The effect of interspecific competition on the foraging behavior of the Eastern Gray Squirrel

Similar documents
Distance from protective tree cover affects foraging times of urbanized gray squirrels at the University of Maryland Abstract

Eastern gray squirrel (Sciurus carolinensis) foraging patterns in the presence of aerial predators

Investigating the effect of differential elevation of food on foraging behavior of the Eastern grey squirrel (Sciurus carolinensis)

Differential Foraging Patterns of Rodents and Birds in a Restored Prairie

THE ROLE OF SCIENCE IN WESTERN GRAY SQUIRREL ENHANCEMENT IN OAK WOODLANDS OF SOUTH PUGET SOUND

Varying levels of bird activity within a forest understory dominated by the invasive glossy buckthorn (Rhamnus frangula)

Wildlife Habitat Patterns & Processes: Examples from Northern Spotted Owls & Goshawks

Investigation of the anthropogenic factors influencing Eastern fox squirrel, Sciurus niger, distribution and abundance in urban residential areas

A Survey of Native and Introduced Anolis Lizards in Southern Florida

Influence of Habitat on the Aggressive Behavior of 13-lined Ground Squirrels (Spermophilus tridecemlineatus) Hui Chien Tan

Foraging Behavior of Rodent and Songbird Populations, Examined with Variation of Predatory Risk

Community Crepuscular Define population Crypsis Natural History Ecosystem. each other and their Fecundity Fledgling Diet, habitat,

Protecting the Endangered Mount Graham Red Squirrel

Distribution of Tree Squirrels in California: A Species Distribution Modeling Approach to Analyzing Data

Oak Woodlands and Chaparral

DIET DIFFERENTIATION AND HABITAT SELECTION OF BIRDS IN FORESTED AND CLEAR-CUT AREAS

Where are the Birds? Urban Birds in a Heat Island

POPULAT A ION DYNAMICS

Golden Eagle (Aquila chrysaetos)

Loggerhead Shrike (Lanius ludovicianus)

The effects of nest box location on Tree Swallow ( Tachycineta bicolor ) productivity and nest. success at Beaverhill Bird Observatory, Alberta

Landscaping for Wildlife TOM PATRICK, President, Windstar Wildlife Institute

Are pine martens the answer to grey squirrel control?

Abstract The American Redstart is a wood warbler that is in population decline in northern Michigan.

Emily Gillmore. Intern at the Beaverhill Bird Observatory

APPENDIX 15.6 DORMOUSE SURVEY

THE CONSERVATION OF THE RED SQUIRREL IN ITALY PROGETTO LIFE+ EC-SQUARE ROSSOSCOIATTOLO. Bertolino Sandro & Task Force EC-SQUARE Università di Torino

OLD NESTS AS CUES FOR NEST-SITE SELECTION: AN EXPERIMENTAL TEST WITH RED-WINGED BLACKBIRDS

* * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * For Judges Use Only

Ferruginous Hawk Buteo regalis

EMERGENCE OF COMMUNICATION IN TEAMS OF EMBODIED AND SITUATED AGENTS

The Biodiversity Box (Biodiversity, Habitat Loss, Invasive Species, and Conservation)

Variation in Bird Diversity with Habitat Quality in Hobart, Tasmania

Ecological Impacts of Australian Ravens on. Bush Bird Communities on Rottnest Island

Quantifying acclimation levels of the northern mockingbird Mimus polyglottos in the presence of people. Jordan Rutherford.

Farr wind farm: A review of displacement disturbance on dunlin arising from operational turbines

Wintering Bird Occupancy and Detection in Response to Proximity to Water and Eastern Screech-Owl Call Playback

Estimating Seasonal Avian Diversity in an Urban Wetland in Columbus, Ohio. Kaitlin Carr 20 April 2018

Hair tube survey for the presence of red squirrels, Sciurus vulgaris, in Cwm Berwyn, mid Wales, 2013

Tree Swallow (Tachycineta bicolour)

Assignment 2 1) DAY TREATMENT TOTALS

National Fish and Wildlife Foundation Executive Summary for the Bog Turtle

VIRGINIA WORKING LANDSCAPES

Step-by-Step Instructions for Documenting Compliance on the Bald Eagle Form For WSDOT s On-Call Consultants

Detecting Area Sensitivity: A Comment on Previous Studies

Lasiurus blossevillii (Red Bat)

Connecting Country s Nest Box Program

APPENDIX H. Small Mammal and Bat Surveys

CONSIDER THE CHIPMUNK; RECONSIDER THE ROAD: ROAD NOISE EFFECTS ON EAVESDROPPING SYSTEMS IN EASTERN CHIPMUNKS

American Kestrel. Appendix A: Birds. Falco sparverius. New Hampshire Wildlife Action Plan Appendix A Birds-183

Effects of Prescribed Burning on Golden-winged Warbler (Vermivora chrysoptera) Habitat and Populations in the Cumberland Mountains

Endangered Species Profile: The Sun Parakeet. By Student Name, Class Period

Project summary. Key findings, Winter: Key findings, Spring:

Cordilleran Flycatcher (Empidonax occidentalis)

Golden Eagle (Aquila chrysaetos) Management Indicator Species Assessment Ochoco National Forest

Birdify Your Yard: Habitat Landscaping for Birds. Melissa Pitkin Klamath Bird Observatory

Progress Report. Population Size and Ecology of Giant Nuthatch (Sitta magna) in Thailand Introduction

INVASIVE SPECIES AND SEABIRDS MIDDLE SCHOOL CURRICULUM

Effects of a New Pedestrian Pathway in Grand Teton National Park on Breeding Sagebrush Songbirds

MASSABESIC AUDUBON CENTER SCHOOL PROGRAMS Let your students Find Science in Nature! Programs correlate with the N.H. Science Literacy Frameworks

Recreational Trails and Bird Communities

Species-Specific Effects on Occupancy

The Long Point Causeway: a history and future for reptiles. Scott Gillingwater

Is chocolate for the birds? Featured scientist: Skye Greenler from Colorado College

B IRD CONSERVATION FOREST BIRD SURVEY ENTERS FINAL WINTER V OLUME 11, NUMBER 1 JANUARY Board of. Trustees. Forest bird survey 1

Conservation Biology 4554/5555. Modeling Exercise: Individual-based population models in conservation biology: the scrub jay as an example

National Park Service U.S. Department of the Interior. Haleakala National Park Makawao, Maui, Hawai'i

Winter Ranges of Birds in New Hampshire; Changes in the Past Two Decades

Trinity River Bird and Vegetation Monitoring: 2015 Report Card

MS.LS2.A: Interdependent Relationships in Ecosystems. MS.LS2.C: Ecosystem Dynamics, Functioning, and Resilience. MS.LS4.D: Biodiversity and Humans

THE EFFECT OF ANTHROPOGENIC NOISE ON VEERY SINGING BEHAVIOR

BEHAVIOUR OF LEPIDODACTYLUS LUGUBRIS ON HERON ISLAND, GREAT BARRIER REEF, AND A RECORD OF GEHYRA DUBIA ON THAT ISLAND

Population Patterns. Math 6.SP.B.4 6.SP.B.5 6.SP.B.5a 6.SP.B.5b 7.SP.B.3 7.SP.A.2 8.SP.A.1. Time: 45 minutes. Grade Level: 3rd to 8th

Learning about Biodiversity. Student Handouts

Dublin Mountains Visitor Centre Red Squirrel (Sciurus vulgaris) Conservation Management Plan ROUGHAN & O DONOVAN

Received: 8 November 2013 / Revised: 8 January 2014 / Accepted: 15 January 2014 Ó Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2014

NEW ASSOCIATION IN BIO-S-POLYMER PROCESS

Comparison of Soundscapes in Disturbed and Undisturbed Environments. Kathryn Ganter. Dominica Study Abroad Dr. Woolley and Dr.

habitat islands; area effects in a Small mammal and raptor densities in south Swedish agricultural landscape Study area

STATUS OF SEABIRDS ON SOUTHEAST FARALLON ISLAND DURING THE 2010 BREEDING SEASON

National Parks Challenges A True to Our Nature Educational Resource

Variable impacts of alien mink predation on birds, mammals and amphibians of the Finnish. a long-term experimental study. Archipelago: Peter Banks

Hermit Crab Shell Selection Based on Coloration

Are Horseshoe Crab Eggs a Limiting Resource for Red Knots?

ICSRS Analysis of scientific research about the reasons of red squirrel population decline in the UK

USING CHRISTMAS BIRD COUNT DATA TO DETERMINE POPULATION TRENDS OF FIVE BIRD SPECIES. by Thomas R. Hamilton

Effects of human activity on the foraging behavior of sanderlings Calidris alba

Peregrine Falcon Falco peregrinus

CORE *REQUIRED OF ALL COMMUNITIES* CIIY TOWN COUNIY YES YES YES YES YES

Supplemental Lab. EXTINCTION GAME

1.1 Students know how to use maps, globes, and other geographic tools to acquire, process, and report information from a spatial perspective.

Wellington City forests: Rodent monitoring report. November 2018

Piping Plovers - An Endangered Beach Nesting Bird, and The Threat of Habitat Loss With. Predicted Sea Level Rise in Cape May County.

Caitlyn Gillespie and Joseph J. Fontaine

Bald Eagles Productivity Summary Lake Clark National Park and Preserve Cook Inlet Coastline

AVIAN USE OF ROADSIDE HABITAT IN THE SOUTHERN DRIFT PLAINS OF NORTH DAKOTA AND IMPLICATIONS FOR CATTAIL (TYPHA SPP.) MANAGEMENT

Note: Some squares have continued to be monitored each year since the 2013 survey.

Shorebird Migration in Nebraska: Stopover Habitat Decisions in a Vanishing Landscape. Caitlyn Gillespie and Joseph J. Fontaine

THE STATUS OF SPARROWS IN LESOTHO, SOUTHERN AFRICA ABSTRACT

Instructor Guide: Birds in Human Landscapes

Transcription:

The effect of interspecific competition on the foraging behavior of the Eastern Gray Squirrel Jessica Dassen, Rachel Gerardy, Amberly Holcomb, and Lydia Nichols-Russell University of Maryland, Department of Biology (BSCI 335), College Park, Maryland Interspecific competition between the Eastern Gray Squirrel, Sciurus carolinensis, and the European Red Squirrel, Sciurus vulgaris, has been shown to negatively affect both populations growth rates by limiting resource availability. In response to the outside threat of another competitor, it is hypothesized that S. carolinensis will modify the time of their foraging behavior in order to more optimally collect resources. A study was conducted in the urban environment of a college campus measuring the differences in foraging behavior based on the absence or presence of a simulated competitor. Research was conducted by evaluating a total of 33 squirrels appearing in one of three scenarios; empty plot, simulated competitor, and non-competitor object. Results showed no statistical difference between the proportion of time spent foraging in the plot containing the competitor or object and an empty plot. More trials could be completed in replicate studies to ensure the most accurate results. It is recommended future studies be done in an environment with little human presence as that was believed to have possibly affected the results. KEY WORDS: Interspecific competition, foraging, behavior, Eastern gray squirrel, European red squirrel, competitor, niche, McKeldin Mall, University of Maryland, ANOVA.

When an individual s niche overlaps with another individual s niche, there is competition for the resources within that environment (Peterson 2003). When this competition occurs between individuals in separate species it is called interspecific competition (Schoener 1983). This study focuses on the effect of interspecific competition on foraging behaviors in a small species of North American mammal. It is not uncommon for an individual's foraging behavior to be affected by external elements. Animals modify their foraging behavior in response to factors such as density of food source, perceived risk of predation, and interference from other foragers (Ward 2000). An optimal forager will use behavioral strategies that increases the payoff, or resources obtained, while minimizing risks of mortality. In many cases, this means modifying the time spent foraging in order to remain vigilant of outside threats. This paper will takes a look at the foraging habits of the eastern gray squirrels (Sciurus carolinensis) and how it is modified in response to specific outside factors. In Europe, eastern gray squirrels are invasive and directly compete with European red squirrels (Sciurus vulgaris) for resources. It has been found that when these two species of squirrel compete for the same resources, there is a decrease in reproduction rates in both types of squirrel (Gurnell et al. 2004). Since the amount of resources available can determine a population s growth rate, limiting resources can decrease a population s growth rate (Grover 1997). It was hypothesized that over time the competition over resources between the two squirrels will result in a decline in population size and eventual extinction of the red squirrel population. Because the appearance of an invasive squirrel population poses such a threat to the longevity of the native squirrel population, it is suspected that current squirrel populations will behave

differently in the presence of the competitor in order to outcompete it. More specifically, this study hypothesizes that in the presence of another squirrel competitor, the average time the Eastern gray squirrel spends foraging will be different from the average time the Eastern gray squirrel spends foraging without the presence of a competitor. To test this theory, the foraging behavior of gray squirrels were monitored with and without the presence of a competitor squirrel. By performing this study, we hope to gain further insight into the behavioral patterns of small mammal populations. If our hypothesis is not rejected, our study will further expand upon the knowledge of interspecific competition as well as provide important information on invasive mammals and their effect on native mammals in foreign environments. Also, as this is a species that is in close proximity with humans, whether in forested or other urban areas, it is important that we understand their behaviors. MATERIALS AND METHODS Study site and species -- This study was conducted on McKeldin Mall, the center of the University of Maryland s campus, in College Park, Maryland. College Park is less than 10 miles from Washington, D.C., and is a highly urbanized area. McKeldin Mall is an open grassy area with several sidewalks and a large fountain; this area is similar to a typical quad on many other college campuses. More than 35,000 students attend the University of Maryland, and a great deal of students, faculty and staff visit McKeldin Mall each day (University of Maryland 2016). Specifically, our study was conducted on the southern edge of McKeldin Mall in an open mulched area beneath oak trees. Our study site is slightly shaded and sheltered from the impacts of harsh weather conditions.

This landscaped area is managed by University of Maryland Facilities Management and is naturally filled with acorns from oak trees above. The test subject, the Eastern gray squirrel, is a small, native rodent in high abundance on the University of Maryland campus. The gray squirrel will den inside of trees or build a large nest from leaves in the canopy (Smithsonian Museum of Natural History). Because of the squirrel s high affinity for trees, the test location included oak trees to encompass a squirrel's natural habitat. Eastern gray squirrels are crepuscular and most active after dawn, and before dusk. Experiment -- We established a single fifteen by fifteen foot plot to apply each of our three treatments and observe the foraging behavior of gray squirrels. Orange flags were placed at the four corners of our plot to clearly identify boundaries for the video review. Saltine crackers with peanut butter were scattered randomly around the center of the plot to draw squirrels into our test area (Figure 1). Experiments were conducted for five mornings with similar, clear weather conditions at 7:00 am. Each treatment was applied for 30 minutes, with 10 minutes between each treatment to prevent humans from affecting the test results. The order of treatments was altered each day to account for differences in peak activity time. All behavior of the gray squirrels was video recorded to be analyzed at a later period. The first treatment tested the effect of an identified interspecific competitor. A stuffed squirrel, roughly the same size as an eastern gray squirrel, was placed in the center of the plot to represent a competitor species. The second treatment tested the effect of a random object on squirrel foraging behavior. A black camera bag, also approximately the same size as an eastern gray squirrel, was placed at the center of the

plot. This treatment was applied to see if grey squirrels were reacting to the stuffed squirrel as a competitor or simply as a foreign object. Lastly, the third treatment was the control; we observed foraging behavior of gray squirrels in the absence of both the camera bag and identified interspecific competitor. Video Observation -- Each trial, and treatment, was recorded using a digital camera. At a later time, individuals observed the videos to make note of foraging behavior for each gray squirrel visiting the experimental plot. The video for each trial was viewed by two individuals, in separate locations. For every video observed, each individual made note of the treatment applied, the time each squirrel was in the plot, and the time spent with their heads down foraging. For this experiment, foraging behavior recorded consisted of a squirrel with its head facing the ground and front limbs searching through the terrain. The time spent foraging was for each squirrel was then averaged between the values obtained by each person viewing the videos. Averaging these values accounted for inter-observer variation that could occur and possibly skew results. The average foraging value for each squirrel was then used to observe patterns in squirrel behavior. Statistical Analysis -- Analysis was performed using a one-way ANOVA to determine if the presence of a competitor or non-competitor object significantly affected the proportion of time spent foraging when compared to an empty plot. Using ANOVA allows for multiple samples to be drawn from the same population, and for even the smallest amount of variability between samples to be detected. The average foraging time for each gray squirrel was used as one data point. Each treatment applied was counted as

a random and independent sample; and, each sample was assumed to be normally distributed. An alpha value of 0.05 was used in ANOVA analysis. RESULTS A total of 33 squirrels were observed, divided as such: 14 squirrels analyzed for the plot containing no object, 11 squirrels in the plot containing an interspecific competitor, and 8 squirrels in the presence of a non-competitor object (in this case a camera bag). The mean proportion of time spent foraging was 0.5478 in the presence of the camera bag, 0.5022 with the competitor, and 0.3383 in the empty plot. The results of the average proportion of time spent foraging in the plot show the squirrels foraged most in the presence of the non-competitor object and least in the plot containing no object (Figure 2). To determine if these results were statistically significant a one-way ANOVA was performed (Table 1). This resulted in the inability to reject the null hypothesis (F 2,30 = 1.11861794, p > 0.05), indicating there is no significant correlation between the proportion of time spent foraging and the presence of the competitor. DISCUSSION The results from this study indicate that Eastern gray squirrels do not significantly alter their foraging habits in the presence of the stuffed squirrel competitor. It was predicted that the competitor would disrupt squirrel foraging activity, possibly by lessening the amount of time squirrels spent in plots when compared to the control and empty trials. In fact, though not statistically significant, plots with a foreign object, either the competitor or the bag, showed a higher rate of foraging activity than those without.

This potentially demonstrates a lack of perceived interspecific competition and potential attraction to sites with evidence of human impact, which could be related to the level of urbanization and habituation. In urban and suburban habitats, gray squirrels do exhibit decreased wariness and increased intraspecific competition, leading to higher levels of aggression and territoriality (Parker and Nilon 2008). The tendency towards more overt aggression could explain the squirrels increased frequency in plots with a competitor. The stuffed squirrel used in this study was intended as a substitute for the European red squirrel due to the competition between the two species in regions of gray squirrel invasion in Europe (Gurnell et al. 2004). However, it is possible that the gray squirrels in the area of the University of Maryland study site, unfamiliar with the presence of another species overlapping in niche, perceived the competing squirrel as gray and a source of intraspecific range overlap. An intraspecific threat in an urban area explains the increase in foraging around the competitor. Urbanization could also rationalize the increased foraging activity around the control, in this case a foreign and human object. Urban squirrels are able to use human food as a provisioning source and can increase in abundance as a result of decreased resource pressure (Hadidian et al. 1988). Human presence becomes associated with food supply and an obviously human object might spark interest in squirrels habituated to recognize that connection. In this study, the use of the camera bag could register with urban squirrels as a non-threatening human item, encouraging their approach with the possibility of food being involved.

The results of our study, though non-statistically significant, do correspond with the findings of previous studies on urban gray squirrel behavior. We were limited in our number of trials by time constraints and inconsistent squirrel presence, but with greater numbers and more consistent sampling, data might begin to show more concrete trends. The potential of the human object as a foraging encouragement device should be studied in more depth, not only as an indicator of squirrel behavior, but also as a sign of urbanization s effects on all local wildlife. Human presence drastically alters the environment--buildings, vegetation, and food provisioning follow a strong rural to urban gradient, directly and indirectly impacting all area wildlife species (McCleery et al. 2007). In order to study the effect of inter or intraspecific competition between squirrels as an isolate, future studies will have to be done eliminating the effects due to human impact. However, gray squirrels, in their small size and abundance, also represent a useful indicator species to document trends in human influence along the progression from rural to urban. Research on their foraging behavior in relation to anthropogenic development hold implications for best management practices surrounding human-wildlife interaction in the greater urban context.

LITERATURE CITED McCleery, R. A., R.R. Lopez, N.J. Silvy, and S.N. Kahlick. 2007. Habitat use of fox squirrels in an urban environment. The Journal of Wildlife Management 71:1149-1157. Grover, J. P. 1997. Resource Competition. Chapman and Hall. London, United Kingdom. Tilman, D. 1982. Resource competition and community structure. Princeton University Press. Princeton, New Jersey. Gurnell, J., L. A. Wauters, P. W. Lurz, and G. Tosi. 2004. Alien species and interspecific competition: effects of introduced eastern grey squirrels on red squirrel population dynamics. Journal of Animal Ecology 73: 26-35. Hadidian, J., D. Manski, V. Flyger, C. Cox, and G. Hodge. 1987. Urban gray squirrel damage and population management: a case history. Third Eastern Wildlife Control Conference 19. Parker, T.S., and C.H. Nilon. 2008. Gray squirrel density, habitat suitability, and behavior in urban parks. Urban Ecosystems 11: 243-255. Peterson, A. T. 2003.. Predicting the geography of species invasions via ecological niche modeling. The quarterly review of biology, 78: 419-433. Schoener, T. W. 1983. Field experiments on interspecific competition. American naturalist, 99: 240-285. Smithsonian Museum of Natural History. Sciurus carolinensis: Eastern Gray Squirrel. North American Mammals. Ward, J. F., R. M. Austin, and D.W. Macdonald. 2000. A simulation model of foraging behaviour and the effect of predation risk. Journal of Animal Ecology, 69: 16-30.

TABLES AND FIGURES TABLE 1 Source of Variation SS df MS F F crit Between Groups 0.2799937053 2 0.139996853 1.1186179 3.32 Within Groups 3.754548742 30 0.125151625 4 Total 4.034542447 32 Table 1. Significant values for ANOVA analysis

FIGURE 1 Figure 1. Plot design to test the foraging habits of gray squirrels.

FIGURE 2 Figure 2. Average proportion of time spent foraging at the experiment site based on object present in the field. (Bag: n=8, Competitor n=11, Control n=14).