VISUALLY INDUCED MOTION SICKNESS DURING COMPUTER GAME PLAYING

Similar documents
Effects of foveal retinal slip on visually induced motion sickness: a pilot study

Cybersickness, Console Video Games, & Head Mounted Displays

CAN GALVANIC VESTIBULAR STIMULATION REDUCE SIMULATOR ADAPTATION SYNDROME? University of Guelph Guelph, Ontario, Canada

Chapter 6. Experiment 3. Motion sickness and vection with normal and blurred optokinetic stimuli

Mitigating Visually Induced Motion Sickness: A Virtual Hand-Eye Coordination Task

COMPUTATIONAL ERGONOMICS A POSSIBLE EXTENSION OF COMPUTATIONAL NEUROSCIENCE? DEFINITIONS, POTENTIAL BENEFITS, AND A CASE STUDY ON CYBERSICKNESS

Multi variable strategy reduces symptoms of simulator sickness

Vertical display oscillation effects on forward vection and simulator sickness

Amy D. Wesley UGS Corporation Bronx, New York, USA Tina Brunetti Sayer Van Buren Township, Michigan, USA

COMPARING TECHNIQUES TO REDUCE SIMULATOR ADAPTATION SYNDROME AND IMPROVE NATURALISTIC BEHAVIOUR DURING SIMULATED DRIVING

Combined Pitch and Roll and Cybersickness in a Virtual Environment

VISUALLY INDUCED MOTION SICKNESS: AN INSIGHT FROM NEUROSCIENCE

Postural instability precedes motion sickness

The Perception of Optical Flow in Driving Simulators

Running head: MOTION SICKNESS AND VEHICLE CONTROL IN VIDEO GAMES. Control of a Virtual Vehicle Influences Postural Activity and Motion Sickness

arxiv: v1 [cs.hc] 6 Oct 2017

Cybersickness in the presence of scene rotational movements along di!erent axes

Factors Associated with Simulator Sickness in a High-Fidelity Simulator

Viewpoint Snapping to Reduce Cybersickness in Virtual Reality

Postural stability and sex differences in visually induced motion sickness A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF UNIVERSITY OF MINNESOTA BY

MULTIPLE EXPOSITION TO A DRIVING SIMULATOR REDUCES SIMULATOR SYMPTOMS FOR ELDERLY DRIVERS. Centre de recherche FRSQ du CHA de Québec

Effects of Duration of Immersion in a Virtual Reality Environment on Postural Stability

Vection and cybersickness generated by head-anddisplay motion in the Oculus Rift

The relationship between postural stability and virtual environment adaptation

Defense Technical Information Center Compilation Part Notice

Simulator Sickness Questionnaire: Twenty Years Later

WCRR2001 ID NUMBER: 169. STUDIES ABOUT MOTION SICKNESS Effect of combined lateral and roll oscillations

Virtual Guiding Avatar: An Effective Procedure to Reduce Simulator Sickness in Virtual Environments

What do I need to know about multisensory interactions?

May Cause Dizziness: Applying the Simulator Sickness Questionnaire to Handheld Projector Interaction

New VR Navigation Techniques to Reduce Cybersickness

Vection change exacerbates simulator sickness in virtual environments

Experimental Setup of Motion Sickness and Situation Awareness in Automated Vehicle Riding Experience

Comparison of Wrap Around Screens and HMDs on a Driver s Response to an Unexpected Pedestrian Crossing Using Simulator Vehicle Parameters

Effects of Navigation Speed on Motion Sickness Caused by an lmmersive Virtual Environment

The Visual Cliff Revisited: A Virtual Presence Study on Locomotion. Extended Abstract

Motion sickness issues in VR content

I T A L I A N J O U R N A L O F P U B L I C H E A L T H

Analysis of Driver s Head Tilt Using a Mathematical Model of Motion. Sickness

THE EFFECTS OF 0.2 HZ VARYING LATENCY WITH MS VARYING AMPLITUDE ON SIMULATOR SICKNESS IN A HELMET MOUNTED DISPLAY

DESIGNING AND CONDUCTING USER STUDIES

Study on Parallax Affect on Simulator Sickness in One-screen and Three-screen Immersive Virtual Environment

Frequency characteristics of visually induced motion sickness

DEMOGRAPHIC AND DRIVING PERFORMANCE FACTORS IN SIMULATOR ADAPTATION SYNDROME

Spatial Judgments from Different Vantage Points: A Different Perspective

Frequency Characteristics of Visually Induced Motion Sickness

Navigation in Immersive Virtual Reality The Effects of Steering and Jumping Techniques on Spatial Updating

Visually Induced Motion Sickness Predicted by Postural Instability

The Effect of Display Type and Video Game Type on Visual Fatigue and Mental Workload

Effect of Visual Realism on Cybersickness in Virtual Reality

Behavioural Realism as a metric of Presence

Motion Sickness from Combined Lateral and Roll Oscillation: Effect of Varying Phase Relationships

Exploring new dimensions in the assessment of virtual reality induced side effects Abstract.

Driving Simulator Sickness Management at Turner-Fairbank Highway Research Center

THE EFFECT OF 0.2 HZ AND 1.0 HZ FREQUENCY AND 100 MS AND MS AMPLITUDE OF LATENCY ON SIMULATORY SICKNESS IN A HEAD MOUNTED DISPLAY

The effect of a projected virtual reality training environment on vision symptoms in undergraduates

FINAL PROJECT BEST WAYS OF PRODUCING CYBERSICKNESS IN VR

VNG Balance Testing. VN415 / VO425 - Complete solutions for balance testing

THE ROLE OF HEAD MOVEMENTS IN SIMULATOR SICKNESS GENERATED BY A VIRTUAL ENVIRONMENT

Real-world analysis of motion sickness sensitivity questionnaires for cases of tall building movement

Preliminary findings on the Virtual Environment for Radiotherapy Training (VERT) system: simulator sickness and presence

Health & Safety

Are There Side Effects to Watching 3D Movies? A Prospective Crossover Observational Study on Visually Induced Motion Sickness

The influence of the visualization task on the Simulator Sickness symptoms - a comparative SSQ study on 3DTV and 3D immersive glasses

Effect of passengers active head tilt and opening/closure of eyes on motion sickness in lateral acceleration environment of cars

Insights into High-level Visual Perception

Part I Introduction to the Human Visual System (HVS)

Human Factors Consideration in Clinical Applications of Virtual Reality

MOTION PARALLAX AND ABSOLUTE DISTANCE. Steven H. Ferris NAVAL SUBMARINE MEDICAL RESEARCH LABORATORY NAVAL SUBMARINE MEDICAL CENTER REPORT NUMBER 673

Controlling Viewpoint from Markerless Head Tracking in an Immersive Ball Game Using a Commodity Depth Based Camera

GROUPING BASED ON PHENOMENAL PROXIMITY

The shape of luminance increments at the intersection alters the magnitude of the scintillating grid illusion

Relationship between the technical skills and eye-movement indicators of pilots

Eccentric gaze dynamics enhance vection in depth

MANUAL CONTROL WITH TIME DELAYS IN AN IMMERSIVE VIRTUAL ENVIRONMENT

Enhancing the Visually Induced Self-Motion Illusion (Vection) under Natural Viewing Conditions in Virtual Reality

Do 3D Stereoscopic Virtual Environments Improve the Effectiveness of Mental Rotation Training?

H.5.m. Information Interfaces and Presentation (e.g. HCI): Miscellaneous. Figure 1. Components of a virtual reality system from [5].

Quiz 2, Thursday, February 28 Chapter 5: orbital geometry (all the Laws for ocular motility, muscle planes) Chapter 6: muscle force mechanics- Hooke

2. GOALS OF THE STUDY 3. EXPERIMENT Method Procedure

Research on visual physiological characteristics via virtual driving platform

Comparison of Visual Discomfort and Visual Fatigue between Head-Mounted Display and Smartphone

The Influence of Visual Illusion on Visually Perceived System and Visually Guided Action System

A Vestibular Sensation: Probabilistic Approaches to Spatial Perception (II) Presented by Shunan Zhang

CSE 190: 3D User Interaction. Lecture #17: 3D UI Evaluation Jürgen P. Schulze, Ph.D.

NAVIGATIONAL CONTROL EFFECT ON REPRESENTING VIRTUAL ENVIRONMENTS

A Mathematical Model of Motion Sickness in 6DOF Motion and Its Application to Vehicle Passengers

Changes in Reaction Times and Executive Decision-Making following Exposure to Waterborne Motion

Motion Sickness: Effect of the Magnitude of Roll and Pitch Oscillation

3D display is imperfect, the contents stereoscopic video are not compatible, and viewing of the limitations of the environment make people feel

Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion

TRAFFIC SIGN DETECTION AND IDENTIFICATION.

/ Impact of Human Factors for Mixed Reality contents: / # How to improve QoS and QoE? #

Motion Perception II Chapter 8

A Wearable Device Providing a Visual Fixation Point for the Alleviation of Motion Sickness Symptoms

Regression models for provoking motion sickness in tilting trains

THE EFFECTIVENESS OF SAFETY CAMPAIGN VMS MESSAGES - A DRIVING SIMULATOR INVESTIGATION

DIFFERENCE BETWEEN A PHYSICAL MODEL AND A VIRTUAL ENVIRONMENT AS REGARDS PERCEPTION OF SCALE

EFFECT OF ACCELERATION FREQUENCY ON SPATIAL ORIENTATION MECHANISMS

Motion sickness evaluation and comparison for a static driving simulator and a dynamic driving simulator

Transcription:

Irt er n) k. or VISUALLY INDUCED MOTION SICKNESS DURING COMPUTER GAME PLAYING C. T. Guo, Chi Wai Tsoi, Yiu Lun Wong, Kwok Chun Yu & R.H.Y. So Department of Industrial Engineering and Logistics Management, Hong Kong University of Science & Technology, Hong Kong i): er er 2, y- 'ece Ie l a- 3. er n: 19 of ill of 12 The objective of this study is to examine the effects of active control verses passive watching on visually induced motion sickness (VIMS) during game playing, as well as the types of eye motions. Two experiments are reported: Experiment one compared sickness levels in active and passive conditions and Experiment two studied the effects of eye fixation during passive watching. Prelinlinary results show that: (i) rated nausea levels during passive watching were significantly higher than active playing (p < 0.05) and (ii) nausea level during passive watching was significantly suppressed by eye fixation (p < 0.05). Introduction Motion sickness can be characterized by a series of symptoms like nausea, sweating, dizziness, vomiting, etc. Study of motion sickness dated back more than one hundred years ago (Reason, 1978). Levels of motion sickness reported by individuals can be affected by susceptibility, age, gender, type of motion stimulus, frequency of exposure, etc. (e.g., Kennedy et at., 2010; So and Ujike, 2010). Stimulus that can provoke motion sickness, according to the sensory organs being explicitly involved, can be categorized into three types: (i) visual motion, (ii) vestibular motion, and (iii) combined visual and vestibular motions. Motion sickness that is purely provoked by visual motion is called visually induced motion sickness (VIMS). With the development of virtual reality (VR) technique, incidences of VIMS when watching movies, VR simulation or computer games have been reported in recent years. Over eighty percent of individuals exposed to VR simulations of 20 minutes reported increases in sickness symptoms (e.g., Cobb et at., 1999; Kennedy and Stanney, 1998; Wilson et al., 2000). Incidents ofvims have also been reported among users of various immersive video game systems, such as Xbox, PlayStation, and Wii (e.g., Merhi et al., 2007; Stoffregen et at., 2008). No doubt, the occurrence ofvims can reduce the joy of entertainment. More importantly, efficiency and accuracy of performance can be reduced in scientific applications of VR technology. Hence, research to study factors affecting VIMS is desirable and meaningful..

52 C. T. Guo et al. Active playing and passive watching The sensory conflict theory is widely cited as a theory to describe the needed conditions for motion sickness to occur (Reason, 1978). The theory explains that motion sickness is likely to occur when there is conflict among signals coming from different sense organs, or when there is conflict between real and expected forms of these signals. Since the "conflict" as defined in the theory is a neural psychological signal that is difficult to quantify, direct verification of this theory can be difficult. However, its ability to describe situations that can provoke motion sickness has been widely accepted. One such example is that a taxi driver should experience less motion sickness than the passengers. Rolnick and Lubow (1991) reported a "driver" and "passengers" experiment. The drivers executed a planned series of vehicle rotations and the passengers just watched and experienced the motion. Results showed that those who had controls of the rotations reported less symptoms of motion sickness than those who just experienced the motion. Xiao et al. (2011) extended the hypothesis to see whether the "control effect" was just limited to physical motion or could be extended to visual motion. One group of subjects drove (the driver) a computer driving simulation game and another group of subjects watched the video (the passenger). Results showed that the influence of control on motion sickness incidence is not limited to vehicle controls. It also applied to the control of virtual environment. Given the importance of eye motion in the generation ofvims (e.g., Ebenholtz et ai., 1994, Ebenholtz, 2001; Hu et ai.; Ji et al., 2009; Yang etal., 2011; Guo et ai., 2011), we hypothesized that eye motions of active and passive gaming is different and this difference could be related to the occurrence ofvims. Eye motions and VIMS The presence of eye motion had been shown to increase VIMS (e.g., Ji et ai., 2009; Yang et al., 2011). However, eye motions in these studies were provoked by watching rotating striped patterns on optokinetic drums. Studies related to eye movements and their effects on VIMS during game playing could not be found. g In other words, although eye fixation has been shown to significantly reduce VIMS when watching rotating drums, its effect on VIMS among players of computer games is not known. If, indeed, eye fixation can be verified to reduce VIMS among game players, it would provide a way to design a VIMS-free computer game. Objectives and hypothesizes The objectives of the first experiment were: (1) to compare VIMS levels between subjects who were actively playing computer games and subjects who were passively watching recorded videos; (2) to compare eye motions under these two modes of playing; and (3) to study effects of eye motion by using eye fixation on VIMS during active game playing. The objectives of the second experiment were: (1) to study types of eye motion during passive viewing conditions; and (2) to investigate whether eye fixation can reduce VIMS during passive viewing conditions. VIMS Ie v, b, 3 D TI (I ac gr co ill' (h Je, E) pi: SI1 7. on faj Th by fol th. tic "f( VH "sl sui (B Pr Th Mi

Visually induced motion sickness during computer game playing 53 ~d at 19 ~d al -y In ld l) :d le ;s. 0 st )f.p :e.0 n., IS,e )., g :s ~r n s e n :- s ;; o e 5 levels provoked by passive watching are hypothesized to be higher than those provoked by active game playing (Hypothesis I), and VIMS levels are hypothesized to be reduced by eye fixations (Hypothesis II). Methods 3 males and 9 female, participated in the first experiment; 3 males and 4 females, participated in Experiment two. All of the subjects were undergraduate students or postgraduate students at the Hong Kong University of Science & Technology. They all signed subject consent forms and passed eye acuity tests before participating the experiment. They received money compensation of 50 HKDlhour. The experiment had been approved by the human subject committee of the Hong Kong University of Science & Technology. Design of experiment The video game named Mirror's Edge, developed by EA Digital Illusions CE (DICE) was used in our experiment. Mirror's Edge is a single-player first person action-adventure video game allowing for a wide range of actions. Players enjoy great freedom of movement. In this study, subjects used mouse and keyboard to control the avatar in the game from a first-person perspective. Due to rapid visual motion involved in the game, players have reported incidences of motion sickness (http://forums.steampowered.comlforums/showthread.php?t= 1683 987). All subjects in experiment one and two did not have experience in the game before training. Experiment one used a within-subject design with 3 conditions: (Al active game playing with no eye fixation; (B) active game playing with eye fixation; and (C) passive viewing of recorded videos of game playing. Participants rested for at least 7 days between two successive conditions to avoid adaptation. A training session one week before the first condition was conducted to help the participants to be familiar with the game. The 12 participants were equally divided into 2 groups. The video that subjects watched in the passive viewing condition were recorded by another subjects during their active game playing conditions. Procedures were followed to match the game playing styles of these two groups of subjects. In short, the frequencies of pressing keys during the training were recorded to classify participants into "fast" moving players and "slow" moving players. In this study, a "fast" player would watch recording from another "fast" player during the passive viewing conditions. Similarly, "slow" players would watch recordings from another "slow" players during passive viewing conditions. Experiment two also used within subject design with two conditions: (A) passive watching without eye fixation; and (B) passive watching with eye fixation. Procedure and apparatus The procedure and apparatus of experiment one and two was the same. The game Mirror's Edge was run in a computer with Windows 7 Enterprise System and

54 C. T Guo et al. a 27-inch LCD monitor 50 cm away from subjects' eyes. During the exposure, all light was turned off except that from the stimulus. Subj ects' eye motions were measured by a 16-mm eye tracker VTl produced by Eye Tech Digital System. During each session, participants were required to place their heads on a chin-rest stand to minimize head motion (Stoffregen and Smart, 1994). At the beginning, each subject went through the calibration of the eye tracker and fill in a pre-exposure simulator sickness questionnaire (SSQ: Kennedy et ai., 1993). Subjects were then exposed to the respective conditions according to a balanced presentation order and with 7 -day rest in between. During the 30 minutes exposure, rated levels of nausea were measured using a 7-point nausea rating adopted from Golding and Kerguelen (1992) at every five minutes. Each player's performance in terms of mouse position and keystrokes was recorded by software named Macro Recorder, and their screenshot of playing were recorded in the form of "avi" video by software named PlayClaw. Also, their head positions were recorded by a webcam to verify that their heads did not move. After the 30-minute exposure, all participants were instructed to fill in the post-exposure SSQ. Results Pearson correlation test results show that there was significant correlation between the two dependent variables: post SSQ total score (SSQT) and the average 7 point nausea rating across 30 minutes, in both active (Pearson Corr.: 0.715, p = 0.009) and passive conditions (Pearson Carr.: 0.8l2,p = 0.001). In Experiment one, the average 7 point nausea rating across 30 minutes in passive watching (mean: 2.2) was significantly higher than active playing (mean: 1.7) (t= -3.592,p < 0.01) and the same significant result was found in 8-people sub-group where all subjects' susceptibility rating were equal, or lower than, 3 (t = -3.173,p < 0.05, left part of Figure 1); the 7 point nausea rating at 30 minute in passive watching (mean: 3.5) was significantly higher than active playing (mean: 2.7) (z = -2.232,p < 0.05, right part of Figure I), and the same significant result could also be found in low susceptibility sub-group (z = -2.041, P < 0.05). The average 7 point nausea data were verified to be normal and with equal variance, so the significant results were based on paired t-test, and the 7 point nausea data at 30 minute was non-normal and also failed to apply Box-cox transformation, so the significant results were based on Wilcoxon Signed Rank Tests. For sub-groups with higher sickness susceptibility (> =4), no significant difference between the active and passive conditions was found. By adding eye fixation, neither sub-groups nor the whole group's data showed significant reduction on 7 point nausea rating. For post SSQ scores, no significant result was found. In Experiment two, the post SSQ sub-scores of nausea (SSQN) was significantly reduced by eye fixation (nfix: 40.4, fix: 27.2, t = -2.8, p < 0.05, Figure 2) based on paired t-test result. There was no other significant result between fixation and 4 l.s 1 2.5 2: 1.5 1 1).5 o Fig 10 ' 00 ' so 40 20 o Fi! se2 SCI n: no] Val Th up ga: th, (x, Fo co: lik bu ph

e, IS al,st :h Ie en ler of nd of.er, lie ify ere een Jint '09) Jas- ~an: Jple [l, 3 [lute ean: :sult The e,so ta at J the with ctive s nor :. For antly lased 1 and Visually induced motion sickness during computer game playing 55 4~--------------------' 1S17polnt_ &rti"" 3S Cl7poln'_passlve l C 7polnt_&rtIYI! fl. 2S 2 ls 1 All 6,.----------.., Cil7polnt at 30m In.ct1Y1! S 07polnt at 30mln passive 1]7polnt at 30m In.ct111e fl. Figure 1. The left figure is the average 7 point nausea data across 30 minutes, and the right figure is the 7 point nausea data at 30 minute. ~~-----------~ so 4.5...------------., 3.5 4 Clflx 'Onflx 3 los 2 L5 1 o.s O+-~~~~--~~~~~~ 7 pointaw. 7 p"'"' 3(Imln Figure 2. Left part the figure is the average SSQ total score (SSQT), SSQ nausea score (SSQN), SSQ oculomotor score (SSQO) and the SSQ disorientation score (SSQD) of all subjects. The right part of the figure is the average 7 point nausea rating across 30 minutes and the rating at 30 minute of all subjects. non-fixation conditions. However, a trend could be observed for all the dependent variables that eye fixation did reduce VIMS. The eye tracker recorded both the left and right eye position ill terms of pixels. Base upon the illdividual calibration data, lillear regressions were conducted to obtaill the gaze positions ill degrees using left and right eye data. Regression results showed that linear model was valid to predict the gaze position, with left eye (x, y) and right (x, y) explailled more than 95% of the variance (RSS > 95%). For Experiment one, overall variance of eye motion durillg active game playillg conditions and passive viewillg conditions were statistically the same. We would like to remind the readers that participants did not watch their own recorded videos but videos of another game players with similar playillgstyles. For the active game playillg condition with eye fixation, which was supposed to suppress eye motions,

56 C. T. Guo et al. no significant reduction in variance of eye motion was found. This indicated that even with an eye fixation pointer placed at the centre ofthe screen and with instructions to fixate ones' eyes, it was difficult to fixate ones' eyes while actively playing the game. For Experiment two, eye fixation significantly reduced eye motion in both horizontal and vertical directions. A Matlab program was run to identify duration of stop «= 2 dps), smooth pursuit «=40 dps and > 2dps) and saccadic eye motion (>40 dps). On average, 62% of the time, eye motion was categorized as "stop", 37% of the time categorized as "smooth pursuit" and no more than 1% of the time categorized to "saccadic". There was no significant correlation between these durations and sickness level. In both experiments, there is not significant difference in VIMS score between genders. Discussion and conclusion Hypothesis I was supported by the nausea data from sub-group with low motion sickness susceptibility and the whole group. In other words, VIMS levels among participants with low sickness susceptibility were significantly lower when they were actively playing the game than when they were watching recorded videos of the same game. This could be explained by a ceiling effect on participants with high susceptibility, in which subjects already got very sick in the active condition, and even there is still room for them to get higher sickness in the passive condition, the difference between the two cases could be small. Hypothesis II was supported only by SSQ nausea sub-scores. In other words, eye fixation was only associated with significant reduction in post-exposure nausea sub-scores. Compared with past studies (Ji et at., 2009; Yang et at., 2010; Guo et at., 2011, 2012) in which OKN (optokinetic nystagmus, a type of eye motion produced by large screen moving scene) occurred nearly 100% during the whole 30-minute period when participants were watching rotating drums, OKN period in Experiment two was lower than 40% of the time. Subjects tended to relax their eyes on the screen most of the time. These observations raised a possible relationship between OKN and VIMS. Further studies are desirable. In summary, the comparison between active and passive condition with more subjects are needs, to further verify the hypothesis. Besides, further investigation on eye motion in terms of retinal slip in the passive condition, as well as the types of eye motion needs to be done. Acknowledgement The authors would like to thank the Research Grant Councils of the Hong Kong Government for partially supporting the study.

Visually induced motion sickness during computer game playing 57 lat lcng!dof Ion p", the ese ion Jng hey s of vith ion, ion, eye Isea 3uo tion 101e,d in ~yes ship sub- 11 on :s of ~ong References Barrett, I (2004). Side Effects of Virtual Environments: A Review of the Literature. Australian Goverrunent, Department of Defence, Defence Science and Technology Organization, Command and Control Division Information Sciences Laboratory, DSTO-TR-1419. Coady, E.A. (2010). The effect of drug mitigated motion sickness on physiological and psychophysical performance: Thesis of School of Graduate Studies of Master of Science in Kinesiology Department of Human Kinetics and Recreation Memorial University of Newfoundland, pp. 1-39. Cobb, S.VG., Nichols, S., Ramsey, A. and Wilson, IR (1999). Virtual reality induced symptoms and effects (VRISE). Presence, 8(2), 169. 186. Chen, YC, Xiao Dong, Jens Hagstrom and Stoffregen T.A. (2011). Control of a virtual ambulation influences body movement and motionsickness. BIO Web of Conferences vol. 1, pp. 16 (2011). Ebenholtz, S.M. (1992). Motion sickness and oculomotor systems in virtual environments, Teleoperators and Virtual Environments 1(3),302-305. Ebenholtz, S.M., Cohen, M.M. and Linder, BJ. (1994). The possible role of nystagmus in motion sickness: a hypothesis. Aviat Space Environ Med.: 65(11): 1032-1035. Ebenholtz, S.M. (2001). Oculomotor systems and perception. Cambridge University Press, c2001. Guo, CT., Ji, IT.T. and So, RH.Y. (2011). Could OKAN be an objective indicator of the susceptibility to visually induced motion sickness? Proceedings ofieee Virtual Reality 2011, 19-23 March, Singapore. Guo, C.T., Yang, IX and So, R.H.Y (2011). "The effects of optokinetic nystagmus on visually induced motion sickness after the confounding effects of retinal slip is controlled" i-perception 2(4), 215. Guo, c.t. and So, RH.Y. (2012). "Effects of foveal retinal slip on visually induced motion sickness: a pilot study" Proceedings of 56th Annual Meeting of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society October 22-26, 2012, Boston. Golding, I E (1998). Motion sickness susceptibility questionnaire revised and its relationship to other forms of sickness. Brain Research Bulletin, 47(5), 507-516. Golding, IE (2006). Motion sickness susceptibility. Auton. Neurosci: Basic and Clinical, 129, pp. 67-76. Hu, S., StemRM., VaseyM.W and KochK.L. (1989). Motion sickness and gastric myoelectric activity as a function of speed of rotation of a circular vection drunl. Aviat Space Environ Med. May, 60(5): 411-414. Ji, IT.T., So, R.H.Y, Lor, E, Cheung, T.ER., Howarth, P. and Stanney, K. (2005). A Search for Possible neural pathways Leading to Visually Induced Motion Sickness. VISION, 17, 2,pp. 131-134. Ji, IT.T., So, R.H.Y. and Chueng, R.T.E (2009). Isolating the Effects ofvection and Optokinetic Nystagmus on Optokinetic Rotation-Induced Motion Sickness. Human Factors, vol. 51, no. 5, 739-751.

58 C. T. Guo et al. Kennedy, R.S., and Kennedy, R.C. (2007). The Past, Present and Future of Research in Visually Induced MotionSickness. Proceedings (VIMS2007), 10-12 Dec., Hong Kong. pp. 3-8. Kennedy, R.S., Drexler, I and Kennedy, R.C. (2010). Research in visually induced motion sickness. Applied Ergonomics, 41(4), pp. 494-503. Kennedy, R.S., Lane, N. E., Berbaum, K. S. and Lilienthal, M. G. (1993). Simulator sickness questionnaire: An enhanced method for quantifying simulator sickness. International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 3, 203-220. Rolnick, A, and Lubow, R. E. (1991). Why the driver is rarely motion sick? The role of controllability in motion sickness. Ergonomics, 34, 867-879. Scibora, L.M., Villard, S., Bardy, B., and Stoffregen, T.A. (2007). Wider Stance Reduces Body Sway and Motion Sickness. Proceedings (VIMS2007), 10-12 Dec., Hong Kong. pp. 18-23. So, R.H., Ho, A and Lo, w.i. (2001). A metric to quantify virtual scene movement for the study of cybersickness: definition, implementation, and verification. Presence, 10, pp. 192-215. So, R.H.Y. and Ujike, H. (2010). Visually induced motion sickness, visual stress and photosensitive epileptic seizures: what do they have in common? - Preface to the special issue. Applied Ergonomics, 41(4), pp. 491-393. Stanney, K.M., Mourant, R.R. and Kennedy, R.S. (1998). Human Factors Issues in Virtual Environments:A Review of the Literature. MIT Press Journals, 328-351. Steele, IE. (1961). Motion Sickness and Spatial perception, A Theoretical Study. Aeronautical Systems Division, Air Force Systems Command, United States Air Force, Wright-Patterson Air Force Base, Ohio. Stoffregen, T.A., and Smart, L.1. (1998). Postural instability precedes motion sickness. Brain Research Bulletin, 47, 437-448. Stoffregen, T.A., Faugloire, E., Yoshida, K., Flanagan, M.B. and Merhi, O. (2008). Motion Sickness and Postural Sway in Console Video Games. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society April 2008 50: 322-331. de Vries S.C., Bos, IE., van Emmerik, M.L. and Groen, E.L. (2007). Internal and external Field of View: computer games and cybersickness. Proceedings of the First International Symposium on Visually Induced Motion Sickness, Fatigue, and Photosensitive Epileptic Seizures (VIMS2007), 10-12 Dec., Hong Kong. pp.89-95. Xiao D., Yoshida, K., and Stoffregen, I.A (20 11). Control of a Virtual Vehicle Influences Postural Activity and Motion Sickness. J Exp Psychol Appl. 20 II Jun; 17(2): 128-38, pp. 1-45. Yang, IX., Guo, C.T., So, R.H.Y. and Cheung, R.T.F. (2011). Effects ofeye Fixation on Visually Induced Motion Sickness: Are they caused by Changes in Retinal Slip Velocity? Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, September, vol. 55, no. I, 1220-1224.