T., Rumsey, F. and Neher, T. (00): Perceptually motivated measurement of spatial sound attributes for audio-based information systems. EPSRC Project Reference: GR/R8/0. http://iosr.uk/projects/pmmp/index.php [9] Macpherson, E. A. and Middlebrooks, J. C. (00): Listener weighting of cues for lateral angle: The duplex theory of sound localization revisited, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. (), pp. 9-6. [0] Okano, T., Beranek, L. L., Hidaka, T. (99): Interaural cross-correlation, lateral fraction, and low- and highfrequency sound levels as measures of acoustical quality in concert halls. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 98 (), pp. -6. [] van Dorp Schuitman, J., de Vries, D., Lindau, A. (0): Deriving content-specific measures of room acoustic perception using a binaural, nonlinear auditory model. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. (), pp. 7-8. [] Terhardt, E. (979): Calculating virtual pitch. Hear. Res. Vol., pp. -8. [] TWO!EARS Consortium (0-06): A computational framework for modelling active exploratory listening that assigns meaning to auditory scenes. EU-Project, no. 6807, Coordinator: Prof. Dr. Alexander Raake, TU Berlin. http://twoears.aipa.tu-berlin.de [] Zotter, F., Frank, M. (0): Efficient phantom source widening. Archives of Acoustics, Vol. 8, No., pp. 7-7. 0