A Neural Edge-Detection Model for Enhanced Auditory Sensitivity in Modulated Noise

Similar documents
19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007

Spectro-Temporal Methods in Primary Auditory Cortex David Klein Didier Depireux Jonathan Simon Shihab Shamma

Perception of pitch. Definitions. Why is pitch important? BSc Audiology/MSc SHS Psychoacoustics wk 5: 12 Feb A. Faulkner.

Perception of pitch. Importance of pitch: 2. mother hemp horse. scold. Definitions. Why is pitch important? AUDL4007: 11 Feb A. Faulkner.

Perception of pitch. Definitions. Why is pitch important? BSc Audiology/MSc SHS Psychoacoustics wk 4: 7 Feb A. Faulkner.

Predicting discrimination of formant frequencies in vowels with a computational model of the auditory midbrain

A cat's cocktail party: Psychophysical, neurophysiological, and computational studies of spatial release from masking

A CLOSER LOOK AT THE REPRESENTATION OF INTERAURAL DIFFERENCES IN A BINAURAL MODEL

Acoustics, signals & systems for audiology. Week 9. Basic Psychoacoustic Phenomena: Temporal resolution

III. Publication III. c 2005 Toni Hirvonen.

The psychoacoustics of reverberation

HCS 7367 Speech Perception

Tone-in-noise detection: Observed discrepancies in spectral integration. Nicolas Le Goff a) Technische Universiteit Eindhoven, P.O.

COM325 Computer Speech and Hearing

Spectral and temporal processing in the human auditory system

You know about adding up waves, e.g. from two loudspeakers. AUDL 4007 Auditory Perception. Week 2½. Mathematical prelude: Adding up levels

Pressure vs. decibel modulation in spectrotemporal representations: How nonlinear are auditory cortical stimuli?

Psycho-acoustics (Sound characteristics, Masking, and Loudness)

Monaural and Binaural Speech Separation

Temporal resolution AUDL Domain of temporal resolution. Fine structure and envelope. Modulating a sinusoid. Fine structure and envelope

Effect of fast-acting compression on modulation detection interference for normal hearing and hearing impaired listeners

Release from masking in uctuating background noise in a songbird's auditory forebrain

Neural Processing of Amplitude-Modulated Sounds: Joris, Schreiner and Rees, Physiol. Rev. 2004

19 th INTERNATIONAL CONGRESS ON ACOUSTICS MADRID, 2-7 SEPTEMBER 2007 AUDITORY EVOKED MAGNETIC FIELDS AND LOUDNESS IN RELATION TO BANDPASS NOISES

The role of intrinsic masker fluctuations on the spectral spread of masking

Binaural Hearing. Reading: Yost Ch. 12

Neural Representations of Sinusoidal Amplitude and Frequency Modulations in the Primary Auditory Cortex of Awake Primates

Results of Egan and Hake using a single sinusoidal masker [reprinted with permission from J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 22, 622 (1950)].

Interaction of Object Binding Cues in Binaural Masking Pattern Experiments

Distortion products and the perceived pitch of harmonic complex tones

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Effect of filter spacing and correct tonotopic representation on melody recognition: Implications for cochlear implants

2920 J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 102 (5), Pt. 1, November /97/102(5)/2920/5/$ Acoustical Society of America 2920

AUDL GS08/GAV1 Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Loudness & Temporal resolution

SOUND QUALITY EVALUATION OF FAN NOISE BASED ON HEARING-RELATED PARAMETERS SUMMARY INTRODUCTION

Complex Sounds. Reading: Yost Ch. 4

An introduction to physics of Sound

Neuronal correlates of pitch in the Inferior Colliculus

Modeling auditory processing of amplitude modulation I. Detection and masking with narrow-band carriers Dau, T.; Kollmeier, B.; Kohlrausch, A.G.

Experiments in two-tone interference

Temporal Modulation Transfer Functions for Tonal Stimuli: Gated versus Continuous Conditions

Hearing and Deafness 2. Ear as a frequency analyzer. Chris Darwin

Exploiting envelope fluctuations to achieve robust extraction and intelligent integration of binaural cues

INTRODUCTION. Address and author to whom correspondence should be addressed. Electronic mail:

SIMULATING RESTING CORTICAL BACKGROUND ACTIVITY WITH FILTERED NOISE. Journal of Integrative Neuroscience 7(3):

Rapid Formation of Robust Auditory Memories: Insights from Noise

Effects of Firing Synchrony on Signal Propagation in Layered Networks

Applying Models of Auditory Processing to Automatic Speech Recognition: Promise and Progress!

Binaural Mechanisms that Emphasize Consistent Interaural Timing Information over Frequency

Across frequency processing with time varying spectra

Limulus eye: a filter cascade. Limulus 9/23/2011. Dynamic Response to Step Increase in Light Intensity

Pitch estimation using spiking neurons

Signals, Sound, and Sensation

Modeling auditory processing of amplitude modulation II. Spectral and temporal integration Dau, T.; Kollmeier, B.; Kohlrausch, A.G.

IN a natural environment, speech often occurs simultaneously. Monaural Speech Segregation Based on Pitch Tracking and Amplitude Modulation

Beyond Blind Averaging Analyzing Event-Related Brain Dynamics

Auditory modelling for speech processing in the perceptual domain

Estimating critical bandwidths of temporal sensitivity to low-frequency amplitude modulation

Contribution of frequency modulation to speech recognition in noise a)

Effects of Reverberation on Pitch, Onset/Offset, and Binaural Cues

Lecture 13 Read: the two Eckhorn papers. (Don t worry about the math part of them).

Block diagram of proposed general approach to automatic reduction of speech wave to lowinformation-rate signals.

Feasibility of Vocal Emotion Conversion on Modulation Spectrogram for Simulated Cochlear Implants

AUDL 4007 Auditory Perception. Week 1. The cochlea & auditory nerve: Obligatory stages of auditory processing

Dominant Voiced Speech Segregation Using Onset Offset Detection and IBM Based Segmentation

Speech Synthesis; Pitch Detection and Vocoders

Physiological evidence for auditory modulation filterbanks: Cortical responses to concurrent modulations

Phase and Feedback in the Nonlinear Brain. Malcolm Slaney (IBM and Stanford) Hiroko Shiraiwa-Terasawa (Stanford) Regaip Sen (Stanford)

An Auditory Localization and Coordinate Transform Chip

THE MATLAB IMPLEMENTATION OF BINAURAL PROCESSING MODEL SIMULATING LATERAL POSITION OF TONES WITH INTERAURAL TIME DIFFERENCES

Lecture 4 Foundations and Cognitive Processes in Visual Perception From the Retina to the Visual Cortex

Measuring the critical band for speech a)

Imagine the cochlea unrolled

INTRODUCTION I. METHODS J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 99 (6), June /96/99(6)/3592/14/$ Acoustical Society of America 3592

I. INTRODUCTION. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 114 (4), Pt. 1, October /2003/114(4)/2079/20/$ Acoustical Society of America

Machine recognition of speech trained on data from New Jersey Labs

A Pole Zero Filter Cascade Provides Good Fits to Human Masking Data and to Basilar Membrane and Neural Data

Spectro-Temporal Processing of Dynamic Broadband Sounds In Auditory Cortex

Perception of low frequencies in small rooms

AN AUDITORILY MOTIVATED ANALYSIS METHOD FOR ROOM IMPULSE RESPONSES

inter.noise 2000 The 29th International Congress and Exhibition on Noise Control Engineering August 2000, Nice, FRANCE

6.551j/HST.714j Acoustics of Speech and Hearing: Exam 2

Chapter 73. Two-Stroke Apparent Motion. George Mather

Testing of Objective Audio Quality Assessment Models on Archive Recordings Artifacts

A unitary model of pitch perception Ray Meddis and Lowel O Mard Department of Psychology, Essex University, Colchester CO4 3SQ, United Kingdom

Detection of Tones in Reproducible Noises: Prediction of Listeners Performance in Diotic and Dichotic Conditions

Figure S3. Histogram of spike widths of recorded units.

An auditory model that can account for frequency selectivity and phase effects on masking

Intensity Discrimination and Binaural Interaction

Detection of Modulated Tones in Modulated Noise by Non-human Primates

AUDL GS08/GAV1 Auditory Perception. Envelope and temporal fine structure (TFS)

Auditory Based Feature Vectors for Speech Recognition Systems

Research Note MODULATION TRANSFER FUNCTIONS: A COMPARISON OF THE RESULTS OF THREE METHODS

Preeti Rao 2 nd CompMusicWorkshop, Istanbul 2012

The effect of noise fluctuation and spectral bandwidth on gap detection

Michael F. Toner, et. al.. "Distortion Measurement." Copyright 2000 CRC Press LLC. <

Monaural and binaural processing of fluctuating sounds in the auditory system

Eighth Quarterly Progress Report

Citation for published version (APA): Lijzenga, J. (1997). Discrimination of simplified vowel spectra Groningen: s.n.

The EarSpring Model for the Loudness Response in Unimpaired Human Hearing

Transcription:

A Neural Edge-etection odel for Enhanced Auditory Sensitivity in odulated Noise Alon Fishbach and Bradford J. ay epartment of Biomedical Engineering and Otolaryngology-HNS Johns Hopkins University Baltimore, 21205 fishbach@northwestern.edu Abstract sychophysical data suggest that temporal modulations of stimulus amplitude envelopes play a prominent role in the perceptual segregation of concurrent sounds. In particular, the detection of an unmodulated signal can be significantly improved by adding amplitude modulation to the spectral envelope of a competing masking noise. This perceptual phenomenon is known as Comodulation asking Release (CR). espite the obvious influence of temporal structure on the perception of complex auditory scenes, the physiological mechanisms that contribute to CR and auditory streaming are not well known. A recent physiological study by Nelken and colleagues has demonstrated an enhanced cortical representation of auditory signals in modulated noise. Our study evaluates these CR-like response patterns from the perspective of a hypothetical auditory edge-detection neuron. It is shown that this simple neural model for the detection of amplitude transients can reproduce not only the physiological data of Nelken et al., but also, in light of previous results, a variety of physiological and psychoacoustical phenomena that are related to the perceptual segregation of concurrent sounds. 1 Introduction The temporal structure of a complex sound exerts strong influences on auditory physiology (e.g. [10, 16]) and perception (e.g. [9, 19, 20]). In particular, studies of auditory scene analysis have demonstrated the importance of the temporal structure of amplitude envelopes in the perceptual segregation of concurrent sounds [2, 7]. Common amplitude transitions across frequency serve as salient cues for grouping sound energy into unified perceptual objects. Conversely, asynchronous amplitude transitions enhance the separation of competing acoustic events [3, 4].

These general principles are manifested in perceptual phenomena as diverse as comodulation masking release (CR) [13], modulation detection interference [22] and synchronous onset grouping [8]. espite the obvious importance of timing information in psychoacoustic studies of auditory masking, the way in which the CNS represents the temporal structure of an amplitude envelope is not well understood. Certainly many physiological studies have demonstrated neural sensitivities to envelope transitions, but this sensitivity is only beginning to be related to the variety of perceptual experiences that are evoked by signals in noise. Nelken et al. [15] have suggested a correspondence between neural responses to time-varying amplitude envelopes and psychoacoustic masking phenomena. In their study of neurons in primary auditory cortex (A1), adding temporal modulation to background noise lowered the detection thresholds of unmodulated tones. This enhanced signal detection is similar to the perceptual phenomenon that is known as comodulation masking release [13]. Fishbach et al. [11] have recently proposed a neural model for the detection of auditory edges (i.e., amplitude transients) that can account for numerous physiological [14, 17, 18] and psychoacoustical [3, 21] phenomena. The encompassing utility of this edge-detection model suggests a common mechanism that may link the auditory processing and perception of auditory signals in a complex auditory scene. Here, it is shown that the auditory edge detection model can accurately reproduce the cortical CR-like responses previously described by Nelken and colleagues. 2 The odel The model is described in detail elsewhere [11]. In short, the basic operation of the model is the calculation of the first-order time derivative of the log-compressed envelope of the stimulus. A computational model [23] is used to convert the acoustic waveform to a physiologically plausible auditory nerve representation (Fig 1a). The simulated neural response has a medium spontaneous rate and a characteristic frequency that is set to the frequency of the target tone. To allow computation of the time derivative of the stimulus envelope, we hypothesize the existence of a temporal delay dimension, along which the stimulus is progressively delayed. The intermediate delay layer (Fig 1b) is constructed from an array of neurons with ascending membrane time constants (τ); each neuron is modeled by a conventional integrate-and-fire model (I&F, [12]). Higher membrane time constant induces greater delay in the neuron s response [1]. The output of the delay layer converges to a single output neuron (Fig. 1c) via a set of connection with various efficacies that reflect a receptive field of a gaussian derivative. This combination of excitatory and inhibitory connections carries out the time-derivative computation. Implementation details and parameters are given in [11]. The model has 2 adjustable and 6 fixed parameters, the former were used to fit the responses of the model to single unit responses to variety of stimuli [11]. The results reported here are not sensitive to these parameters.

(a) AN model (b) delay-layer (c) edge-detector neuron τ=6ms τ=4ms I&F Neuron τ=3ms bandpass RS log d dt Figure 1: Schematic diagram of the model and a block diagram of the basic operation of each model component (shaded area). The stimulus is converted to a neural representation (a) that approximates the average firing rate of a medium spontaneous-rate AN fiber [23]. The operation of this stage can be roughly described as the log-compressed rms output of a bandpass filter. The neural representation is fed to a series of neurons with ascending membrane time constant (b). The kernel functions that are used to simulate these neurons are plotted for a few neurons along with the time constants used. The output of the delay-layer neurons converge to a single I&F neuron (c) using a set of connections with weights that reflect a shape of a gaussian derivative. Solid arrows represent excitatory connections and white arrows represent inhibitory connections. The absolute efficacy is represented by the width of the arrows. 3 Result s Nelken et al. [15] report that amplitude modulation can substantially modify the noise-driven discharge rates of A1 neurons in Halothane-anesthetized cats. any cortical neurons show only a transient onset response to unmodulated noise but fire in synchrony ( lock ) to the envelope of modulated noise. A significant reduction in envelope-locked discharge rates is observed if an unmodulated tone is added to modulated noise. As summarized in Fig. 2, this suppression of envelope locking can reveal the presence of an auditory signal at sound pressure levels that are not detectable in unmodulated noise. It has been suggested that this pattern of neural responding may represent a physiological equivalent of CR. Reproduction of CR-like cortical activity can be illustrated by a simplified case in which the analytical amplitude envelope of the stimulus is used as the input to the edge-detector model. In keeping with the actual physiological approach of Nelken et al., the noise envelope is shaped by a trapezoid modulator for these simulations. Each cycle of modulation, E N (t), is given by: E N = ( t 3) 0 t < t < 3 3 t < 4 4 t < 8 where is the peak pressure level and is set to 12.5 ms. 0 t

(a) Unmodulated noise (b) odulated noise Tone level (db SL) 76 26 0 150 300 0 150 300 Time (ms) Figure 2: Responses of an A1 unit to a combination of noise and tone at many tone levels, replotted from Nelken et al. [15]. (a) Unmodulated noise and (b) modulated noise. The noise envelope is illustrated by the thick line above each figure. Each row shows the response of the neuron to the noise plus the tone at the level specified on the ordinate. The dashed line in (b) indicates the detection threshold level for the tone. The detection threshold (as defined and calculated by Nelken et al.) in the unmodulated noise was not reached. Spikes/sec Since the basic operation of the model is the calculation of the rectified timederivative of the log-compressed envelope of the stimulus, the expected noisedriven rate of the model can be approximated by: d t) = max 0, A ln 1 dt E ( t ) ( + ) N ( 0 where A=20/ln(10) and 0 =2e-5 a. The expected firing rate in response to the noise plus an unmodulated signal (tone) can be similarly approximated by: d E ( t ) + S N + S = max 0, A ln( 1 + ) dt 0 where S is the peak pressure level of the tone. Clearly, both N (t) and N+S (t) are identically zero outside the interval [0 ]. Within this interval it holds that: A N < = 0 t + t 0 and A N + S = 0 t < 0 + S + t and the ratio of the firing rates is: N N + S = 1 + + 0 0 t < Clearly, N + S < N for the interval [0 ] of each modulation cycle. That is, the addition of a tone reduces the responses of the model to the rising part of the modulated envelope. Higher tone levels ( s ) cause greater reduction in the model s firing rate. S t

(a) (b) Level (db SL) (c) Level derivative (db SL/ms) (d) Time (ms) Figure 3: An illustration of the basic operation of the model on various amplitude envelopes. The simplified operation of the model includes log compression of the amplitude envelope (a and c) and rectified time-derivative of the log-compressed envelope (b and d). (a) A 30 db SL tone is added to a modulated envelope (peak level of 70 db SL) 300 ms after the beginning of the stimulus (as indicated by the horizontal line). The addition of the tone causes a great reduction in the time derivative of the log-compressed envelope (b). When the envelope of the noise is unmodulated (c), the time-derivative of the log-compressed envelope (d) shows a tiny spike when the tone is added (marked by the arrow). Fig. 3 demonstrates the effect of a low-level tone on the time-derivative of the logcompressed envelope of a noise. When the envelope is modulated (Fig. 3a) the addition of the tone greatly reduces the derivative of the rising part of the modulation (Fig. 3b). In the absence of modulations (Fig. 3c), the tone presentation produces a negligible effect on the level derivative (Fig. 3d). odel simulations of neural responses to the stimuli used by Nelken et al. are plotted in Fig. 4. As illustrated schematically in Fig 3 (d), the presence of the tone does not cause any significant change in the responses of the model to the unmodulated noise (Fig. 4a). In the modulated noise, however, tones of relatively low levels reduce the responses of the model to the rising part of the envelope modulations.

(a) Unmodulated noise (b) odulated noise Tone level (db SL) 76 26 0 150 300 0 150 300 Time (ms) Figure 4: Simulated responses of the model to a combination of a tone and Unmodulated noise (a) and modulated noise (b). All conventions are as in Fig. 2. Spikes/sec 4 iscussion This report uses an auditory edge-detection model to simulate the actual physiological consequences of amplitude modulation on neural sensitivity in cortical area A1. The basic computational operation of the model is the calculation of the smoothed time-derivative of the log-compressed stimulus envelope. The ability of the model to reproduce cortical response patterns in detail across a variety of stimulus conditions suggests similar time-sensitive mechanisms may contribute to the physiological correlates of CR. These findings augment our previous observations that the simple edge-detection model can successfully predict a wide range of physiological and perceptual phenomena [11]. Former applications of the model to perceptual phenomena have been mainly related to auditory scene analysis, or more specifically the ability of the auditory system to distinguish multiple sound sources. In these cases, a sharp amplitude transition at stimulus onset ( auditory edge ) was critical for sound segregation. Here, it is shown that the detection of acoustic signals also may be enhanced through the suppression of ongoing responses to the concurrent modulations of competing background sounds. Interestingly, these temporal fluctuations appear to be a common property of natural soundscapes [15]. The model provides testable predictions regarding how signal detection may be influenced by the temporal shape of amplitude modulation. Carlyon et al. [6] measured CR in human listeners using three types of noise modulation: squarewave, sine wave and multiplied noise. From the perspective of the edge-detection model, these psychoacoustic results are intriguing because the different modulator types represent manipulations of the time derivative of masker envelopes. Squarewave modulation had the most sharply edged time derivative and produced the greatest masking release. Fig. 5 plots the responses of the model to a pure-tone signal in square-wave and sine-wave modulated noise. As in the psychoacoustical data of Carlyon et al., the simulated detection threshold was lower in the context of square-wave modulation. Our modeling results suggest that the sharply edged square wave evoked higher levels of noise-driven activity and therefore created a sensitive background for the suppressing effects of the unmodulated tone.

(a) (b) Tone level (db SL) 60 10 0 200 400 600 0 200 400 600 Time (ms) Figure 5: Simulated responses of the model to a combination of a tone at various levels and a sine-wave modulated noise (a) or a square-wave modulated noise (b). Each row shows the response of the model to the noise plus the tone at the level specified on the abscissa. The shape of the noise modulator is illustrated above each figure. The 100 ms tone starts 250 ms after the noise onset. Note that the tone detection threshold (marked by the dashed line) is 10 db lower for the square-wave modulator than for the sine-wave modulator, in accordance with the psychoacoustical data of Carlyon et al. [6]. Spikes/sec Although the physiological basis of our model was derived from studies of neural responses in the cat auditory system, the key psychoacoustical observations of Carlyon et al. have been replicated in recent behavioral studies of cats (Budelis et al. [5]). These data support the generalization of human perceptual processing to other species and enhance the possible correspondence between the neuronal CR-like effect and the psychoacoustical masking phenomena. Clearly, the auditory system relies on information other than the time derivative of the stimulus envelope for the detection of auditory signals in background noise. Further physiological and psychoacoustic assessments of CR-like masking effects are needed not only to refine the predictive abilities of the edge-detection model but also to reveal the additional sources of acoustic information that influence signal detection in constantly changing natural environments. Acknowledgments This work was supported in part by a NIC grant R01 C004841. References [1] Agmon-Snir H., Segev I. (1993). Signal delay and input synchronization in passive dendritic structure, J. Neurophysiol. 70, 2066-2085. [2] Bregman A.S. (1990). Auditory scene analysis: The perceptual organization of sound, IT ress, Cambridge, A. [3] Bregman A.S., Ahad.A., Kim J., elnerich L. (1994) Resetting the pitch-analysis system. 1. Effects of rise times of tones in noise backgrounds or of harmonics in a complex tone, ercept. sychophys. 56 (2), 155-162.

[4] Bregman A.S., Ahad.A., Kim J. (1994) Resetting the pitch-analysis system. 2. Role of sudden onsets and offsets in the perception of individual components in a cluster of overlapping tones, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 96 (5), 2694-2703. [5] Budelis J., Fishbach A., ay B.J. (2002) Behavioral assessments of comodulation masking release in cats, Abst. Assoc. for Res. in Otolaryngol. 25. [6] Carlyon R.., Buus S., Florentine. (1989) Comodulation masking release for three types of modulator as a function of modulation rate, Hear. Res. 42, 37-46. [7] arwin C.J. (1997) Auditory grouping, Trends in Cog. Sci. 1(9), 327-333. [8] arwin C.J., Ciocca V. (1992) Grouping in pitch perception: Effects of onset asynchrony and ear of presentation of a mistuned component, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 91, 3381-3390. [9] rullman R., Festen H.., lomp R. (1994) Effect of temporal envelope smearing on speech reception, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 95 (2), 1053-1064. [10] Eggermont J J. (1994). Temporal modulation transfer functions for A and F stimuli in cat auditory cortex. Effects of carrier type, modulating waveform and intensity, Hear. Res. 74, 51-66. [11] Fishbach A., Nelken I., Yeshurun Y. (2001) Auditory edge detection: a neural model for physiological and psychoacoustical responses to amplitude transients, J. Neurophysiol. 85, 2303 2323. [12] Gerstner W. (1999) Spiking neurons, in ulsed Neural Networks, edited by W. aass, C.. Bishop, (IT ress, Cambridge, A). [13] Hall J.W., Haggard.., Fernandes.A. (1984) etection in noise by spectrotemporal pattern analysis, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 76, 50-56. [14] Heil. (1997) Auditory onset responses revisited. II. Response strength, J. Neurophysiol. 77, 2642-2660. [15] Nelken I., Rotman Y., Bar-Yosef O. (1999) Responses of auditory cortex neurons to structural features of natural sounds, Nature 397, 154-157. [16] hillips.. (1988). Effect of Tone-ulse Rise Time on Rate-Level Functions of Cat Auditory Cortex Neurons: Excitatory and Inhibitory rocesses Shaping Responses to Tone Onset, J. Neurophysiol. 59, 1524-1539. [17] hillips.., Burkard R. (1999). Response magnitude and timing of auditory response initiation in the inferior colliculus of the awake chinchilla, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 105, 2731-2737. [18] hillips.., Semple.N., Kitzes L.. (1995). Factors shaping the tone level sensitivity of single neurons in posterior field of cat auditory cortex, J. Neurophysiol. 73, 674-686. [19] Rosen S. (1992) Temporal information in speech: acoustic, auditory and linguistic aspects, hil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 336, 367-373. [20] Shannon R.V., Zeng F.G., Kamath V., Wygonski J, Ekelid. (1995) Speech recognition with primarily temporal cues, Science 270, 303-304. [21] Turner C.W., Relkin E.., oucet J. (1994). sychophysical and physiological forward masking studies: probe duration and rise-time effects, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 96 (2), 795-800. [22] Yost W.A., Sheft S. (1994) odulation detection interference across-frequency processing and auditory grouping, Hear. Res. 79, 48-58. [23] Zhang X., Heinz.G., Bruce I.C., Carney L.H. (2001). A phenomenological model for the responses of auditory-nerve fibers: I. Nonlinear tuning with compression and suppression, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 109 (2), 648-670.