Visual computation of surface lightness: Local contrast vs. frames of reference

Similar documents
Limitations of the Oriented Difference of Gaussian Filter in Special Cases of Brightness Perception Illusions

The effect of illumination on gray color

Munker ^ White-like illusions without T-junctions

T-junctions in inhomogeneous surrounds

Three-dimensional spatial grouping affects estimates of the illuminant

Object Perception. 23 August PSY Object & Scene 1

Color Outline. Color appearance. Color opponency. Brightness or value. Wavelength encoding (trichromacy) Color appearance

Lecture 15 End Chap. 6 Optical Instruments (2 slides) Begin Chap. 7 Visual Perception

Chapter 73. Two-Stroke Apparent Motion. George Mather

The human visual system

Color appearance in image displays

Constancy PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 19. Brightness illusions

Don t twinkle, little star!

Contours, Saliency & Tone Mapping. Donald P. Greenberg Visual Imaging in the Electronic Age Lecture 21 November 3, 2016

Brightness with and without perceived transparency: When does it make a difference?

Psych 333, Winter 2008, Instructor Boynton, Exam 1

This is due to Purkinje shift. At scotopic conditions, we are more sensitive to blue than to red.

Color Assimilation and Contrast near Absolute Threshold

Hint: Think about linear systems and how they behave).

Human Vision and Human-Computer Interaction. Much content from Jeff Johnson, UI Wizards, Inc.

Visual Perception of Images

What is Color. Color is a fundamental attribute of human visual perception.

Using Color Appearance Models in Device-Independent Color Imaging. R. I. T Munsell Color Science Laboratory

Additive Color Synthesis

Our visual system always has to compute a solid object given definite limitations in the evidence that the eye is able to obtain from the world, by

STUDY NOTES UNIT I IMAGE PERCEPTION AND SAMPLING. Elements of Digital Image Processing Systems. Elements of Visual Perception structure of human eye

For a long time I limited myself to one color as a form of discipline. Pablo Picasso. Color Image Processing

Lecture 4. Opponent Colors. Hue Cancellation Experiment HUV Color Space

Unit 8: Color Image Processing

Simple Figures and Perceptions in Depth (2): Stereo Capture

The User Experience: Proper Image Size and Contrast

Understanding Optical Illusions. Mohit Gupta

Lecture 2 Digital Image Fundamentals. Lin ZHANG, PhD School of Software Engineering Tongji University Fall 2016

CSE 332/564: Visualization. Fundamentals of Color. Perception of Light Intensity. Computer Science Department Stony Brook University

Higher Visual Mechanisms. Higher Visual Mechanisms

24 Lightness Perception and

Frequency Domain Based MSRCR Method for Color Image Enhancement

The Effect of Opponent Noise on Image Quality

GROUPING BASED ON PHENOMENAL PROXIMITY

Perception: From Biology to Psychology

IMAGES AND COLOR. N. C. State University. CSC557 Multimedia Computing and Networking. Fall Lecture # 10

Color and Perception. CS535 Fall Daniel G. Aliaga Department of Computer Science Purdue University

EC-433 Digital Image Processing

Appearance at the low-radiance end of HDR vision: Achromatic & Chromatic

Visual Perception. Jeff Avery

Contrast, Luminance and Colour

TDI2131 Digital Image Processing

Multiscale model of Adaptation, Spatial Vision and Color Appearance

Resolving Perceptual Ambiguity Visual Rules & Other Factors

icam06, HDR, and Image Appearance

The Use of Color in Multidimensional Graphical Information Display

Paintings, photographs, and computer graphics are calculated appearances

Spatial Judgments from Different Vantage Points: A Different Perspective

CS-2000/2000A. Spectroradiometer NEW

Articulation: brightness, apparent illumination, and contrast ratios

any kind, you have two receptive fields, one the small center region, the other the surround region.

PSSA Calibration and Colour Management

Image Processing Computer Graphics I Lecture 20. Display Color Models Filters Dithering Image Compression

Visual Perception. human perception display devices. CS Visual Perception

Digital Image Processing

CS 4300 Computer Graphics. Prof. Harriet Fell Fall 2012 Lecture 4 September 12, 2012

Cognition and Perception

Vision and Color. Reading. Optics, cont d. Lenses. d d f. Brian Curless CSE 557 Autumn Good resources:

Vision and Color. Brian Curless CSE 557 Autumn 2015

IOC, Vector sum, and squaring: three different motion effects or one?

Vision. The eye. Image formation. Eye defects & corrective lenses. Visual acuity. Colour vision. Lecture 3.5

Visual Rules. Why are they necessary?

NON UNIFORM BACKGROUND REMOVAL FOR PARTICLE ANALYSIS BASED ON MORPHOLOGICAL STRUCTURING ELEMENT:

Creating Digital Artwork

vertical horizonta fovea Figure by MIT OCW.

Color and More. Color basics

Our Color Vision is Limited

Computer Graphics: Graphics Output Primitives Primitives Attributes

Fig Color spectrum seen by passing white light through a prism.

Lecture 4 Foundations and Cognitive Processes in Visual Perception From the Retina to the Visual Cortex

Reference Free Image Quality Evaluation

Spectroradiometer CS-2000/2000A. The world's top-level capability spectroradiometers make further advances with addition of second model to lineup.

Understand brightness, intensity, eye characteristics, and gamma correction, halftone technology, Understand general usage of color

On Contrast Sensitivity in an Image Difference Model

FEATURE. Adaptive Temporal Aperture Control for Improving Motion Image Quality of OLED Display

Capturing Light in man and machine. Some figures from Steve Seitz, Steve Palmer, Paul Debevec, and Gonzalez et al.

The Science Seeing of process Digital Media. The Science of Digital Media Introduction

Quality of the light sources and colour constancy

Spectroradiometer CS-2000/2000A. The world's top-level capability spectroradiometers make further advances with addition of second model to lineup.

Vision and Color. Reading. Optics, cont d. Lenses. d d f. Brian Curless CSEP 557 Fall Good resources:

Vision and Color. Brian Curless CSEP 557 Fall 2016

Slide 1. Slide 2. Slide 3. Light and Colour. Sir Isaac Newton The Founder of Colour Science

An Engineer s Perspective on of the Retina. Steve Collins Department of Engineering Science University of Oxford

Digital Image Processing (DIP)

Chapter 8. Representing Multimedia Digitally

02/02/10. Image Filtering. Computer Vision CS 543 / ECE 549 University of Illinois. Derek Hoiem

Introduction to computer vision. Image Color Conversion. CIE Chromaticity Diagram and Color Gamut. Color Models

Color , , Computational Photography Fall 2018, Lecture 7

Vision and Color. Reading. The lensmaker s formula. Lenses. Brian Curless CSEP 557 Autumn Good resources:

S imultaneous brightness contrast (SBC) is a visual phenomenon in which a mid-luminance test patch

On the intensity maximum of the Oppel-Kundt illusion

The luminance of pure black: exploring the effect of surround in the context of electronic displays

Radiometry vs. Photometry. Radiometric and photometric units

Colour + Perception. CMPT 467/767 Visualization Torsten Möller. Pfister/Möller

The eye, displays and visual effects

Transcription:

1 Visual computation of surface lightness: Local contrast vs. frames of reference Alan L. Gilchrist 1 & Ana Radonjic 2 1 Rutgers University, Newark, USA 2 University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, USA Seeing black, white and gray surfaces, called lightness perception, might seem simple because white surfaces reflect 90% of the light they receive while black surfaces reflect only 3%, and the human retina is composed of light sensitive cells. The problem is that, because illumination varies from time to time and from place to place, any amount of light can be reflected from any shade of gray. Thus the amount of light reflected by an object, called luminance, says nothing about its lightness. Experts agree that the lightness of a surface can be computed only by using the surrounding context, but they disagree about how the context is used. We have tested an image in which two major classes of theory, contrast theories and frame-of-reference theories, make very different predictions regarding what gray shades will be seen by human observers. We show that when frame-of-reference is varied while contrast is held constant, lightness varies strongly. But when contrast is varied but frame-of-reference is held constant, little or no variation is seen. These results suggest that efforts to discover the exact algorithm by which the human visual system segments the image received by the retina into frames of reference should be given high priority. The challenge confronting the human visual system in assigning black, white, and gray shades to visible surfaces is illustrated in Figure 1. Three identical disks have been pasted into this photograph. One appears black, one appears gray and one appears white even though all three send exactly the same amount of light to the eye. Clearly the

2 luminance value of the light reflected by an object says nothing about its perceived lightness value. Or consider Adelson s Checker shadow illusion in Figure 2. Although the two squares A and B are identical in luminance, they appear very different in lightness. These examples make it obvious that the human visual system uses the surrounding context to compute the lightness of a given surface. Here we report a test between two general ways of looking at context: local contrast theories and frame-ofreference theories.

3 Local contrast theories. In perhaps the clearest formulation of this point of view, Wallach proposed in 1948 that object lightness is determined, not by the luminance of a surface, but by the luminance ratio between the surface and its immediate surround 1. Contrast theories 2, 3 and brightness induction theories 4, which derive from Hering 5, are couched in physiological terms, invoking the mechanism of lateral inhibition, but they share with Wallach the view that lightness is directly tied to local relative retinal stimulation. Frame-of-reference theories. This approach takes into account a larger context and exploits more of the structure of the retinal image. The gestalt theorist Koffka proposed that lightness depends on higher order luminance relationships and that the field of illumination in which a surface is embedded serves as a frame of reference against which the luminance of the surface is evaluated 6. Gilchrist has demonstrated that, within such a framework, the highest luminance serves as an anchor and is assigned the value of white. The lightness of other surfaces within the framework depends on the luminance ratio between the surface and the highest luminance 7, 8. These two classes of theory are both consistent with the appearance of the disks in Figure 1. Notice that the three disks have very different disk/surround luminance ratios, but they also lie in very different frameworks of illumination. We sought an image in which the two approaches would predict different lightness values. To this end we pasted four identical disks into the Checker shadow image, as shown in Figure 3, and asked human observers to match each disk to a Munsell chart of gray shades ranging from white to black. If lightness depends primarily on local contrast, the upper and lower rightmost disks should appear as the same shade of gray, because they have the same local contrast (remember that the squares on which they lie are equal in luminance and all the disks are equal in luminance). And the two upper disks that have different local contrast should appear very different from each other, as should the two lower

4 disks. But if lightness is determined primarily by frame of reference, then the two upper disks in the light should appear approximately equal, and much darker than the two lower disks in the shadow, which should also appear approximately equal to each other. The results shown in the graph, confirming what is obvious from mere inspection, are consistent with the frame of reference hypothesis. In short, any effect of local contrast is small relative to the effect of illumination framework. Although the data were analyzed in terms of log reflectance, the results are more intuitively described in Munsell values, ranging from a typical black at Munsell 2.0 to a typical white at Munsell 9.5. The mean lightness matches for each disk are shown in Figure 4a.

5 The average lightness of the disks in the shadow was 2.3 Munsell units (0.49 units of log reflectance) higher than that of the disks in the light. We compared the lightness of each disk with every other disk on the checkerboard using pairwise comparisons, with Boniferroni adjustment for multiple comparisons. We found that each disk in the shadow was perceived as lighter than each disk in the light (p < 0.001), but that the two disks in the shadow did not significantly differ in matched lightness, despite their different local contrasts. Outside the shadow, the disk on the light square

6 appeared darker than the disk on the dark square (p = 0.015) but the difference was very small, 0.6 Munsell units (0.15 units of log reflectance). To confirm that these results are not the product of a particular value of disk luminance, we repeated the experiment using a lighter set of disks, as shown in Figure 4b. We obtained the same pattern of results. The average lightness of the disks in the shadow was 2.8 Munsell units (0.42 units of log reflectance) higher than that of the disks in the light. Pairwise comparisons showed that each disk in the shadow was perceived as lighter than each disk in the light (p < 0.001). And in this case, the two disks in the shadow showed no significant difference in matched lightness, despite their different local contrasts. Nor did the two disks in the light. These results suggest that the dramatically different appearance of the three disks in Figure 1 is due, not to their different local contrasts, but to the different frames of reference within which they lie. This in turn reveals the compelling need for a better grasp of the nature of the software by which the visual system segments a complex scene into frameworks of illumination. Local contrast theories have the distinct advantage of being clearly operationalized, but this does not excuse their clear failure against more sophisticated models. Frame-of-reference theories are criticized as ambiguous but advances have been made. The rules of lightness computation within a framework of illumination are by now quite well worked out 9, 10. Perhaps the biggest remaining challenge concerns how such frameworks are extracted from the complex image on the retina. To a first approximation, there are two main features of the image that functionally segment the image: penumbrae (blurred edges) and depth boundaries (corners and occlusion boundaries) 8, 9.

7 More recently the basic contrast theory of Hering has morphed into more than a dozen models called spatial filtering models. The ODOG model of Blakeslee and McCourt is a prominent example 11. Because these models invoke center/surround receptive fields of different spatial scales, they are not tied to local contrast. Typically one cannot predict how a given model will compute lightness values for a given image without actually feeding the image into a computer program that instantiates the model. But in general, spatial filtering models fail to predict lightness values in images containing pronounced spatial variations in illumination level, as do most real world scenes. Methods The Adelson Checker shadow image was downloaded (source: http://web.mit.edu/persci/people/adelson/checkershadow_downloads.html) and displayed on a 20 Apple Multiple Scan CRT monitor (resolution of 1280 x 960 pixels at 75Hz refresh rate) in a dark room, using Microsoft PowerPoint 2004 software (Microsoft Corporation WA, USA). Four circular gray disks were compressed to ellipsoidal shape, to match the perspective projection of the checkerboard (but only roughly, being done manually in PowerPoint, using the cylinder top as a template). A dark gray set, with triple values of 80 in RGB notation, was shown to one group of 15 observers while a light gray set with values of 127 was shown to another group of 15. At the viewing distance of 80 cm, each disk subtended 2.1 of visual angle horizontally and 1.1 of visual angle vertically, while the entire image subtended 26 horizontally and 20 vertically. Measurements with a Konica Minolta LS-100 spotmeter showed an average luminance of the disks in the dark disk condition of 13.65 cd/m2 (+/- 2%, equivalent to the measuring error) and in the light disk condition 31.3 (+/- 2%). Thus, within a condition, all disks were practically equiluminant. The luminance of the light and the dark check, respectively, was 50.8 cd/m2 and 22.3 cd/m2 in the light and

8 22.3 cd/ m2 and 9.5 cd/m2 in the shadow. The highest luminance on the screen was 71.0 cd/m2. A gray scale housed in a metal chamber and separately illuminated by a 15W fluorescent tube was located just to the right of the observer, who was seated and instructed to pick a chip from the scale for each disk that is the same color (shade of gray) as that disk, like they were cut from the same piece of paper. The scale consisted of 16 Munsell chips (each 1x3 cm) mounted on a white background and arranged in ascending reflectance order from black (Munsell 2.0) to white (Munsell 9.5). The luminance of the white chip was 395 cd/m2. Three observers (one from the dark disks and two from the light disks condition) were excluded because their matches fell 3 standard deviations or more from the mean of the group. Each was replaced by a new observer. References: 1. Wallach, H. (1948). Brightness constancy and the nature of achromatic colors. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 310-324. 2. Jameson, D., and Hurvich, L.M. (1961). Complexities of perceived brightness. Science, 133, 174-179. 3. Cornsweet, T. N. (1970). Visual Perception. New York: Academic Press. 4. Heinemann, E. G. (1955). Simultaneous brightness induction as a function of inducing- and test- inducing luminances. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50, 89-96. 5. Hering, E. (1974/1964). Outlines of a Theory of the Light Sense. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

9 6. Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of Gestalt Psychology. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World, Inc. 7. Li, X., and Gilchrist, A.L. (1999). Relative area and relative luminance combine to anchor surface lightness values. Perception & Psychophysics, 61(5), 771-785. 8. Gilchrist, A. L., Kossyfidis C., Bonato F., Agostini T., Cataliotti, J., Li X., Spehar, B., Annan V. & Economou, E. (1999). An Anchoring Theory of Lightness Perception. Psychological Review, 106(4), 795-834. 9. Gilchrist, A. L. (2006). Seeing black and white. New York: Oxford University Press, Inc. 10. Bressan, P. (2006). The place of white in a world of grays: a double-anchoring theory of lightness perception. Psychological Review, 113, 526-553. 11. Blakeslee, B. & McCourt, M.E. (2004). A unified theory of brightness contrast and assimilation incorporating oriented multiscale spatial filtering and contrast normalization. Vision Research, 44, 2483-2503. Author Contributions: A.G. and A.R. designed the experiments and created the stimulus display. A.R. tested the subjects and analyzed the data. A.G. and A.R. wrote the paper. Competing financial interest statement: There are no competing financial interests. Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to alan@psychology.rutgers.edu. Figure legends Figure 1: The three disks that have been pasted into this photograph appear black, gray and white, although they are identical. Figure 2: Adelson s Checker shadow illusion: Squares A and B are identical in luminance, but they appear different in lightness.

10 Figure 3: Adelson s checker shadow illusion with equluminant probe disks (dark-disk condition). The upper and lower rightmost disks have the same local contrast, but they appear different in lightness. The two lower disks have different local contrast but they appear almost the same shade of gray; the same is true for the two upper disks. Figure 4. Perceived disk lightness in the dark-disk (top: Figure 4a) and the lightdisk condition (bottom: Figure 4b). Each data point represents the mean matched lightness of a separate group of 15 observers (total N=60) expressed in log reflectance with error bars indicating ± 1 between-subject SEM. For clarity, we added a gray-scale on the Y-axis that (approximately) corresponds to each value of log-reflectance. The relative position of the target disk is plotted on the X-axis. Black lines connect the data points corresponding to disks that lie in the same framework (Shadow vs. Light). Horizontal gray arrows indicate pairs of disks that have different contrast, but belong to the same framework. Vertical gray arrow indicate a pair of disks that have the same local contrast, but belong to different frameworks of illumination.