NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Cogn Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 June 23.

Size: px
Start display at page:

Download "NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Cogn Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 June 23."

Transcription

1 NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Published in final edited form as: J Cogn Neurosci January ; 22(1): doi: /jocn Perception of Face Parts and Face Configurations: An fmri Study Jia Liu 1, Alison Harris 2, and Nancy Kanwisher 3 1 Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China 2 Hospital of the University of Pennsylvania 3 Massachusetts Institute of Technology Abstract fmri studies have reported three regions in human ventral visual cortex that respond selectively to faces: the occipital face area (OFA), the fusiform face area (FFA), and a face-selective region in the superior temporal sulcus (fsts). Here, we asked whether these areas respond to two first-order aspects of the face argued to be important for face perception, face parts (eyes, nose, and mouth), and the T-shaped spatial configuration of these parts. Specifically, we measured the magnitude of response in these areas to stimuli that (i) either contained real face parts, or did not, and (ii) either had veridical face configurations, or did not. The OFA and the fsts were sensitive only to the presence of real face parts, not to the correct configuration of those parts, whereas the FFA was sensitive to both face parts and face configuration. Further, only in the FFA is the response to configuration and part information correlated across voxels, suggesting that the FFA contains a unified representation that includes both kinds of information. In combination with prior results from fmri, TMS, MEG, and patient studies, our data illuminate the functional division of labor in the OFA, FFA, and fsts. INTRODUCTION Prior work with fmri has identified three face-selective regions in occipito-temporal cortex: the fusiform face area (FFA) (1997; McCarthy, Puce, Gore, & Allison, 1997), found in the mid-fusiform gyrus; the occipital face area (OFA) (Gauthier et al., 2000), found in the lateral inferior occipital gyri; and a face-selective region in the posterior part of the superior temporal sulcus that we will refer to here as the fsts (Allison, Puce, & McCarthy, 2000; Hoffman & Haxby, 2000). Despite considerable research, the precise role of each of these regions in face perception remains unclear. Here, we approached this question by asking what aspects of the face stimulus each of these regions is sensitive to: the configuration of the face (i.e., the T- shaped configuration of eyes above nose above mouth), and/or the presence of individual face parts (i.e., eyes, nose, and mouth). Several prior studies suggest a functional division of labor among the three face-selective regions, with each apparently involved in a different aspect of face perception. Specifically, several studies have supported the hypothesis (Calder & Young, 2005; Haxby, Hoffman, & Gobbini, 2000) that the OFA and the FFA are more involved in recognition of individual identity, whereas the fsts is more involved in recognition of social information in faces. For example, the FFA and the OFA, but not the fsts, are correlated trial-by-trial with successful Reprint requests should be sent to Jia Liu, Room 405, Yingdong Building, 19 Xinjiekouwai St., Haidian District, , Beijing, China, or via liujia@bnu.edu.cn. UNCITED REFERENCE Kanwisher, McDermott, & Chun, 1997

2 Liu et al. Page 2 detection and identification of faces (Grill-Spector, Knouf, & Kanwisher, 2004), whereas attention to eye-gaze direction of faces (vs. identity of the same faces) increases the fmri response of the fsts but not the FFA or the OFA (Hoffman & Haxby, 2000; see also Winston, Henson, Fine-Goulden, & Dolan, 2004). Further evidence showing that the FFA and the OFA are necessary for determining face identity comes from the fact that the critical lesion site for apperceptive prosopagnosia is in the region of the FFA (Riddoch, Johnston, Bracewell, Boutsen, & Humphreys, 2008; Barton, Press, Keenan, & O Connor, 2002; Wada & Yamamoto, 2001) and/or OFA (Steeves et al., 2006; Rossion et al., 2003) as defined by anatomical coordinates. Almost all prior work on these regions has investigated the effect of task or repetition on second-order aspects of face stimuli, that is, how one individual face differs from another. Here, we ask the more basic first-order question of how the response of each region is affected by the mere presence (vs. absence) of basic properties of face stimuli: face parts (eyes, nose, and mouth) and the basic T-shaped face configuration of those parts. Given that face processing is, in large part, automatic (Vuilleumier, 2000), the stimulus manipulations used here may be stronger than the relatively weak (Hoffman & Haxby, 2000) or nonexistent (Yovel & Kanwisher, 2004) effects of task manipulations on face-selective regions tested in prior work. Any differences between face-selective regions in the aspects of face stimuli they respond to should provide clues about the function of those regions. For example, prior evidence linking the FFA to face identification suggests that this region must be sensitive to both face parts and face configurations, as both are relevant to face identification. Consistent with this hypothesis, Yovel and Kanwisher (2004) found strong activation in the FFA when subjects discriminated between faces either on the basis of part appearance or part spacing. On the other hand, the higher response in the fsts during an eye-gaze direction discrimination task than an identity discrimination task (Hoffman & Haxby, 2000) and the recent report of a patient with a circumscribed STS lesion with a deficit in gaze perception (Akiyama et al., 2006) suggest that this region may be more sensitive to face parts (at least eyes). Although the functional profile of the OFA is harder to predict, several lines of evidence suggest that it may be more selective for face parts (Pitcher, Walsh, Yovel, & Duchaine, 2007; McCarthy, Puce, Belger, & Allison, 1999), and for earlier representations closer to the physical properties of faces (Rotshtein, Henson, Treves, Driver, & Dolan, 2005; Tanskanen, Nasanen, Montez, Paallysaho, & Hari, 2005). Here, we measured the response of the OFA, FFA and fsts to the first-order properties of face stimuli by decomposing face stimuli into two relatively independent and intuitively natural components: face configuration and face parts. Specifically, we measured fmri responses to face stimuli in which we orthogonally varied whether the images contained: (a) real face parts versus solid black ovals in the corresponding locations; and (b) veridical face configurations versus rearranged nonface configurations (Figure 1). Because external contours (a roughly oval shape with hair on the top and sides) may interact with the processing of internal face features (Sinha & Poggio, 1996), we varied part and configuration information both in the context of whole faces (including external contours) and in versions of the same stimuli from which external contours were removed, leaving only a bounding rectangle around the central face region. During the scan, subjects were instructed to passively view the images to minimize biases toward specific aspects of the face stimuli that may arise in the context of a given face task. Based on previous reports, we predicted that the response of the FFA would depend strongly on the presence of both a veridical face configuration and veridical face parts, whereas the responses of the OFA and the fsts would depend more strongly on the presence of real face parts in the stimuli.

3 Liu et al. Page 3 METHODS Subjects Stimuli Experimental Procedure Nine subjects (age 18 45; 7 men) participated in the study, which was conducted at the Athinoula A. Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging at the Massachusetts General Hospital, Charlestown, MA. All subjects were right-handed, and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. The fmri protocol was approved by both MIT COUHES and by the Partner s IRB. Informed consent was obtained from all subjects before participating. Eight stimulus categories (Figure 1) were constructed from veridical face photographs by orthogonally eliminating versus preserving face parts (i.e., eyes, nose, mouth), face configurations, and external contours (i.e., hairline, chin, ears). To vary whether information about face parts and face configuration was present, face parts were either intact, or were replaced by black ovals in their corresponding locations, and face configurations were either left intact, or were rearranged into novel nonface configurations. The size of the ovals was approximately matched to the actual size of corresponding face parts in each face stimulus, and the arrangements of nonface configurations varied across exemplars. The effect of external contours was eliminated by cropping stimuli to show the central face region only. In addition, two other stimulus categories were included for comparison: photographs of houses, and images containing only external contours. There were 50 exemplars from each stimulus category, and each exemplar occurred twice in the whole session (i.e., 100 trials per category). Each subject participated in a single session consisting of (1) two blocked-design functional localizer scans, and (2) 10 event-related experimental scans. The localizer scan lasted 5 min and 15 sec and consisted of sixteen 15-sec epochs with fixation periods interleaved. During each epoch, 20 different photographs of a given stimulus category (frontal-view human faces or familiar objects) were shown. Each photograph was presented for 300 msec followed by a blank interval of 450 msec. The experimental scan contained a total of 176 trials, with 10 experimental trials for each of the 10 conditions (8 stimulus manipulations plus house and external-feature-only conditions) plus 76 fixation trials. A new trial began every 1.5 sec. In each trial, an image from the relevant condition (subtending about 6.2 by 6.2 visual angle) was presented at the center of gaze for 300 msec followed by a fixation-only interval of 1.2 sec. The order of conditions was counter-balanced using the optseq2 program ( so that trials from each condition, including the fixation condition (i.e., temporal jitter), were preceded, on average, equally often by trials from each of the other conditions. Subjects pressed a button whenever they saw two identical pictures in a row (1-back task) in the localizer scan, and in the experimental scan they fixated on a black dot continuously present in the center, and passively viewed stimuli displayed in a pseudorandom order. Scanning Procedures and Data Analysis Scanning was done on a 3-T Siemens Trio scanner, using a custom eight-channel phased-array surface coil (built by Dr. Lawrence Wald), which provided a relatively high spatial resolution and signal-to-noise ratio in posterior brain regions. Fifteen 2-mm-thick (20% skip) near-axial slices were collected (in-plane resolution = mm), oriented parallel to each subject s temporal cortex to cover the inferior portion of the occipital lobes as well as the posterior portion of the temporal lobes, including the OFA, the FFA and part of the STS. T2*-weighted, gradient-echo, echo-planar imaging procedures were used (TR = 3 sec, TE = 32 msec, flip

4 Liu et al. Page 4 RESULTS angle = 90 ). Motion correction, intensity normalization, and spatial smoothing (FWHM = 3 mm) were performed prior to signal averaging (FreeSurfer functional analysis stream, Cortechs, Charlestown, MA). Face-selective regions of interest (ROIs) were identified separately for each subject and hemisphere from the localizer scan. Specifically, the FFA was defined as the set of contiguous voxels in the mid-fusiform gyrus that showed significantly higher responses to front-view human faces compared to familiar objects (p < 10 4, uncorrected). The OFA and the fsts were defined in the same way but localized in inferior occipital cortex and the STS, respectively (Figure 2). Finally, a non-face-selective region, the lateral occipital (LO), was also localized by the contrast of objects versus faces. This region was designed to serve as a control for the possible con-founding role of attention because stimuli with black ovals may not attract as much attention as those with face parts. For the ROI analysis, percent signal change data were extracted and averaged by condition across all 10 runs and all voxels within each subject s predefined ROIs. Because the fmri response typically lags 4 to 6 sec after the neural response, the magnitude of the ROI activity was measured as the average percentage change in MR signal at the latency of 6 and 9 sec (TR = 3) compared to a fixation as a baseline. In addition to the traditional ROI-based analysis, we also employed multivoxel pattern analysis (O Toole et al., 2007; Haynes & Rees, 2006; Norman, Polyn, Detre, & Haxby, 2006) to examine whether face configurations and face parts were represented by the same or different sets of neuronal populations in the previously defined ROIs. Rather than pooling the responses across voxels, we measured the sensitivity of each voxel to configural effect (indexed by the t value for that voxel comparing conditions with vs. without face configurations) and featural effect (indexed by the t value for that voxel comparing conditions with vs. without parts). For this analysis, the data were first re-preprocessed without spatial smoothing so as to maximize the sensitivity to any information present in the spatial pattern of response. Finally, the correlation across voxels was then calculated between these two sets of t values to measure the similarity of the spatial activation patterns of these two effects. The FFA and the OFA were successfully localized in both hemispheres of all subjects. Analysis of the data from the experimental scan showed no difference in the pattern of response between the right and left hemispheres for each of the ROIs (ps >.1). Therefore, analysis of these two regions was based on a pooled analysis in which right- and left-hemisphere voxels were combined in each ROI. The right fsts was localized in all nine subjects, whereas the left fsts was found in only four subjects. Therefore, the analysis of the fsts was carried out in the right fsts only. Having identified the ROIs in each subject, we then calculated the magnitude of the BOLD response in each of these ROIs in each of the conditions of the main experiments. These data were used to examine what stimulus information each of the three ROIs is sensitive to. The magnitude of the response of each ROI to each stimulus condition (Supplementary Figure 1) was analyzed in a four-way ANOVA, where the factors were face-selective cortical region (OFA vs. FFA vs. fsts), face parts (real vs. black ovals), face configurations (veridical vs. scrambled), and external contours (external contours vs. square cutouts). This ANOVA found significant main effects of cortical region [F(2, 8) = 28.0, p <.001], face parts [F(1, 8) = 180.2, p <.001], and external contours [F(1, 8) = 53.6, p <.001]. The significant interactions of cortical region by face configuration [F(2, 7) = 12.7, p <.005], by face parts [F(2, 7) = 28.0, p <.001], and by external contours [F(2, 7) = 21.8, p <.001] indicated that the amount of information about each of these dimensions differs across ROIs. No other main effects, or twoway, three-way, or four-way interactions reached significance (all ps >.1). In addition, these

5 Liu et al. Page 5 predefined ROIs also showed a significantly higher response to faces than nonfaces (i.e., houses) in the experimental scans (ps <.01), indicating that the localizer-defined face-selective regions were reliably face selective. The significant main effect of face parts was also found individually in each region, indicating that all three face-selective regions are sensitive to face parts. Specifically, FFA [F(1, 8) = 426.7, p <.001], OFA [F(1, 8) = 89.0, p <.001], and fsts [F(1, 8) = 31.1, p <.001] responses were each independently significantly higher when real face parts rather than black ovals were present (Figure 3, left). This featural effect was observed independent of whether the face configuration was present [FFA: F(1, 8) = 95.7, p <.001; OFA: F(1, 8) = 22.0, p <.002; fsts: F(1, 8) = 23.1, p <.001] or absent [FFA: F(1, 8) = 55.7, p <.001; OFA: F(1, 8) = 54.8, p <. 001; fsts: F(1, 8) = 9.1, p <.02]. Nonetheless, the sensitivity to face parts varies across faceselective regions as the featural effect was significantly higher in both the FFA and the OFA than that in the fsts [FFA vs. fsts: F(1, 8) = 45.6, p <.001; OFA vs. fsts: F(1, 8) = 22.3, p <.002; FFA vs. OFA: F < 1]. Finally, the higher response to parts is unlikely to reflect an overall attentional bias favoring stimuli with parts because the higher response to parts was not found in the non-face-selective region LO (F < 1). Much like the responses to face parts, the FFA [F(1, 8) = 37.4, p <.001], OFA [F(1, 8) = 70.8, p <.001], and fsts (F(1, 8) = 23.5, p <.001) also showed a greater response to the stimuli with external contours compared to stimuli from which the external contours were cropped (Supplementary Figure 2). In addition, the sensitivity to external contours was the highest in the FFA [FFA vs. OFA: F(1, 8) = 6.3, p <.05], and the lowest in the fsts [OFA vs. fsts: F (1, 8) = 15.4, p <.002]. However, only the FFA, not the OFA or the fsts, is sensitive to face configurations. This was revealed by the previously mentioned significant interaction of cortical regions by face configuration, along with a significant configural effect for the FFA [F(1, 8) = 11.8, p <.01], but not for the OFA or the fsts (both Fs < 1) (Figure 3, right). This interaction of cortical region by face configuration was observed independent of whether the face parts were present [FFA vs. OFA: F(1, 8) = 17.3, p <.001] or absent [FFA vs. OFA: F(1, 8) = 12.0, p <.005]. In addition, no significant interaction of face configurations by face parts in the FFA was observed [F(1, 8) < 1], consistent with the fact that the FFA showed a significantly larger response to stimuli with face configurations regardless of whether face parts were present [F(1, 8) = 5.7, p <.05] or absent [F(1, 8) = 11.3, p <.01]. Are face parts and face configurations represented by a single neural population in the FFA, or does the FFA contain distinct neural populations, one responsive only to face parts and another responsive only to face configurations? To test these two alternatives, we used multivoxel pattern analysis to compare the similarity between the spatial activation pattern of face configurations and that of face parts. Specifically, a t value was extracted for the configural effect (stimuli with face configurations stimuli without) and the featural effect (stimuli with face parts stimuli without) for each voxel within the FFA in each subject, and then a correlation between these two sets of t values was calculated, separately for each subject. The correlation score of the right FFA s spatial activation patterns of two effects from a representative subject is shown in Figure 4A (r =.29, p <.001). To check further the distribution of r values against chance, we used a permutation test that randomly shuffled the pairings between the configural and featural effect for each voxel 1000 times and computed a correlation coefficient each time. This gave us random distributions of r values for each ROI, which enabled us to measure the difference between the observed r value and the mean of the shuffled r values in Z-score units. The Z score of the r value for this subject shown in Figure 4B was 7.8, which was significantly different from the mean value (r = 0) of the correlation coefficients from the permutation test (p <.001). Eight out of nine subjects showed significant Z scores (evaluated at p <.02) in the

6 Liu et al. Page 6 DISCUSSION FFA, which was significantly greater than chance (χ 2 = 5.4, p <.02), suggesting that the FFA not only is sensitive to both face configurations and face parts, but also is engaged in integrating them into a single holistic representation. Similar correlations between configural and featural effects among voxels were not observed in the OFA and the fsts (ps >.3). In this study, we measured the extent to which two first-order aspects of face stimuli, the presence of face parts and the configuration of those parts, drive three face-selective regions in occipito-temporal cortex, namely, the OFA, the FFA, and the fsts. We find that the OFA and the fsts are sensitive to the presence of face parts in the stimulus but not to the presence of a veridical face configuration, whereas the FFA is sensitive to both kinds of information. Further, only in the FFA is the response to configuration and part information correlated across voxels, implying that the FFA contains a unified representation that includes both kinds of information. These results dovetail interestingly with a number of recent findings that collectively help specify the functional division of labor and connectivity between the different face-selective regions in the human ventral visual pathway. Our results fit most strikingly with those of Pitcher et al. (2007), who applied TMS to the right OFA and selectively disrupted subjects ability to discriminate faces on the basis of differences in face parts, but not differences in the spacing among those parts. Importantly, that study found that this disruption occurred at a relatively early latency, only when TMS was applied 60 and 100 msec after stimulus onset, not later. Although this study manipulated the second-order property of face parts (i.e., the shape of face parts), whereas we measured the first-order property (i.e., the presence vs. absence of face parts), it clearly reinforces our finding that the OFA is involved in the analysis of face parts but not face configurations, and further shows that the OFA is not only activated by but necessary for the analysis of face parts (see also Rossion et al., 2003), and that it conducts a relatively early stage of face processing. The idea that the OFA conducts an earlier stage of face processing than the FFA is consistent with its location posterior to the FFA as well as the fact that its response is more biased to the contralateral hemifield than that of the FFA (Hemond, Kanwisher, & Op de Beeck, 2007). This idea is further consistent with the hypothesis that the OFA is the source of the face-selective M100 response, whereas the FFA is the source of the face-selective M170 response (Liu, Harris, & Kanwisher, 2002; Halgren, Raij, Marinkovic, Jousmaki, & Hari, 2000; but see McCarthy et al., 1999; Bentin, Allison, Puce, Perez, & McCarthy, 1996 for evidence that the FFA may not be the source of the N170 measured with ERPs). That hypothesis, if correct, provides another link to the present data because an MEG experiment parallel to that described here (Liu et al., 2002) found that the face-selective M100 response is sensitive only to face parts, whereas the M170 is sensitive to both face parts and face configurations. In contrast to the OFA, the FFA responded both to the presence of face parts and to their veridical configuration. This finding is generally consistent with previous studies that have shown a link between the neural activity of the FFA and behavioral signatures of the holistic processing of faces, including the face-inversion effect (Mazard, Schiltz, & Rossion, 2006; Yovel & Kanwisher, 2005; Gauthier, Tarr, Anderson, Skudlarski, & Gore, 1999; Haxby et al., 1999; Kanwisher, Tong, & Nakayama, 1998; but see Aguirre, Singh, & D Esposito, 1999), the composite effect (Schiltz & Rossion, 2006), and the discrimination of the fine distance among face parts (Rotshtein, Geng, Driver, & Dolan, 2007; Yovel & Kanwisher, 2004; Barton et al., 2002; but see Maurer et al., 2007). However, our findings of a similar response to face configurations and face parts in the right and left FFAs do not fit straightforwardly with the findings of a previous study (Rossion et al., 2000), which found a higher response in the region of the right FFA when subjects attended to a whole face relative to a face part and the opposite

7 Liu et al. Page 7 pattern in the region of the left FFA. Although the reason for this discrepancy is not clear, the paradigms are quite different, with Rossion et al. (2000) relying on an attention/task manipulation, and our study relying on a stimulus manipulation. Other studies have found both FFAs to be involved in processing configural information of faces, with the right FFA showing either no higher engagement (Yovel & Kanwisher, 2005; Haxby et al., 1999) or only a slight preference for configural processing (Mazard et al., 2006; Schiltz & Rossion, 2006). Thus, considerable converging evidence suggests that the OFA conducts an early analysis of faces that is necessary for the perception of face parts, and the FFA conducts a later analysis of both the parts and configurations of faces (see also Calder & Young, 2005; Haxby et al., 2000). An obvious extension of the above conclusion is the speculation that the FFA and the OFA comprise a hierarchical network for face perception with the FFA inheriting the part sensitivity of the OFA, and then further integrating or elaborating this information to include sensitivity to the spatial configuration of these parts. This hierarchical representation of faces seems to reflect a widely held view, according to which global properties of an object are represented by the activity of neurons that receive convergent input from populations of neurons that encode relatively simple and local features of the object at lower levels in the hierarchy (Lerner, Hendler, Ben-Bashat, Harel, & Malach, 2001; Tanaka, 1996; Barlow, 1972). Consistent with this hypothesis, high spatial frequencies have been found to be critical for identifying face parts, whereas low spatial frequencies are more important for discriminating spacing differences between those parts (Goffaux, Hault, Michel, Vuong, & Rossion, 2005), suggesting that the OFA and the FFA may process faces at difference scales, with local properties of face extracted in the OFA and then more global properties in the FFA for further global processing (see also Tanskanen et al., 2005). Although the hierarchical hypothesis is plausible, several considerations suggest that the OFA is not the only input to the FFA. First, given that the OFA is responsive only to face parts and not to face-like configurations of ovals, it cannot be the only input to the FFA, which responded to face-like configurations of ovals in the present study. That is, the representation of face parts in the OFA is not a necessary intermediate step for the representation of face configurations in the FFA. Second, both intact discrimination of face spacing (Pitcher et al., 2007) and activation of the FFA (Steeves et al., 2006; Rossion et al., 2003) can occur when the OFA is disrupted. Thus, although the OFA likely sends input to the FFA, the FFA likely receives input from other areas as well (see also Barbeau et al., 2008; Allison, Puce, Spencer, & McCarthy, 1999; Halgren et al., 1994). Our further finding that the response to face parts and face configurations is correlated across voxels in the FFA is consistent with several other lines of evidence for an integrated, holistic representation that contains both kinds of information. First, Yovel and Kanwisher (in press) have found high correlations across subjects in performance discriminating face parts and face spacing, but only for upright faces, consistent with the idea that a common mechanism is engaged in the analysis of both kinds of information. Second, although Schiltz and Rossion (2006) found evidence for holistic representations in both the OFA and the FFA using composite face stimuli, this effect was largest for the FFA. Third, recent work manipulated binocular disparity of stimuli so that faces were perceived either as wholes or parts and again found that, although both the OFA and the FFA have both holistic and part-based representations, only in the FFA are such representations modulated by familiarity (Harris & Aguirre, 2008). These lines of evidence indicate that information about different aspects of the face (parts and configuration) is ultimately integrated into a single holistic representation of the face, and this holistic representation may reside in the FFA (see also Rotshtein et al., 2007).

8 Liu et al. Page 8 Much like the OFA, the fsts was sensitive only to face parts, not face configurations. This finding is generally consistent with prior findings implicating this region in the discrimination of gaze direction and expression. However, the OFA and the fsts may extract different aspects of information from face parts. For example, a recent fmri study reports that the fsts was activated by the directional information from eye gaze but not from the physical properties of the eyes, whereas the OFA showed the opposite pattern (Materna, Dicke, & Thier, 2008). On the other hand, even gaze and expression perception are presumably clearer in faces with veridical configurations than faces with parts rearranged, which seems inconsistent with our finding that the fsts was sensitive only to face parts. One possibility is that the STS regions previously implicated in the perception of gaze and expression may not be identical to the region tested here (see also Winston et al., 2004). Finally, our finding that all three face-selective regions are sensitive to the external contours of faces suggests that this aspect of faces is also used for constructing the representation of faces at different stages of face processing. Indeed, when fine-grained details of the internal face features are missing, the coarse information of external features may help to detect faces among objects (see also Cox, Meyers, & Sinha, 2004). Further, a change of the external contours can actually alter our identification of faces even when the internal features remain constantly (Sinha & Poggio, 1996; Haig, 1986). Alternatively, the sensitivity to the external contours of the face-selective regions may reflect the remnant of its critical role in recognizing faces for infants and children which becomes less important with age compared to the internal features (Want, Pascalis, Coleman, & Blades, 2003; Campbell & Tuck, 1995; Campbell, Walker, & Baron-Cohen, 1995). Further investigations are necessary to reveal the functional role of the external contours of faces in these three face-selective regions. In sum, the current data dovetail with prior results from fmri, TMS, MEG, and patient studies to indicate that the OFA is selectively involved in perceptual processing of face parts, whereas the FFA processes both the parts and configurations of faces. Although a feedforward model from the OFA to the FFA and the fsts is likely to be part of the story (e.g., Rotshtein et al., 2007), several mysteries remain for future investigation. First, how can activation of the FFA and processing of face spacing can be preserved when the OFA is disrupted? Second, what is the role of the anterior temporal lobe in face perception (Behrmann, Avidan, Gao, & Black, 2007; Kriegeskorte, Formisano, Sorger, & Goebel, 2007)? Third, by what route does face information reach the fsts? These and other mysteries will require better methods for tracking the responses of each of these regions over time to discover how exactly information flows in this face processing network. Supplementary Material Acknowledgments References Refer to Web version on PubMed Central for supplementary material. We thank K. Nakayama, P. Sinha, J. DiCarlo, W. Freiwald, and G. Yovel for helpful discussions and comments. This study is supported by the National Institutes of Health (66696 to N. K.), National Eye Institute (EY13455 to N. K.), National Natural Science Foundation of China ( to J. L.), and National Key Basic Research Development Program of China (2007CB to J. L.). Aguirre GK, Singh R, D Esposito M. Stimulus inversion and the responses of face and object-sensitive cortical areas. NeuroReport 1999;10: [PubMed: ]

9 Liu et al. Page 9 Akiyama T, Kato M, Muramatsu T, Saito F, Nakachi R, Kashima H. A deficit in discriminating gaze direction in a case with right superior temporal gyrus lesion. Neuropsychologia 2006;44: [PubMed: ] Allison T, Puce A, McCarthy G. Social perception from visual cues: Role of the STS region. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2000;4: [PubMed: ] Allison T, Puce A, Spencer DD, McCarthy G. Electrophysiological studies of human face perception: I. Potentials generated in occipitotemporal cortex by face and non-face stimuli. Cerebral Cortex 1999;9: [PubMed: ] Barbeau EJ, Taylor MJ, Regis J, Marquis P, Chauvel P, Liegeois-Chauvel C. Spatio temporal dynamics of face recognition. Cerebral Cortex 2008;18: [PubMed: ] Barlow HB. Single units and sensation: A neuron doctrine for perceptual psychology? Perception 1972;1: [PubMed: ] Barton JJ, Press DZ, Keenan JP, O Connor M. Lesions of the fusiform face area impair perception of facial configuration in prosopagnosia. Neurology 2002;58: [PubMed: ] Behrmann M, Avidan G, Gao F, Black S. Structural imaging reveals anatomical alterations in inferotemporal cortex in congenital prosopagnosia. Cerebral Cortex 2007;17: [PubMed: ] Bentin S, Allison T, Puce A, Perez E, McCarthy G. Electrophysiological studies of face perception in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 1996;8: Calder AJ, Young AW. Understanding the recognition of facial identity and facial expression. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2005;6: Campbell R, Tuck M. Recognition of parts of famous-face photographs by children: An experimental note. Perception 1995;24: [PubMed: ] Campbell R, Walker J, Baron-Cohen S. The development of differential use of inner and outer face features in familiar face identification. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 1995;59: Cox D, Meyers E, Sinha P. Contextually evoked object-specific responses in human visual cortex. Science 2004;304: [PubMed: ] Gauthier I, Tarr MJ, Anderson AW, Skudlarski P, Gore JC. Activation of the middle fusiform face area increases with expertise in recognizing novel objects. Nature Neuroscience 1999;2: Gauthier I, Tarr MJ, Moylan J, Skudlarski P, Gore JC, Anderson AW. The fusiform face area is part of a network that processes faces at the individual level. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2000;12: [PubMed: ] Goffaux V, Hault B, Michel C, Vuong QC, Rossion B. The respective role of low and high spatial frequencies in supporting configural and featural processing of faces. Perception 2005;34: [PubMed: ] Grill-Spector K, Knouf N, Kanwisher N. The fusiform face area subserves face perception, not generic within-category identification. Nature Neuroscience 2004;7: Haig ND. Exploring recognition with interchanged facial features. Perception 1986;15: [PubMed: ] Halgren E, Baudena P, Heit G, Clarke JM, Marinkovic K, Clarke M. Spatio-temporal stages in face and word processing: I. Depth-recorded potentials in the human occipital, temporal and parietal lobes [corrected]. Journal of Physiology Paris 1994;88:1 50. Halgren E, Raij T, Marinkovic K, Jousmaki V, Hari R. Cognitive response profile of the human fusiform face area as determined by MEG. Cerebral Cortex 2000;10: [PubMed: ] Harris A, Aguirre GK. The representation of parts and wholes in face-selective cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2008;20: [PubMed: ] Haxby JV, Hoffman EA, Gobbini MI. The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2000;4: [PubMed: ] Haxby JV, Ungerleider LG, Clark VP, Schouten JL, Hoffman EA, Martin A. The effect of face inversion on activity in human neural systems for face and object perception. Neuron 1999;22: [PubMed: ] Haynes JD, Rees G. Decoding mental states from brain activity in humans. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2006;7:

10 Liu et al. Page 10 Hemond CC, Kanwisher NG, Op de Beeck HP. A preference for contralateral stimuli in human objectand face-selective cortex. PLoS ONE 2007;2:e574. [PubMed: ] Hoffman EA, Haxby JV. Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in the distributed human neural system for face perception. Nature Neuroscience 2000;3: Kanwisher N, McDermott J, Chun MM. The fusiform face area: A module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. Journal of Neuroscience 1997;17: [PubMed: ] Kanwisher N, Tong F, Nakayama K. The effect of face inversion on the human fusiform face area. Cognition 1998;68:B1 B11. [PubMed: ] Kriegeskorte N, Formisano E, Sorger B, Goebel R. Individual faces elicit distinct response patterns in human anterior temporal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA 2007;104: Lerner Y, Hendler T, Ben-Bashat D, Harel M, Malach R. A hierarchical axis of object processing stages in the human visual cortex. Cerebral Cortex 2001;11: [PubMed: ] Liu J, Harris A, Kanwisher N. Stages of processing in face perception: An MEG study. Nature Neuroscience 2002;5: Materna S, Dicke PW, Thier P. Dissociable roles of the superior temporal sulcus and the intraparietal sulcus in joint attention: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2008;20: [PubMed: ] Maurer D, O Craven KM, Le Grand R, Mondloch CJ, Springer MV, Lewis TL, et al. Neural correlates of processing facial identity based on features versus their spacing. Neuropsychologia 2007;45: [PubMed: ] Mazard A, Schiltz C, Rossion B. Recovery from adaptation to facial identity is larger for upright than inverted faces in the human occipito-temporal cortex. Neuropsychologia 2006;44: [PubMed: ] McCarthy G, Puce A, Belger A, Allison T. Electrophysiological studies of human face perception: II. Response properties of face-specific potentials generated in occipitotemporal cortex. Cerebral Cortex 1999;9: [PubMed: ] McCarthy G, Puce A, Gore JC, Allison T. Face-specific processing in the human fusiform gyrus. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 1997;9: Norman KA, Polyn SM, Detre GJ, Haxby JV. Beyond mind-reading: Multi-voxel pattern analysis of fmri data. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2006;10: [PubMed: ] O Toole AJ, Jiang F, Abdi H, Penard N, Dunlop JP, Parent MA. Theoretical, statistical, and practical perspectives on pattern-based classification approaches to the analysis of functional neuroimaging data. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2007;19: [PubMed: ] Pitcher D, Walsh V, Yovel G, Duchaine B. TMS evidence for the involvement of the right occipital face area in early face processing. Current Biology 2007;17: [PubMed: ] Riddoch MJ, Johnston RA, Bracewell RM, Boutsen L, Humphreys GW. Are faces special? A case of pure prosopagnosia. Cognitive Neuropsychology 2008;25:3 26. [PubMed: ] Rossion B, Caldara R, Seghier M, Schuller AM, Lazeyras F, Mayer E. A network of occipito-temporal face-sensitive areas besides the right middle fusiform gyrus is necessary for normal face processing. Brain 2003;126: [PubMed: ] Rossion B, Dricot L, Devolder A, Bodart JM, Crommelinck M, De Gelder B, et al. Hemispheric asymmetries for whole-based and part-based face processing in the human fusiform gyrus. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2000;12: [PubMed: ] Rotshtein P, Geng JJ, Driver J, Dolan RJ. Role of features and second-order spatial relations in face discrimination, face recognition, and individual face skills: Behavioral and functional magnetic resonance imaging data. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2007;19: [PubMed: ] Rotshtein P, Henson RN, Treves A, Driver J, Dolan RJ. Morphing Marilyn into Maggie dissociates physical and identity face representations in the brain. Nature Neuroscience 2005;8: Schiltz C, Rossion B. Faces are represented holistically in the human occipito-temporal cortex. Neuroimage 2006;32: [PubMed: ] Sinha P, Poggio T. I think I know that face. Nature 1996;384:404. [PubMed: ]

11 Liu et al. Page 11 Steeves JK, Culham JC, Duchaine BC, Pratesi CC, Valyear KF, Schindler I, et al. The fusiform face area is not sufficient for face recognition: Evidence from a patient with dense prosopagnosia and no occipital face area. Neuropsychologia 2006;44: [PubMed: ] Tanaka K. Inferotemporal cortex and object vision. Annual Review of Neuroscience 1996;19: Tanskanen T, Nasanen R, Montez T, Paallysaho J, Hari R. Face recognition and cortical responses show similar sensitivity to noise spatial frequency. Cerebral Cortex 2005;15: [PubMed: ] Vuilleumier P. Faces call for attention: Evidence from patients with visual extinction. Neuropsychologia 2000;38: [PubMed: ] Wada Y, Yamamoto T. Selective impairment of facial recognition due to a haematoma restricted to the right fusiform and lateral occipital region. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 2001;71: Want SC, Pascalis O, Coleman M, Blades M. Recognizing people from the inner or outer parts of their faces: Developmental data concerning unfamiliar faces. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 2003;21: Winston JS, Henson RN, Fine-Goulden MR, Dolan RJ. fmri-adaptation reveals dissociable neural representations of identity and expression in face perception. Journal of Neurophysiology 2004;92: [PubMed: ] Yovel G, Kanwisher N. Face perception: Domain specific, not process specific. Neuron 2004;44: [PubMed: ] Yovel G, Kanwisher N. The neural basis of the behavioral face-inversion effect. Current Biology 2005;15: [PubMed: ] Yovel G, Kanwisher N. The representations of spacing and part-based information are associated for upright faces but dissociated for objects: Evidence from individual differences. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. (in press).

12 Liu et al. Page 12 Figure 1. Stimulus manipulations. The 2 2 design involved orthogonal manipulation of the presence versus absence of face parts (eyes, nose, and mouth; horizontal axis) and face configurations (the placement of these parts in a face arrangement vs. scrambled arrangement; vertical axis). External contours were either intact or removed.

13 Liu et al. Page 13 Figure 2. Face-selective regions, the OFA, FFA, and fsts, from an fmri localizer scan (p < 10 4, uncorrected) in the right hemisphere of a typical subject, shown on a flattened surface. Sulci are shown in dark gray and gyri in light gray. Talairach coordinates and statistics for each ROI from a group analysis (right hemisphere): OFA (BA 18), coordinates = 46, 78, 7, voxel number = 118, max t value = 4.6; FFA (BA 37), coordinates = 43, 53, 12, voxel number = 71, max t value = 3.9; fsts (BA 22), coordinates = 60, 51, 9, voxel number = 259, max t value = 6.3.

14 Liu et al. Page 14 Figure 3. Responses of the FFA, the OFA, and the fsts to face parts and configurations. Featural effect (left): The stimuli were pooled by the presence (black) versus absence (gray) of face parts. The hemodynamic responses of the face-selective regions were averaged across voxels, stimulus categories, and subjects. The y-axis indicates the percent signal change. The error bars show the standard error of the mean of the BOLD responses across subjects. Configural effect (right): The stimuli were pooled by whether stimuli contain veridical face configurations or not.

15 Liu et al. Page 15 Figure 4. The correlation of the configural and featural effect in the FFA from a typical subject. (A) The featural effect was significantly correlated with the configural effect indexed by t values across voxels in both the right (r =.29) and left (r =.24, not shown) FFA. (B) Distribution of correlation coefficients for randomly shuffled pairs of configural and part effect across voxels pooled from both the right and left FFA. The vertical dotted line indicates the mean value (r = 0) of the correlation coefficients from the permutation test, and the solid line indicates the observed correlation coefficient.

Supplementary Figure 1

Supplementary Figure 1 Supplementary Figure 1 Left aspl Right aspl Detailed description of the fmri activation during allocentric action observation in the aspl. Averaged activation (N=13) during observation of the allocentric

More information

Image-Invariant Responses in Face-Selective Regions Do Not Explain the Perceptual Advantage for Familiar Face Recognition

Image-Invariant Responses in Face-Selective Regions Do Not Explain the Perceptual Advantage for Familiar Face Recognition Cerebral Cortex February 2013;23:370 377 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhs024 Advance Access publication February 17, 2012 Image-Invariant Responses in Face-Selective Regions Do Not Explain the Perceptual Advantage

More information

Domain-Specificity versus Expertise in Face Processing

Domain-Specificity versus Expertise in Face Processing Domain-Specificity versus Expertise in Face Processing Dan O Shea and Peter Combs 18 Feb 2008 COS 598B Prof. Fei Fei Li Inferotemporal Cortex and Object Vision Keiji Tanaka Annual Review of Neuroscience,

More information

Orientation-sensitivity to facial features explains the Thatcher illusion

Orientation-sensitivity to facial features explains the Thatcher illusion Journal of Vision (2014) 14(12):9, 1 10 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/14/12/9 1 Orientation-sensitivity to facial features explains the Thatcher illusion Department of Psychology and York Neuroimaging

More information

The Representation of Parts and Wholes in Faceselective

The Representation of Parts and Wholes in Faceselective University of Pennsylvania ScholarlyCommons Cognitive Neuroscience Publications Center for Cognitive Neuroscience 5-2008 The Representation of Parts and Wholes in Faceselective Cortex Alison Harris University

More information

NeuroImage 56 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

NeuroImage 56 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage: NeuroImage 56 (2011) 2356 2363 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect NeuroImage journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg Differential selectivity for dynamic versus static information in face-selective

More information

Rapid Face-Selective Adaptation of an Early Extrastriate Component in MEG

Rapid Face-Selective Adaptation of an Early Extrastriate Component in MEG Cerebral Cortex January 2007;17:63--70 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj124 Advance Access publication January 25, 2006 Rapid Face-Selective Adaptation of an Early Extrastriate Component in MEG Alison Harris and

More information

Specialized Face Perception Mechanisms Extract Both Part and Spacing Information: Evidence from Developmental Prosopagnosia

Specialized Face Perception Mechanisms Extract Both Part and Spacing Information: Evidence from Developmental Prosopagnosia Specialized Face Perception Mechanisms Extract Both Part and Spacing Information: Evidence from Developmental Prosopagnosia Galit Yovel 1 and Brad Duchaine 2 Abstract & It is well established that faces

More information

Bodies are Represented as Wholes Rather Than Their Sum of Parts in the Occipital-Temporal Cortex

Bodies are Represented as Wholes Rather Than Their Sum of Parts in the Occipital-Temporal Cortex Cerebral Cortex February 2016;26:530 543 doi:10.1093/cercor/bhu205 Advance Access publication September 12, 2014 Bodies are Represented as Wholes Rather Than Their Sum of Parts in the Occipital-Temporal

More information

Fusiform Face Area in Chess Expertise

Fusiform Face Area in Chess Expertise Fusiform Face Area in Chess Expertise Merim Bilalić (merim.bilalic@med.uni-tuebingen.de) Department of Neuroradiology, Hoppe-Seyler Str. 2 Tübingen, 72076, Germany Abstract The ability to recognize faces

More information

A Revised Neural Framework for Face Processing

A Revised Neural Framework for Face Processing ANNUAL REVIEWS Further Click here to view this article's online features: Download figures as PPT slides Navigate linked references Download citations Explore related articles Search keywords A Revised

More information

Faces are represented holistically in the human occipito-temporal cortex

Faces are represented holistically in the human occipito-temporal cortex www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg NeuroImage 32 (2006) 1385 1394 Faces are represented holistically in the human occipito-temporal cortex Christine Schiltz a,b, and Bruno Rossion a,c a Laboratoire de Neurophysiologie,

More information

Distributed representation of objects in the human ventral visual pathway (face perception functional MRI object recognition)

Distributed representation of objects in the human ventral visual pathway (face perception functional MRI object recognition) Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 96, pp. 9379 9384, August 1999 Neurobiology Distributed representation of objects in the human ventral visual pathway (face perception functional MRI object recognition)

More information

Event-Related Potential and Functional MRI Measures of Face-Selectivity are Highly Correlated: A Simultaneous ERP-fMRI Investigation

Event-Related Potential and Functional MRI Measures of Face-Selectivity are Highly Correlated: A Simultaneous ERP-fMRI Investigation r Human Brain Mapping 000:000 000 (2010) r Event-Related Potential and Functional MRI Measures of Face-Selectivity are Highly Correlated: A Simultaneous ERP-fMRI Investigation Boaz Sadeh, 1 Ilana Podlipsky,

More information

Budapest University of Technology and Economics Department of Cognitive Science Psychology PhD School

Budapest University of Technology and Economics Department of Cognitive Science Psychology PhD School Budapest University of Technology and Economics Department of Cognitive Science Psychology PhD School Németh Kornél The possible subtypes of the developmental prosopagnosia in the light of the neuropsychological,

More information

Let s face it: It s a cortical network

Let s face it: It s a cortical network Target Article Let s face it: It s a cortical network www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg NeuroImage 40 (2008) 415 419 Alumit Ishai Institute of Neuroradiology, University of Zurich, Winterthurerstrasse 190,

More information

Human Brain Mapping. Face-likeness and image variability drive responses in human face-selective ventral regions

Human Brain Mapping. Face-likeness and image variability drive responses in human face-selective ventral regions Face-likeness and image variability drive responses in human face-selective ventral regions Journal: Human Brain Mapping Manuscript ID: HBM--0.R Wiley - Manuscript type: Research Article Date Submitted

More information

A Cortical Network for Face Perception

A Cortical Network for Face Perception A Cortical Network for Face Perception Alumit Ishai Institute of Neuroradiology, University of Zurich, Switzerland Address for correspondence: Alumit Ishai, PhD Professor of Cognitive Neuroscience University

More information

Normal and abnormal face selectivity of the M170 response in developmental prosopagnosics

Normal and abnormal face selectivity of the M170 response in developmental prosopagnosics Neuropsychologia 43 (2005) 2125 2136 Normal and abnormal face selectivity of the M170 response in developmental prosopagnosics Alison M. Harris, Bradley C. Duchaine, Ken Nakayama Vision Science Laboratory,

More information

The fusiform face area is not sufficient for face recognition: Evidence from a patient with dense prosopagnosia and no occipital face area

The fusiform face area is not sufficient for face recognition: Evidence from a patient with dense prosopagnosia and no occipital face area Neuropsychologia 44 (2006) 594 609 The fusiform face area is not sufficient for face recognition: Evidence from a patient with dense prosopagnosia and no occipital face area Jennifer K.E. Steeves a,, Jody

More information

Holistic Processing of Faces: Learning Effects with Mooney Faces

Holistic Processing of Faces: Learning Effects with Mooney Faces Holistic Processing of Faces: Learning Effects with Mooney Faces Marianne Latinus and Margot J. Taylor* Abstract & The specialness of faces is seen in the face inversion effect, which disrupts the configural,

More information

Face Perception. The Thatcher Illusion. The Thatcher Illusion. Can you recognize these upside-down faces? The Face Inversion Effect

Face Perception. The Thatcher Illusion. The Thatcher Illusion. Can you recognize these upside-down faces? The Face Inversion Effect The Thatcher Illusion Face Perception Did you notice anything odd about the upside-down image of Margaret Thatcher that you saw before? Can you recognize these upside-down faces? The Thatcher Illusion

More information

Parvocellular layers (3-6) Magnocellular layers (1 & 2)

Parvocellular layers (3-6) Magnocellular layers (1 & 2) Parvocellular layers (3-6) Magnocellular layers (1 & 2) Dorsal and Ventral visual pathways Figure 4.15 The dorsal and ventral streams in the cortex originate with the magno and parvo ganglion cells and

More information

The recognition of objects and faces

The recognition of objects and faces The recognition of objects and faces John Greenwood Department of Experimental Psychology!! NEUR3001! Contact: john.greenwood@ucl.ac.uk 1 Today The problem of object recognition: many-to-one mapping Available

More information

Stimulus-dependent position sensitivity in human ventral temporal cortex

Stimulus-dependent position sensitivity in human ventral temporal cortex Stimulus-dependent position sensitivity in human ventral temporal cortex Rory Sayres 1, Kevin S. Weiner 1, Brian Wandell 1,2, and Kalanit Grill-Spector 1,2 1 Psychology Department, Stanford University,

More information

Seeing face-like objects: an event-related potential study Owen Churches a,b, Simon Baron-Cohen a and Howard Ring b

Seeing face-like objects: an event-related potential study Owen Churches a,b, Simon Baron-Cohen a and Howard Ring b Cognitive neuroscience and neuropsychology 1 Seeing face-like objects: an event-related potential study Owen Churches a,b, Simon Baron-Cohen a and Howard Ring b The N17 event-related potential component

More information

Inversion improves the recognition of facial expression in thatcherized images

Inversion improves the recognition of facial expression in thatcherized images Perception, 214, volume 43, pages 715 73 doi:1.168/p7755 Inversion improves the recognition of facial expression in thatcherized images Lilia Psalta, Timothy J Andrews Department of Psychology and York

More information

Received 28 September 1999; accepted 15 October 1999

Received 28 September 1999; accepted 15 October 1999 COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE NEUROREPORT The N7 occipito-temporal component is delayed and enhanced to inverted faces but not to inverted objects: an electrophysiological account of face-speci c processes in

More information

Normal perception of Mooney faces in developmental prosopagnosia: Evidence from the N170 component and rapid neural adaptation

Normal perception of Mooney faces in developmental prosopagnosia: Evidence from the N170 component and rapid neural adaptation 1 Journal of Neuropsychology (2014) 2014 The British Psychological Society www.wileyonlinelibrary.com Normal perception of Mooney faces in developmental prosopagnosia: Evidence from the N170 component

More information

Prosopagnosia and structural encoding of faces: Evidence from event-related potentials

Prosopagnosia and structural encoding of faces: Evidence from event-related potentials Cognitive neuroscience 10, 255±259 (1999) EVENT-RELATED brain potentials (ERPs) were recorded in response to unfamiliar faces and to houses from a severely prosopagnosic patient (PHD) and 24 control subjects.

More information

The Anterior Temporal Face Area Contains Invariant Representations of Face Identity That Can Persist Despite the Loss of Right FFA and OFA

The Anterior Temporal Face Area Contains Invariant Representations of Face Identity That Can Persist Despite the Loss of Right FFA and OFA Cerebral Cortex Advance Access published December 19, 2014 Cerebral Cortex, 2014, 1 12 doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu289 Original Article ORIGINAL ARTICLE The Anterior Temporal Face Area Contains Invariant Representations

More information

Residual fmri sensitivity for identity changes in acquired prosopagnosia

Residual fmri sensitivity for identity changes in acquired prosopagnosia ORIGINAL RESEARCH ARTICLE published: 18 October 2013 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00756 Residual fmri sensitivity for identity changes in acquired prosopagnosia Christopher J. Fox 1 *, Giuseppe Iaria 2, Bradley

More information

Structural Encoding of Human and Schematic Faces: Holistic and Part-Based Processes

Structural Encoding of Human and Schematic Faces: Holistic and Part-Based Processes Structural Encoding of Human and Schematic Faces: Holistic and Part-Based Processes Noam Sagiv 1 and Shlomo Bentin Abstract & The range of specificity and the response properties of the extrastriate face

More information

The Hierarchical Brain Network for Face Recognition

The Hierarchical Brain Network for Face Recognition The Hierarchical Brain Network for Face Recognition Zonglei Zhen 1, Huizhen Fang 1, Jia Liu 1,2 * 1 State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China,

More information

Invariant Object Recognition in the Visual System with Novel Views of 3D Objects

Invariant Object Recognition in the Visual System with Novel Views of 3D Objects LETTER Communicated by Marian Stewart-Bartlett Invariant Object Recognition in the Visual System with Novel Views of 3D Objects Simon M. Stringer simon.stringer@psy.ox.ac.uk Edmund T. Rolls Edmund.Rolls@psy.ox.ac.uk,

More information

John Towler*, Joanna Parketny, & Martin Eimer Department of Psychological Sciences Birkbeck College, University of London, UK. *Corresponding Author

John Towler*, Joanna Parketny, & Martin Eimer Department of Psychological Sciences Birkbeck College, University of London, UK. *Corresponding Author Perceptual face processing in developmental prosopagnosia is not sensitive to the canonical location of face parts: Evidence from event-related brain potentials John Towler*, Joanna Parketny, & Martin

More information

Methods. Experimental Stimuli: We selected 24 animals, 24 tools, and 24

Methods. Experimental Stimuli: We selected 24 animals, 24 tools, and 24 Methods Experimental Stimuli: We selected 24 animals, 24 tools, and 24 nonmanipulable object concepts following the criteria described in a previous study. For each item, a black and white grayscale photo

More information

The Physiology of the Senses Lecture 3: Visual Perception of Objects

The Physiology of the Senses Lecture 3: Visual Perception of Objects The Physiology of the Senses Lecture 3: Visual Perception of Objects www.tutis.ca/senses/ Contents Objectives... 2 What is after V1?... 2 Assembling Simple Features into Objects... 4 Illusory Contours...

More information

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION a b STS IOS IOS STS c "#$"% "%' STS posterior IOS dorsal anterior ventral d "( "& )* e f "( "#$"% "%' "& )* Supplementary Figure 1. Retinotopic mapping of the non-lesioned hemisphere. a. Inflated 3D representation

More information

Exploring body holistic processing investigated with composite illusion

Exploring body holistic processing investigated with composite illusion Exploring body holistic processing investigated with composite illusion Dora E. Szatmári (szatmari.dora@pte.hu) University of Pécs, Institute of Psychology Ifjúság Street 6. Pécs, 7624 Hungary Beatrix

More information

Object Perception. 23 August PSY Object & Scene 1

Object Perception. 23 August PSY Object & Scene 1 Object Perception Perceiving an object involves many cognitive processes, including recognition (memory), attention, learning, expertise. The first step is feature extraction, the second is feature grouping

More information

- Faces - A Special Problem of Object Recognition

- Faces - A Special Problem of Object Recognition - Faces - A Special Problem of Object Recognition Lesson II: Perception module 10 Perception.10. 1 Why are faces interesting? A face provides some of the most important cues about someone s identity Facial

More information

A Real-World Size Organization of Object Responses in Occipitotemporal Cortex

A Real-World Size Organization of Object Responses in Occipitotemporal Cortex Article A Real-World Size Organization of Object Responses in Occipitotemporal Cortex Talia Konkle 1, * and Aude Oliva 1,2 1 Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology,

More information

Henriksson, Linda; Mur, Marieke; Kriegeskorte, Nikolaus Faciotopy - A face-feature map with face-like topology in the human occipital face area

Henriksson, Linda; Mur, Marieke; Kriegeskorte, Nikolaus Faciotopy - A face-feature map with face-like topology in the human occipital face area Powered by TCPDF (www.tcpdf.org) This is an electronic reprint of the original article. This reprint may differ from the original in pagination and typographic detail. Henriksson, Linda; Mur, Marieke;

More information

Haptic study of three-dimensional objects activates extrastriate visual areas

Haptic study of three-dimensional objects activates extrastriate visual areas Neuropsychologia 40 (2002) 1706 1714 Haptic study of three-dimensional objects activates extrastriate visual areas Thomas W. James, G. Keith Humphrey, Joseph S. Gati, Philip Servos, Ravi S. Menon, Melvyn

More information

The Effect of Face Inversion on Activity in Human Neural Systems for Face and Object Perception

The Effect of Face Inversion on Activity in Human Neural Systems for Face and Object Perception Neuron, Vol. 22, 189 199, January, 1999, Copyright 1999 by Cell Press The Effect of Face Inversion on Activity in Human Neural Systems for Face and Object Perception James V. Haxby,* Leslie G. Ungerleider,*

More information

The effect of rotation on configural encoding in a face-matching task

The effect of rotation on configural encoding in a face-matching task Perception, 2007, volume 36, pages 446 ^ 460 DOI:10.1068/p5530 The effect of rotation on configural encoding in a face-matching task Andrew J Edmondsô, Michael B Lewis School of Psychology, Cardiff University,

More information

Explicating the Face Perception Network with White Matter Connectivity

Explicating the Face Perception Network with White Matter Connectivity Explicating the Face Perception Network with White Matter Connectivity John A. Pyles 1,2 *, Timothy D. Verstynen 1,2, Walter Schneider 1,3,4, Michael J. Tarr 1,2 1 Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition,

More information

FAQ. Feature detection

FAQ. Feature detection Categorization I FAQ Why are we reading about perception in a class about memory? Surprise: A lot of perception is about memory. Top-down effects = context Where does context come from? Perception and

More information

Vision V Perceiving Movement

Vision V Perceiving Movement Vision V Perceiving Movement Overview of Topics Chapter 8 in Goldstein (chp. 9 in 7th ed.) Movement is tied up with all other aspects of vision (colour, depth, shape perception...) Differentiating self-motion

More information

Cognitive Response Profile of the Human Fusiform Face Area as Determined by MEG

Cognitive Response Profile of the Human Fusiform Face Area as Determined by MEG Cognitive Response Profile of the Human Fusiform Face Area as Determined by MEG Eric Halgren 1,2, Tommi Raij 3, Ksenija Marinkovic 1,2, Veikko Jousmäki 3 and Riitta Hari 3 1 INSERM E9926, Marseilles, France,

More information

Vision V Perceiving Movement

Vision V Perceiving Movement Vision V Perceiving Movement Overview of Topics Chapter 8 in Goldstein (chp. 9 in 7th ed.) Movement is tied up with all other aspects of vision (colour, depth, shape perception...) Differentiating self-motion

More information

Detecting symmetry and faces: Separating the tasks and identifying their interactions

Detecting symmetry and faces: Separating the tasks and identifying their interactions Atten Percept Psychophys () 7:988 DOI 8/s--7- Detecting symmetry and faces: Separating the tasks and identifying their interactions Rebecca M. Jones & Jonathan D. Victor & Mary M. Conte Published online:

More information

The visual and oculomotor systems. Peter H. Schiller, year The visual cortex

The visual and oculomotor systems. Peter H. Schiller, year The visual cortex The visual and oculomotor systems Peter H. Schiller, year 2006 The visual cortex V1 Anatomical Layout Monkey brain central sulcus Central Sulcus V1 Principalis principalis Arcuate Lunate lunate Figure

More information

The Functional Neuroanatomy of Human Face Perception

The Functional Neuroanatomy of Human Face Perception Annu. Rev. Vis. Sci. 2017. 3:167 96 First published as a Review in Advance on July 17, 2017 The Annual Review of Vision Science is online at vision.annualreviews.org https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-vision-102016-061214

More information

Running head: MOVEMENT IN DEVELOPMENTAL PROSOPAGNOSIA. Rachel J Bennetts. Bournemouth University. Natalie Butcher. York St John University

Running head: MOVEMENT IN DEVELOPMENTAL PROSOPAGNOSIA. Rachel J Bennetts. Bournemouth University. Natalie Butcher. York St John University Running head: MOVEMENT IN DEVELOPMENTAL PROSOPAGNOSIA Movement Cues Aid Face Recognition in Developmental Prosopagnosia Rachel J Bennetts Bournemouth University Natalie Butcher York St John University

More information

It Takes Two Skilled Recognition of Objects Engages Lateral Areas in Both Hemispheres

It Takes Two Skilled Recognition of Objects Engages Lateral Areas in Both Hemispheres It Takes Two Skilled Recognition of Objects Engages Lateral Areas in Both Hemispheres Merim Bilalić 1 *, Andrea Kiesel 2, Carsten Pohl 2, Michael Erb 1, Wolfgang Grodd 3 1 Department of Neuroradiology,

More information

Tilburg University. Haptic face recognition and prosopagnosia Kilgour, A.R.; de Gelder, Bea; Bertelson, P. Published in: Neuropsychologia

Tilburg University. Haptic face recognition and prosopagnosia Kilgour, A.R.; de Gelder, Bea; Bertelson, P. Published in: Neuropsychologia Tilburg University Haptic face recognition and prosopagnosia Kilgour, A.R.; de Gelder, Bea; Bertelson, P. Published in: Neuropsychologia Publication date: 2004 Link to publication Citation for published

More information

Faces are «spatial» - Holistic face perception is supported by low spatial frequencies

Faces are «spatial» - Holistic face perception is supported by low spatial frequencies Faces are «spatial» - Holistic face perception is supported by low spatial frequencies Valérie Goffaux & Bruno Rossion Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, in press Main

More information

PERCEIVING MOTION CHAPTER 8

PERCEIVING MOTION CHAPTER 8 Motion 1 Perception (PSY 4204) Christine L. Ruva, Ph.D. PERCEIVING MOTION CHAPTER 8 Overview of Questions Why do some animals freeze in place when they sense danger? How do films create movement from still

More information

FOCAL ELECTRICAL INTRACEREBRAL STIMULATION OF A FACE-SENSITIVE AREA CAUSES TRANSIENT PROSOPAGNOSIA

FOCAL ELECTRICAL INTRACEREBRAL STIMULATION OF A FACE-SENSITIVE AREA CAUSES TRANSIENT PROSOPAGNOSIA Neuroscience 222 (2012) 281 288 FOCAL ELECTRICAL INTRACEREBRAL STIMULATION OF A FACE-SENSITIVE AREA CAUSES TRANSIENT PROSOPAGNOSIA J. JONAS, a,b,f * M. DESCOINS, c,d L. KOESSLER, b S. COLNAT-COULBOIS,

More information

Face Processing Systems: From Neurons to Real-World Social Perception

Face Processing Systems: From Neurons to Real-World Social Perception ANNUAL REVIEWS Further Click here to view this article's online features: Download figures as PPT slides Navigate linked references Download citations Explore related articles Search keywords Annu. Rev.

More information

H uman perception is not a sequence of snapshots of the outer world but a constructive process to cope with

H uman perception is not a sequence of snapshots of the outer world but a constructive process to cope with OPEN SUBJECT AREAS: MOTION STRIATE CORTEX Received 21 May 2014 Accepted 25 July 2014 Published 14 August 2014 Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to M.A. (michel. akselrod@epfl.ch)

More information

Neural tuning size is a key factor underlying holistic face processing by Cheston Tan and Tomaso Poggio

Neural tuning size is a key factor underlying holistic face processing by Cheston Tan and Tomaso Poggio CBMM Memo No. 21 June 14, 2014 Neural tuning size is a key factor underlying holistic face processing by Cheston Tan and Tomaso Poggio Abstract: Faces are a class of visual stimuli with unique significance,

More information

The role of holistic face processing in acquired prosopagnosia: evidence from the composite face effect

The role of holistic face processing in acquired prosopagnosia: evidence from the composite face effect VISUAL COGNITION, 2016 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13506285.2016.1261976 The role of holistic face processing in acquired prosopagnosia: evidence from the composite face effect R. Dawn Finzi a, Tirta Susilo

More information

Chapter 8: Perceiving Motion

Chapter 8: Perceiving Motion Chapter 8: Perceiving Motion Motion perception occurs (a) when a stationary observer perceives moving stimuli, such as this couple crossing the street; and (b) when a moving observer, like this basketball

More information

Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion

Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion Vision Research 42 (2002) 2731 2735 www.elsevier.com/locate/visres Modulating motion-induced blindness with depth ordering and surface completion Erich W. Graf *, Wendy J. Adams, Martin Lages Department

More information

When Holistic Processing is Not Enough: Local Features Save the Day

When Holistic Processing is Not Enough: Local Features Save the Day When Holistic Processing is Not Enough: Local Features Save the Day Lingyun Zhang and Garrison W. Cottrell lingyun,gary@cs.ucsd.edu UCSD Computer Science and Engineering 9500 Gilman Dr., La Jolla, CA 92093-0114

More information

Solving the upside-down puzzle: Why do upright and inverted face aftereffects look alike?

Solving the upside-down puzzle: Why do upright and inverted face aftereffects look alike? Journal of Vision (2010) 10(13):1, 1 16 http://www.journalofvision.org/content/10/13/1 1 Solving the upside-down puzzle: Why do upright and inverted face aftereffects look alike? Tirta Susilo Elinor McKone

More information

This is a repository copy of Thatcher s Britain: : a new take on an old illusion.

This is a repository copy of Thatcher s Britain: : a new take on an old illusion. This is a repository copy of Thatcher s Britain: : a new take on an old illusion. White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: http://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/103303/ Version: Submitted Version Article:

More information

1/21/2019. to see : to know what is where by looking. -Aristotle. The Anatomy of Visual Pathways: Anatomy and Function are Linked

1/21/2019. to see : to know what is where by looking. -Aristotle. The Anatomy of Visual Pathways: Anatomy and Function are Linked The Laboratory for Visual Neuroplasticity Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary Harvard Medical School to see : to know what is where by looking -Aristotle The Anatomy of Visual Pathways: Anatomy and Function

More information

Faces and objects in macaque cerebral cortex

Faces and objects in macaque cerebral cortex Faces and objects in macaque cerebral cortex Doris Y Tsao 1,2,Winrich A Freiwald 3 5,Tamara A Knutsen 1,Joseph B Mandeville 1 & Roger B H Tootell 1,6 How are different object categories organized by the

More information

Spatial scale contribution to early visual differences between face and object processing

Spatial scale contribution to early visual differences between face and object processing Cognitive Brain Research 16 (2003) 416 424 www.elsevier.com/ locate/ cogbrainres Research report Spatial scale contribution to early visual differences between face and object processing a Valerie Goffaux

More information

A network of occipito-temporal face-sensitive areas besides the right middle fusiform gyrus is necessary for normal face processing

A network of occipito-temporal face-sensitive areas besides the right middle fusiform gyrus is necessary for normal face processing DOI: 10.1093/brain/awg241 Advanced Access publication July 22, 2003 Brain (2003), 126, 2381±2395 A network of occipito-temporal face-sensitive areas besides the right middle fusiform gyrus is necessary

More information

A network of occipito-temporal face-sensitive areas besides the right middle fusiform gyrus is necessary for normal face processing

A network of occipito-temporal face-sensitive areas besides the right middle fusiform gyrus is necessary for normal face processing DOI: 10.1093/brain/awg241 Advanced Access publication July 22, 2003 Brain (2003), 126, 2381±2395 A network of occipito-temporal face-sensitive areas besides the right middle fusiform gyrus is necessary

More information

Con gural face processes in acquired and developmental prosopagnosia: evidence for two separate face systems?

Con gural face processes in acquired and developmental prosopagnosia: evidence for two separate face systems? COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE NEUROREPORT Con gural face processes in acquired and developmental prosopagnosia: evidence for two separate face systems? Beatrice de Gelder 1,2,CA and Romke Rouw 1 1 Cognitive Neuroscience

More information

The effect of face orientation on holistic processing

The effect of face orientation on holistic processing Perception, 2008, volume 37, pages 1175 ^ 1186 doi:10.1068/p6048 The effect of face orientation on holistic processing Catherine J Mondloch Department of Psychology, Brock University, 500 Glenridge Avenue,

More information

Simulating Biological Motion Perception Using a Recurrent Neural Network

Simulating Biological Motion Perception Using a Recurrent Neural Network Simulating Biological Motion Perception Using a Recurrent Neural Network Roxanne L. Canosa Department of Computer Science Rochester Institute of Technology Rochester, NY 14623 rlc@cs.rit.edu Abstract People

More information

Inverting an Image Does Not Improve Drawing Accuracy

Inverting an Image Does Not Improve Drawing Accuracy Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts 2010 American Psychological Association 2010, Vol. 4, No. 3, 168 172 1931-3896/10/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0017054 Inverting an Image Does Not Improve Drawing

More information

Basic MVPA strategies

Basic MVPA strategies Basic MVPA strategies Michael Hanke & Yaroslav Halchenko University of Magdeburg, Germany Dartmouth College, USA Giessen 2014 H 2 (Dartmouth; Magdeburg) MVPA Intro Giessen 2014 1 / 8 How it all began:

More information

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution

More information

Electrophysiological Correlates of Visual Adaptation to Faces and Body Parts in Humans

Electrophysiological Correlates of Visual Adaptation to Faces and Body Parts in Humans Cerebral Cortex Advance Access published August 24, 2005 Cerebral Cortex doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj020 Electrophysiological Correlates of Visual Adaptation to Faces and Body Parts in Humans Gyula Kova cs 1,Ma

More information

UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society

UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society Title When Holistic Processing is Not Enough: Local Features Save the Day Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/6ds7h63h

More information

CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines. Due: Wednesday, February 8

CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines. Due: Wednesday, February 8 CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines Lab 2: Human Face Recognition and Holistic Processing Due: Wednesday, February 8 This lab explores our ability to recognize familiar and unfamiliar faces, and the

More information

A specialized face-processing network consistent with the representational geometry of monkey face patches

A specialized face-processing network consistent with the representational geometry of monkey face patches A specialized face-processing network consistent with the representational geometry of monkey face patches Amirhossein Farzmahdi, Karim Rajaei, Masoud Ghodrati, Reza Ebrahimpour, Seyed-Mahdi Khaligh-Razavi

More information

The reference frame of figure ground assignment

The reference frame of figure ground assignment Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2004, 11 (5), 909-915 The reference frame of figure ground assignment SHAUN P. VECERA University of Iowa, Iowa City, Iowa Figure ground assignment involves determining which

More information

Author's personal copy

Author's personal copy Provided for non-commercial research and educational use only. Not for reproduction, distribution or commercial use. This article was originally published in the International Encyclopedia of the Social

More information

The Neural Basis of Intuitive Best Next-Move Generation in Board Game Experts

The Neural Basis of Intuitive Best Next-Move Generation in Board Game Experts www.sciencemag.org/cgi/content/full/331/6015/341/dc1 Supporting Online Material for The Neural Basis of Intuitive Best Next-Move Generation in Board Game Experts Xiaohong Wan, Hironori Nakatani, Kenichi

More information

Retinotopy versus Face Selectivity in Macaque Visual Cortex

Retinotopy versus Face Selectivity in Macaque Visual Cortex Retinotopy versus Face Selectivity in Macaque Visual Cortex Reza Rajimehr 1,2, Natalia Y. Bilenko 1, Wim Vanduffel 1,3, and Roger B. H. Tootell 1 Abstract Retinotopic organization is a ubiquitous property

More information

Electrophysiological Studies of Human Face Perception. I: Potentials Generated in Occipitotemporal Cortex by Face and Non-face Stimuli

Electrophysiological Studies of Human Face Perception. I: Potentials Generated in Occipitotemporal Cortex by Face and Non-face Stimuli Electrophysiological Studies of Human Face Perception. I: Potentials Generated in Occipitotemporal Cortex by Face and Non-face Stimuli This and the following two papers describe event-related potentials

More information

Lecture 4 Foundations and Cognitive Processes in Visual Perception From the Retina to the Visual Cortex

Lecture 4 Foundations and Cognitive Processes in Visual Perception From the Retina to the Visual Cortex Lecture 4 Foundations and Cognitive Processes in Visual Perception From the Retina to the Visual Cortex 1.Vision Science 2.Visual Performance 3.The Human Visual System 4.The Retina 5.The Visual Field and

More information

P rcep e t p i t on n a s a s u n u c n ons n c s ious u s i nf n e f renc n e L ctur u e 4 : Recogni n t i io i n

P rcep e t p i t on n a s a s u n u c n ons n c s ious u s i nf n e f renc n e L ctur u e 4 : Recogni n t i io i n Lecture 4: Recognition and Identification Dr. Tony Lambert Reading: UoA text, Chapter 5, Sensation and Perception (especially pp. 141-151) 151) Perception as unconscious inference Hermann von Helmholtz

More information

iris pupil cornea ciliary muscles accommodation Retina Fovea blind spot

iris pupil cornea ciliary muscles accommodation Retina Fovea blind spot Chapter 6 Vision Exam 1 Anatomy of vision Primary visual cortex (striate cortex, V1) Prestriate cortex, Extrastriate cortex (Visual association coretx ) Second level association areas in the temporal and

More information

Author's Accepted Manuscript

Author's Accepted Manuscript Author's Accepted Manuscript Fixation location on upright and inverted faces modulates the N170 Peter de Lissa, Genevieve McArthur, Stefan Hawelka, Romina Palermo, Yatin Mahajan, Florian Hutzler www.elsevier.com/locate/neuropsychologia

More information

Chapter 73. Two-Stroke Apparent Motion. George Mather

Chapter 73. Two-Stroke Apparent Motion. George Mather Chapter 73 Two-Stroke Apparent Motion George Mather The Effect One hundred years ago, the Gestalt psychologist Max Wertheimer published the first detailed study of the apparent visual movement seen when

More information

Neural representations of faces and limbs neighbor in human high-level visual cortex: evidence for a new organization principle

Neural representations of faces and limbs neighbor in human high-level visual cortex: evidence for a new organization principle Psychological Research (2013) 77:74 97 DOI 10.1007/s00426-011-0392-x REVIEW Neural representations of faces and limbs neighbor in human high-level visual cortex: evidence for a new organization principle

More information

Neural Coding of Multiple Stimulus Features in Auditory Cortex

Neural Coding of Multiple Stimulus Features in Auditory Cortex Neural Coding of Multiple Stimulus Features in Auditory Cortex Jonathan Z. Simon Neuroscience and Cognitive Sciences Biology / Electrical & Computer Engineering University of Maryland, College Park Computational

More information

No symmetry advantage when object matching involves accidental viewpoints

No symmetry advantage when object matching involves accidental viewpoints Psychological Research (2006) 70: 52 58 DOI 10.1007/s00426-004-0191-8 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Arno Koning Æ Rob van Lier No symmetry advantage when object matching involves accidental viewpoints Received: 11

More information

Spectro-Temporal Methods in Primary Auditory Cortex David Klein Didier Depireux Jonathan Simon Shihab Shamma

Spectro-Temporal Methods in Primary Auditory Cortex David Klein Didier Depireux Jonathan Simon Shihab Shamma Spectro-Temporal Methods in Primary Auditory Cortex David Klein Didier Depireux Jonathan Simon Shihab Shamma & Department of Electrical Engineering Supported in part by a MURI grant from the Office of

More information

Mapping face categorization in the human ventral occipitotemporal cortex with direct neural intracranial recordings

Mapping face categorization in the human ventral occipitotemporal cortex with direct neural intracranial recordings Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. ISSN 0077-8923 ANNALS OF THE NEW YORK ACADEMY OF SCIENCES Issue: The Year in Cognitive Neuroscience REVIEW Mapping face categorization in the human ventral occipitotemporal cortex

More information