First-order structure induces the 3-D curvature contrast effect

Similar documents
The Shape-Weight Illusion

Chapter 3. Adaptation to disparity but not to perceived depth

Discriminating direction of motion trajectories from angular speed and background information

IOC, Vector sum, and squaring: three different motion effects or one?

COPYRIGHTED MATERIAL. Overview

COPYRIGHTED MATERIAL OVERVIEW 1

Perceived depth is enhanced with parallax scanning

Human Vision and Human-Computer Interaction. Much content from Jeff Johnson, UI Wizards, Inc.

Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion

Perception. What We Will Cover in This Section. Perception. How we interpret the information our senses receive. Overview Perception

T-junctions in inhomogeneous surrounds

Self-motion perception from expanding and contracting optical flows overlapped with binocular disparity

Module 2. Lecture-1. Understanding basic principles of perception including depth and its representation.

Psychophysics of night vision device halo

Muscular Torque Can Explain Biases in Haptic Length Perception: A Model Study on the Radial-Tangential Illusion

Perceiving binocular depth with reference to a common surface

Factors affecting curved versus straight path heading perception

Stereo-slant adaptation is high level and does not involve disparity coding

the dimensionality of the world Travelling through Space and Time Learning Outcomes Johannes M. Zanker

GROUPING BASED ON PHENOMENAL PROXIMITY

The vertical-horizontal illusion: Assessing the contributions of anisotropy, abutting, and crossing to the misperception of simple line stimuli

Experiments on the locus of induced motion

Apparent depth with motion aftereffect and head movement

A Fraser illusion without local cues?

Visual computation of surface lightness: Local contrast vs. frames of reference

Human heading judgments in the presence. of moving objects.

Psych 333, Winter 2008, Instructor Boynton, Exam 1

The horizon line, linear perspective, interposition, and background brightness as determinants of the magnitude of the pictorial moon illusion

Häkkinen, Jukka; Gröhn, Lauri Turning water into rock

P rcep e t p i t on n a s a s u n u c n ons n c s ious u s i nf n e f renc n e L ctur u e 4 : Recogni n t i io i n

The Haptic Perception of Spatial Orientations studied with an Haptic Display

MOTION PARALLAX AND ABSOLUTE DISTANCE. Steven H. Ferris NAVAL SUBMARINE MEDICAL RESEARCH LABORATORY NAVAL SUBMARINE MEDICAL CENTER REPORT NUMBER 673

The cyclopean (stereoscopic) barber pole illusion

Haptic perception of spatial relations

Salient features make a search easy

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Report

Stereoscopic occlusion and the aperture problem for motion: a new solution 1

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Report

Prof. Riyadh Al_Azzawi F.R.C.Psych

IV: Visual Organization and Interpretation

Haptic control in a virtual environment

AD-A lji llllllllllii l

Human Visual System. Prof. George Wolberg Dept. of Computer Science City College of New York

Anisotropy in Werner s Binocular Depth-contrast Effect

3D Space Perception. (aka Depth Perception)

Gestalt Principles of Visual Perception

Algebraic functions describing the Zöllner illusion

Vision V Perceiving Movement

Vision V Perceiving Movement

Experience-dependent visual cue integration based on consistencies between visual and haptic percepts

Simple Figures and Perceptions in Depth (2): Stereo Capture

Monocular occlusion cues alter the influence of terminator motion in the barber pole phenomenon

Slide 4 Now we have the same components that we find in our eye. The analogy is made clear in this slide. Slide 5 Important structures in the eye

The ground dominance effect in the perception of 3-D layout

On the intensity maximum of the Oppel-Kundt illusion

Spatial Judgments from Different Vantage Points: A Different Perspective

Human Vision. Human Vision - Perception

Directional Bias in the Perception of Cast Shadows

Depth adjacency and the rod-and-frame illusion

Interference in stimuli employed to assess masking by substitution. Bernt Christian Skottun. Ullevaalsalleen 4C Oslo. Norway

Vision: Distance & Size Perception

The Human Visual System!

Distance perception from motion parallax and ground contact. Rui Ni and Myron L. Braunstein. University of California, Irvine, California

The human visual system

Marks + Channels. Large Data Visualization Torsten Möller. Munzner/Möller

Measurement of Visual Resolution of Display Screens

Poles for Increasing the Sensibility of Vertical Gradient. in a Downhill Road

Visual Effects of Light. Prof. Grega Bizjak, PhD Laboratory of Lighting and Photometry Faculty of Electrical Engineering University of Ljubljana

Limitations of the Oriented Difference of Gaussian Filter in Special Cases of Brightness Perception Illusions

How various aspects of motion parallax influence distance judgments, even when we think we are standing still

The shape of luminance increments at the intersection alters the magnitude of the scintillating grid illusion

The Mona Lisa Effect: Perception of Gaze Direction in Real and Pictured Faces

Simple reaction time as a function of luminance for various wavelengths*

THE POGGENDORFF ILLUSION WITH ANOMALOUS SURFACES: MANAGING PAC-MANS, PARALLELS LENGTH AND TYPE OF TRANSVERSAL.

Perception of scene layout from optical contact, shadows, and motion

Geometrical Structures of Photographic and Stereoscopic Spaces

Visual Effects of. Light. Warmth. Light is life. Sun as a deity (god) If sun would turn off the life on earth would extinct

Perception: From Biology to Psychology

Depth seen with subjective

The eye, displays and visual effects

Moving Cast Shadows and the Perception of Relative Depth

This is a repository copy of Thatcher s Britain: : a new take on an old illusion.

The Performance of CIECAM02

Scene layout from ground contact, occlusion, and motion parallax

CYCLOPEAN MOTION AFTEREFFECTS USING SPIRAL PATTERNS: DISSOCIATION BETWEEN LOCAL AND GLOBAL PROCESSING JASON ALAN ROGERS

AGING AND STEERING CONTROL UNDER REDUCED VISIBILITY CONDITIONS. Wichita State University, Wichita, Kansas, USA

Brightness with and without perceived transparency: When does it make a difference?

Unit IV: Sensation & Perception. Module 19 Vision Organization & Interpretation

For the Subjective Evaluation Items of the Pencil Still Drawing Evaluation System

The Influence of Visual Illusion on Visually Perceived System and Visually Guided Action System

Chapter 73. Two-Stroke Apparent Motion. George Mather

Shape constancy measured by a canonical-shape method

Varilux Comfort. Technology. 2. Development concept for a new lens generation

A Vestibular Sensation: Probabilistic Approaches to Spatial Perception (II) Presented by Shunan Zhang

Three-dimensional spatial grouping affects estimates of the illuminant

Learned Stimulation in Space and Motion Perception

Sensation and Perception. What We Will Cover in This Section. Sensation

Lecture 2 Digital Image Fundamentals. Lin ZHANG, PhD School of Software Engineering Tongji University Fall 2016

Perceiving Motion and Events

ISSN: X Impact factor: (Volume3, Issue1) Available online at: Human Depth Perception Kiran Kumari Department of Physics

Transcription:

Vision Research 41 (2001) 3829 3835 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres First-order structure induces the 3-D curvature contrast effect Susan F. te Pas a, *, Astrid M.L. Kappers b a Psychonomics, Helmholtz Instituut, Uni ersiteit Utrecht, Heidelberglaan 2, 3584 CS Utrecht, The Netherlands b Human Perception, Helmholtz Instituut, Uni ersiteit Utrecht, Princetonplein 5, 3584 CC Utrecht, The Netherlands Received 8 December 2000; received in revised form 27 July 2001 Abstract A 3-D curvature contrast effect has been reported in shading-and-texture-defined (Curran & Johnson (1996). Vision Research 36, 3641 3653) and in stereoscopically defined (te Pas, Rogers, & Ledgeway (2000). Current Psychology Letters: Brain, Beha iour and Cognition 1, 117 126) stimuli. Our experiments show that a clear 3-D curvature contrast effect also occurs in motion-defined stimuli. The magnitude of the effect is similar in motion-, stereo- and shading-and-texture defined stimuli, suggesting that the 3-D curvature contrast effect is shape-based. We find a distinct contrast effect that is similar in the case of inducers that contain second-order (curvature) information and in the case of inducers that contain only first-order (slant and tilt) information. The effect with inducers that contain only zeroth-order (depth) information is very small. We conclude that the first-order structure is sufficient to induce a 3-D contrast effect. 2001 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Vision; Structure-from-motion; Curvature discrimination; Curvature contrast 1. Introduction Visual perception is often based on relative, rather than absolute, measures. This means that the global aspects of a scene such as layout and geometry can influence the perception of local object properties like depth, attitude and curvature. Clear examples of such influence are simultaneous contrast effects. Contrast effects can be found in stimuli that are defined by different cues such as luminance, motion and disparity. They can also be found in a wide range of geometrical properties like depth, slant and curvature (e.g. Gibson, 1933; Cornsweet, 1970; Anstis, 1975; Graham & Rogers, 1982). In this paper, we address the 3-D curvature contrast effect, the phenomenon whereby a curved object looks less curved when it is surrounded by one or more heavily curved surrounding objects. A 3-D curvature contrast effect has been reported for shading- and texture-defined surfaces (Curran & Johnston, 1996) as * Corresponding author. Tel.: +31-3025-34589; fax: +31-3025- 34511. E-mail address: s.tepas@fss.uu.nl (S.F. te Pas). well as for stereoscopically defined surfaces (te Pas, Rogers, & Ledgeway, 2000). Although their stimuli were defined by different cues, both Curran and Johnston (1996) and te Pas et al. (2000) report that the 3-D curvature contrast effect was of the same order of magnitude, suggesting that the phenomenon is actually shape-based, not cue-based. The biases, which are heavily dependent on the ratio of the surround curvatures, can easily add up to 30% of the reference curvature. In order to measure such a 3-D curvature contrast effect, one necessarily has to use stimuli that contain second-order shape information. However, there is some discussion in the literature as to whether the visual system can access second-order, or even first-order, information directly. In the stereo domain, for instance, Lunn and Morgan (1997) have demonstrated that although second-order information (disparity curvature) seems to be accessible, observers tend to use zeroth-order information (disparity) to infer second-order structure whenever such information is available. Rogers and Cagenello (1989) suggested that disparity curvature might be derived indirectly from the differences in the curvature of corresponding line elements in the two eyes. 0042-6989/01/$ - see front matter 2001 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. PII: S0042-6989(01)00208-5

3830 S.F. te Pas, A.M.L. Kappers / Vision Research 41 (2001) 3829 3835 Curran and Johnston (1996) demonstrate that the 3-D curvature contrast effect that they report cannot be induced by luminance or luminance contrast information. They conclude that the effect must therefore be induced by second-order luminance information. However, this does not mean that the effect is necessarily induced by second-order shape information. Curvature could still be inferred indirectly from either zeroth-order (depth) or first-order (slant and tilt) shape information. te Pas et al. (2000) ruled out the absolute disparity range and disparity gradient range as causal factors. They increased the absolute disparity range and disparity gradient range in the stimulus by putting the inducers and the test stimulus in different depth planes and in different orientations; these adjustments had no effect on the results. However, the relati e depth or disparity range might still be sufficient to induce a 3-D curvature contrast effect. The aims of the present study are threefold. First of all, we want to investigate whether a similar 3-D curvature contrast effect can be found in structure-from-motion-defined stimuli. If we can report a contrast effect of the same order of magnitude for stimuli that are defined by yet another cue, this would strengthen the argument for a shape-based phenomenon. Secondly, we Fig. 1. Experiment 1: Schematic representation of the stimuli. We use two different test surround curvatures and three different reference surround curvatures, resulting in six different surround curvature ratios. The surround curvature ratio is A: 1/3. B: 2/3; C: 1; D: 1; E: 2; F: 3. want to investigate the nature of the 3-D curvature contrast effect. Is it really an effect of curvature, or can the same effect be induced by first-order (slant and tilt) or even zeroth-order (depth) shape information alone? We created a set of zeroth-, first- and second-order 3D inducers to help us address this question. Lastly, we will look at whether curvature discrimination thresholds are affected by variations in the surround. 2. Experiment 1 2.1. Methods The stimuli that we use are defined by structurefrom-motion. Approximately 500 sparse random dots are distributed over the surface of a stimulus. White dots are presented on a dark screen. By simulating a surface rotation of 30 back and forth around a vertical axis, we create a powerful illusion of a surface that extends in depth. Schematic examples of the stimuli are shown in Fig. 1. Stimuli consist of a small central paraboloid with a high curvature surrounded by a large paraboloid of smaller curvature. The central paraboloid subtends approximately 9.15 of visual angle. We slightly randomise the angular size to make sure that the absolute depth of the central paraboloid is not a strong cue. The surround subtends 22.6 of visual angle. The curvature of the central reference paraboloid is either 36.6/m or 76.6/m. We vary the curvature of the central test paraboloid around the curvature of the central reference paraboloid in order to obtain a psychometric curve. The curvature of the reference surround can be either 15.25/m or 45.75/m. The curvature of the test surround is 15.25, 30.5 or 45.75/m. Thus, we have six different combinations of reference and test surround curvatures. We divide the test surround curvature by the reference surround curvature to obtain the surround curvature ratio. From previous experiments (te Pas et al., 2000), we know that it is this ratio that determines the size of the curvature contrast effect. We have two sets of stimuli with a surround curvature ratio of 1, one set where both surround curvatures are low (Fig. 1C) and one set where both surround curvatures are high (Fig. 1D). In total, we have 12 conditions in Experiment 1: two central reference curvatures times six surround curvature ratios. The task of the observers is to determine whether the curvature of the first or the second central paraboloid is higher. The observers are explicitly told that the surround does not contain any information that they need for their task. We use the method of constant stimuli to determine a psychometric curve. For each condition, we vary the value of the central test curvature around the

S.F. te Pas, A.M.L. Kappers / Vision Research 41 (2001) 3829 3835 3831 Fig. 2. Overview of the experimental set-up. Subjects were seated 50 cm in front of a Radius PressView 21 monitor with their heads in a chin-rest. Stimuli were generated on an Apple Power Macintosh 9600/300 computer. value of the central reference curvature. We present 10 different central test curvatures for each of the 12 conditions. These 120 trials are all presented randomly in one session that takes on average 20 min to measure. Each observer runs 15 of such sessions, which brings the total amount of measuring time per observer to about 5 h. The psychometric curve provides us with a point of subjective equality (PSE) and an 84% correct threshold (the slope of the curve) for each condition. If there is no effect of the surround, the PSE should be equal to the curvature of the central reference paraboloid. Thus, the interesting parameter in this experiment is the difference between the PSE and the central reference curvature, namely the bias. We present our results in terms of this bias, which we define as follows: bias=pse central reference curvature. We define the cur ature discrimination threshold as the 84% correct threshold that we obtain from the psychometric curve. Six naïve observers participated in Experiment 1 (BS, DB, FS, JP, RH and RK). All observers had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity. The observers were seated in a dark room, 50 cm in front of a Radius PressView 21 monitor with their heads in a chin rest to prevent head movements. Stimuli were viewed monocularly to prevent conflicting stereo cues. A schematic overview of the experimental set-up is shown in Fig. 2. reference surround curvature (ratio 1), the central test paraboloid is perceived as more curved than the central reference paraboloid and vice versa (F(5,11) =18.6; P=0.0001). When the test and reference surround curvature are equal (ratio=1), there is hardly any bias. There is no interaction between the central reference curvature and surround curvature ratio (F(5,11) =1.50; P=0.20). Fig. 3B shows curvature discrimination thresholds as a function of surround ratio averaged over all six 2.2. Results Fig. 3A shows the bias as a function of surround curvature ratio averaged over all six observers. Black bars denote results for a central reference curvature of 76.6/m; white bars denote results for a central reference curvature of 36.6/m. We have analysed the results using a 3 by 3 factorial analysis. Our results show that there is no main effect of central reference curvature (F(1,11) =0.71; P=0.40). From Fig. 3A, we can clearly observe an effect of surround curvature ratio: when the test surround curvature is smaller than the Fig. 3. Experiment 1. (A) Bias (PSE-central reference curvature) as a function of surround curvature ratio. (B) Curvature discrimination thresholds as a function of surround curvature ratio. Results are averaged over six subjects. Black and white bars are results for central reference curvatures of 76.6/m and 36.6/m, respectively.

3832 S.F. te Pas, A.M.L. Kappers / Vision Research 41 (2001) 3829 3835 Fig. 4. Experiment 2. Schematic representation of the stimuli. Equivalent surround curvature ratio is 3. (A) Zeroth-order stimuli. (B) First-order stimuli. (C) Second-order stimuli. observers. Black bars show thresholds for a central reference curvature of 76.6/m; white bars show thresholds for a central reference curvature of 36.6/m. There is a clear main effect of central reference curvature on curvature discrimination thresholds (F(1,11) = 25.4; P=0.0001). Curvature discrimination thresholds increase with increasing central reference curvature. Although we measured thresholds for only two values of the central reference curvature, the Weber fractions are both around 0.4, suggesting that this main effect of central reference curvature can be accounted for by Weber-like behaviour. There is no main effect of surround curvature ratio (F(5,11) =1.99; P=0.093) and no interaction (F(5,11) =1.70; P=0.15). 3. Experiment 2 3.1. Methods In a second experiment, we set out to investigate the nature of the 3-D curvature contrast effect. Is it really a second-order effect (an effect of curvature), or can similar biases be obtained by inducers that contain only first-order structure (slant and tilt), or only zeroth-order structure (depth)? We have constructed three sets of surrounding inducers, a zeroth-order, a first-order and a second-order set. For the second-order condition, we have chosen two stimulus combinations that demonstrated the largest contrast effect in Experiment 1. As a control condition, we also add a surround curvature ratio of 1. Thus, the central reference paraboloid had a curvature of 76.6/m, and the surround curvature ratios could be 1/3, 1 or 3. For the first-order condition, we use cones as surrounding inducers. We take the average slant of the 2D projection of the paraboloids in the second-order condition to define the slant of the cones in the first-order condition. For the zeroth-order condition, the surrounding inducers are depth planes. These are chosen at the average depth of the 2D projection of the paraboloids in the second-order condition. Fig. 4 shows a schematic representation of the different stimulus sets we use in Experiment 2. We present the results of the first- and zeroth-order conditions in terms of equivalent curvature. For Experiment 2, we define the equivalent curvature of a cone as the curvature of a paraboloid with the same a erage slant. The equivalent curvature of a depth plane is the curvature of a paraboloid with the same a erage depth. Six naïve observers participate in Experiment 2 (AD, CG, CL, LW, MK and PL). All observers have normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity. The experimental set-up is the same as in Experiment 1. Again, the task of the observers is to determine whether the first or the second central paraboloid had the higher curvature. For each of the nine conditions, we obtain a psychometric curve, which gives us a bias and a curvature discrimination threshold. We present 12 different central test curvatures for each of the nine conditions. These 108 trials are all presented randomly in one session that takes on average 18 min to measure. In total, 15 of such sessions are measured, leading to a total of about 4.5 h measuring time per observer. 3.2. Results Results averaged over all six observers are presented in Fig. 5. Fig. 5A shows the bias as a function of surround order. White, grey and black bars denote surround curvature ratios of 1/3, 1 and 3, respectively. There is a clear main effect of surround curvature ratio (F(2,8) =27.4; P=0.0001): Biases for surround curvature ratios 1/3 and 3 are clearly opposite in sign, and they reveal a contrast effect. Results also show a clear interaction between surround curvature ratio and surround order (F(4,8) =3.73; P=0.010): there are clear biases for second- and first-order stimuli, but there is almost no bias for zeroth-order stimuli. This effect is entirely captured by the interaction; there is no main effect of surround order (F(2,8) =0.18; P=0.84). A pairwise comparison shows that the first- and secondorder biases are not significantly different (F(2,5) = 1.02; P=0.37 for the interaction). The zeroth-order bias is significantly lower than the second-order bias (F(2,5) =10.85; P=0.00028 for the interaction). However, the zeroth and first-order biases do not differ significantly (F(2,5) =2.30; P=0.12 for the interaction). Fig. 5B shows curvature discrimination thresholds as a function of surround order averaged over all six

observers. White, grey and black bars denote surround curvature ratios of 1/3, 1 and 3, respectively. Curvature discrimination thresholds are not affected by surround order (F(2,8) =0.41; P=0.67) or by surround curvature ratio (F(2,8) =1.88; P=0.16). S.F. te Pas, A.M.L. Kappers / Vision Research 41 (2001) 3829 3835 3833 4. Experiment 3 4.1. Methods In a third experiment, we want to check whether the small effect of zeroth-order that we find in Experiment 2 can be enhanced by setting the depth plane at the maximum depth instead of the a erage depth present in the paraboloid. We also make the slant of the inducing cones in the first-order stimuli equal to the maximum slant present in the paraboloids. We choose the same stimulus combinations that we use in Experiment 2, but we leave out all stimulus combinations with a surround curvature ratio of 1. In this experiment, the equivalent curvature of the cone and the depth plane are defined as the curvature of a paraboloid with the same maximum slant or the same maximum depth, respectively. Fig. 6. Experiment 3. (A) Bias as a function of surround order. (B) Curvature discrimination thresholds as a function of surround order. Results are averaged over six subjects. White and black bars show results for curvature ratios of 1/3 and 3, respectively. Six naïve observers participated in Experiment 3 (AW, AZ, FP, JS, LS and MR). All observers had (corrected to) normal visual acuity. The experimental set-up was the same as in Experiment 1. Again, the task of the observers was to determine whether the first or the second central paraboloid had the higher curvature. For each of the six conditions, we measure a psychometric curve by varying the central test curvature. This gives us a bias and a curvature discrimination threshold. We present 12 different central test curvatures for each of the six conditions. These 72 trials are all presented randomly in one session that takes on average 12 min to measure. In total, 15 of such sessions are measured, leading to a total of about 3 h measuring time per observer. 4.2. Results Fig. 5. Experiment 2. (A) Bias as a function of surround order. (B) Curvature discrimination thresholds as a function of surround order. Results are averaged over six subjects. White, grey and black bars show results for curvature ratios of 1/3, 1 and 3, respectively. Results averaged over all six observers are presented in Fig. 6. Fig. 6A shows the bias as a function of surround order. White and black bars denote surround curvature ratios of 1/3 and 3, respectively. The results show a main effect of surround curvature ratio (F(2,5) =117.9; P=0.0001): Biases for surround curvature ratios 1/3 and 3 are clearly opposite in sign; they reveal a contrast effect. Results also show an interac-

3834 S.F. te Pas, A.M.L. Kappers / Vision Research 41 (2001) 3829 3835 tion between surround curvature ratio and surround order (F(2,5) =16.6; P=0.0001): there are clear biases for second- and first-order stimuli and almost no bias for zeroth-order stimuli. A pairwise comparison shows that the first- and the second-order biases are not significantly different (F(1,3) =0.85; P=0.37 for the interaction). The zeroth-order bias is significantly lower than both the first- and second-order biases (F(1,3) = 24.3; P=0.0001 and F(1,3) =30.7; P=0.0001, respectively, for the interactions). There is no main effect of surround order (F(1,5) =1.10; P=0.35). Fig. 6B depicts curvature discrimination thresholds as a function of surround order. White and black bars show surround curvature ratios of 1/3 and 3, respectively. Curvature discrimination thresholds are not affected by surround order (F(2,5) =1.05; P=0.36). However, curvature discrimination thresholds are slightly higher for curvature surround ratio 3 than for curvature surround ratio 1/3 (F(1,5) =13.5; P= 0.00093). 5. Discussion In Experiment 1 we demonstrate that a clear 3-D curvature contrast effect occurs for motion-defined stimuli. The size of this contrast effect depends on the curvature ratio between the test and the reference surround. The 3-D curvature contrast effect for motiondefined stimuli is of the same order of magnitude as the effect reported by Curran and Johnston (1996) for shading-and-texture-defined stimuli and by te Pas et al. (2000) for stereoscopically defined stimuli. Biases are about 30% of the central reference curvature for surround curvature ratios of approximately 3. This suggests that the 3-D curvature contrast effect is present at a level where the different cues are already combined. It is likely to be shape-based, not cue-based. Ultimately, this conclusion will have to be tested by presenting the surround and central stimuli in different cues, and investigating whether a similar contrast effect can still be found. In Experiment 2, we set out to investigate the nature of the 3-D curvature contrast effect. We investigate whether the contrast effect was really an effect of the second-order structure (curvature) or whether it could also be induced by first-order (slant and tilt) or zerothorder (depth) structure. Besides paraboloids, we used depth planes (zeroth-order) that are at the same average depth as the second order paraboloids and cones (firstorder) that have the same average slant as the paraboloids as inducers. We found a clear contrast effect of the same order of magnitude for the first- and second-order inducers. For zeroth-order inducers, there was only a very small contrast effect. This was significantly smaller than the effect of second-order inducers. In Experiments 2 and 3, we find that the contrast effect was much smaller when zeroth-order inducers are used than when first- and second-order inducers are used. Although not significant, in Experiment 2, the contrast effect also seems slightly reduced with first-order inducers than with the second-order inducers. One can argue that any reduction of the effect when first- or zeroth-order inducers are used can be due to the fact that we use the a erage slant and depth of the paraboloids to define the cones and depth planes in Experiment 2. In Experiment 3, we test whether the contrast effect for first- and zeroth-order stimuli can be enhanced by using the maximum depth and slant that is present in the second-order paraboloids to define the first- and zeroth-order surrounds. As in Experiment 2, we find a strong contrast effect with both first- and second-order surrounds, but a very much reduced contrast effect with zeroth-order surrounds. When we use the maximum slant, we find no reduction of the contrast effect for the first-order inducers. This suggests that the maximum slant present in the stimulus is enough to evoke the reported contrast effect. The results make it highly unlikely that direct access to second-order shape information is used in this task. The results of Experiment 1 suggest that the 3-D curvature contrast effect is shape-based. It seems to be present at a level where different cues are already combined. Therefore, we feel justified in concluding that first-order shape information will be sufficient to induce the contrast effect in stimuli that are defined by cues other than structure-from-motion. More specifically, we feel that although second-order luminance information was certainly used in the experiments by Curran and Johnston (1996), first-order shape information probably led to the contrast effect for shading-andtexture-defined stimuli they reported. A similar conclusion can be drawn with regards to the contrast effect for stereoscopically defined stimuli reported by te Pas et al. (2000). Besides a bias, we also obtained curvature discrimination thresholds for all conditions. Weber fractions for curvature discrimination of paraboloids defined by structure-from-motion are known to be around 0.4 for a passive observer (van Damme, Oosterhoff, & van de Grind, 1994). Although we have measured for only two different central reference curvatures, we can roughly determine a Weber fraction from the curvature discrimination thresholds in Experiment 1. The crude Weber fractions that we obtain in this way are also around 0.4. We found no effect of surround order on curvature discrimination thresholds. In Experiment 3, we found that curvature discrimination thresholds for a surround curvature ratio of 3 were higher than those for a surround curvature ratio of 1/3. The fact that we did not find such an effect in Experiment 2 is due almost entirely to the fact that we included a surround curva-

S.F. te Pas, A.M.L. Kappers / Vision Research 41 (2001) 3829 3835 3835 ture ratio of 1. If we omit this condition and reanalyse the data, the difference in curvature discrimination thresholds between ratio 3 and ratio 1/3 is almost significant (P=0.058). This means that the difficulty of the task can indeed be affected by the nature of the surround. We conclude that the perceived shape of an object depends heavily on the nature of the surround. Relative, rather than absolute, measures determine the nature of the percept. Furthermore, changes in the slant and tilt (first-order) of the surround can bring about changes in curvature (second-order) perception just as readily as changes in the curvature of the surround. Thus, direct access to first-order shape information is probably sufficient to infer surface curvature. It is not necessary to have direct access to second-order shape information. Acknowledgements This research was supported by the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO, ALW grant 809-37-005; SGW grant 575-24-004). The authors would like to thank Erik Kreiter and Mohamed el Kaddouri for their work on this project. References Anstis, S. M. (1975). What does visual perception tell us about visual coding. In C. Blakemore, & M. S. Gazzaniga, Handbook of psychobiology (pp. 269 323). New York: Academic Press. Cornsweet, T. N. (1970). Visual perception. New York: Academic Press. Curran, W., & Johnston, A. (1996). Three-dimensional curvature contrast geometric or brightness illusion? Vision Research, 36(22), 3641 3653. Gibson, J. J. (1933). Adaptation, after-effect and contrast in the perception of curved lines. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 16(1), 1 31. Graham, M. E., & Rogers, B. J. (1982). Simultaneous and successive contrast effects in the perception of depth from motion parallax and stereoscopic information. Perception, 11, 247 262. Lunn, P. D., & Morgan, M. J. (1997). Discrimination of the spatial derivatives of horizontal binocular disparity. Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 14(2), 360 371. Rogers, B. J., & Cagenello, R. (1989). Disparity curvature and the perception of three-dimensional surfaces. Nature, 339, 135 137. te Pas, S. F., Rogers, B. J., & Ledgeway, T. (2000). A curvature contrast effect for stereoscopically defined surfaces. Current Psychology Letters: Brain, Beha iour and Cognition, 1(1), 117 126. van Damme, W. J. M., Oosterhoff, F. H., & van de Grind, W. A. (1994). Discrimination of 3-D shape and 3-D curvature from motion in active vision. Perception & Psychophysics, 55(3), 340 349.